• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Effects of Chronic Morphine Exposure and Withdrawal on Social Novelty- and Food-seeking Behaviors

    2016-12-09 05:13:56GuzalnurMammatWANGJianliZHANBoLIUChaobaoCHENMoxuan
    四川動物 2016年1期
    關鍵詞:買買提努爾新穎性

    Guzalnur Mammat, WANG Jianli, ZHAN Bo, LIU Chaobao, CHEN Moxuan

    (College of Biological Sciences and Engineering, Beifang University of Nationalities, Yinchuan 750021, China)

    ?

    Effects of Chronic Morphine Exposure and Withdrawal on Social Novelty- and Food-seeking Behaviors

    Guzalnur Mammat, WANG Jianli*, ZHAN Bo, LIU Chaobao, CHEN Moxuan

    (College of Biological Sciences and Engineering, Beifang University of Nationalities, Yinchuan 750021, China)

    Opioid peptides play a crucial role in the control of social and non-social natural rewards. The motivation to seeking natural rewards is associated with the emotional state of an animal. The exposure and withdrawal from morphine provokes different emotional states. The present study assessed whether morphine exposure and withdrawal affects the seeking behaviors of two types of natural rewards: social novelty and food. Male mice were administered morphine (10 mg·kg-1twice daily, or saline in control) for 14 days. 20 minutes after the last injection, locomotor activity and the approach to familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics or food were examined. Subsequently, the social novelty-seeking and food-seeking behaviors of mice that had undergone a seven-day morphine or saline withdrawal were examined. During the phases of saline exposure and withdrawal, we observed that mice would prefer food and spend more time on approaching the unfamiliar conspecifics rather than on the familiar conspecifics. Morphine exposure increased the locomotion and the morphine-exposed mice spent a similar amount of time on approaching the unfamiliar and familiar conspecifics. Morphine exposure did not significantly affect the food-seeking behavior. Compare to saline controls, morphine withdrawal lowered the time spent on approaching to food, while there were no specific effects in social novelty-seeking. These findings indicated that morphine experience significantly impaired the appetitive motivations to social stimuli and food; the incentives for the two stimuli were affected differently by chronic exposure and withdrawal from morphine, and these behavioral changes may be associated with morphine experience-induced different neuroadaptations.

    morphine; abstinence; natural reward; social stimuli; food

    Food, water and sexual stimuli are called primary rewards. These stimuli are considered innate because they are essential for survival and reproduction (Walteretal., 2005). Food provides a rewarding effect, which has been reflected in the conditioned place preference (CPP) test (Qietal., 2011; Duarteetal., 2014; Monclaroetal., 2014). Rats tend to approach and spend more time in an environment paired with another rat (Calcagnettietal., 1992; Crowder & Hutto, 1992; Thieletal., 2008), and animals spend more time with an unfamiliar individual than a more familiar conspecific (Nadleretal., 2004), showing that social stimuli and social novelty can serve as reinforcing cues. In addition to natural rewards, primary reinforcers also include a variety of drugs, opioids and stimulants. The brain circuitry that mediates behavior essential for survival becomes compromised in individuals that have been exposed to drugs, and affects the drive to acquire natural reinforcers (Kelley, 2004; Aragonaetal., 2007). For example, chronic exposure to a constant dose of cocaine is sufficient to reduce natural reinforcement (Barnea-Ygaeletal., 2014). Opiate and psychostimulant abuse respectively fulfills the need for social comfort and for natural rewards that sustain life, by directly impinging on the underlying emotional substrates (Pankseppetal., 1980). Many literatures indicate that endogenous opioid peptides play a crucial role in controlling the motivation for social interactions and ingestion (Cooper & Kirkham, 1993; Moles & Cooper, 1995; Pankseppetal., 1997; Nocjar & Panksepp, 2007; Baietal., 2014). The blockade of opioid activity is sufficient to impair the expression of a socially acquired food preference (Molesetal., 1999). Morphine-treated mice consume significantly less food, and the incentives for social and non-social natural rewards increase following withdrawal from intermittent opiate treatment (Nocjar & Panksepp, 2007).

    Previous work suggests that abstinence from drugs can lead to altered emotional systems. Protracted abstinence is characterized by lowered mood or depression (Goeldneretal., 2011; Lutzetal., 2013). Withdrawal from morphine is associated with an increase in anxiogenic-like behaviors (Castilhoetal., 2008; Miladi-Gorjietal., 2012), while morphine exposure is known to induce anxiolytic effects (Shinetal., 2003; Motevasselietal., 2010; Rezayofetal., 2013). The pharmacologic effect of the drug exposure differs from abstinence. The decreased motivation for a natural reinforcement is thought to be associated with the drug withdrawal-induced depressive state (Zhangetal., 2007). Although it has been widely researched that opiates regulate natural rewards-associated behaviors, including food consumption (Moles & Cooper, 1995; Baietal., 2014), food preference (Nocjar & Panksepp, 2007; Kerstetteetal., 2012), social transmission of a food preference (Molesetal., 1999), and social interest (Nocjar & Panksepp, 2007; Baietal., 2014), it is not known whether morphine exposure and withdrawal differently affect the motivation to approaching the natural reinforcers. To better understand the causal relationship between morphine experience and natural reward-seeking behavior, we examined the effects of chronic morphine exposure and withdrawal on the social novelty- and food-seeking behaviors in mice.

    1 Materials and methods

    1.1 Subjects

    Six-week old male ICR mice were [production license No.: SCXK(寧) 2011-0001] obtained from Ningxia Medical University Laboratory Animal Center (Yinchuan, China). The mice were housed in groups of two in standard transparent Makrolon cages (l×w×h, 32 cm×21.5 cm×17 cm). The colony room was illuminated on a 12∶12 light-dark cycle (lights on 2000 h), and the temperature was maintained at 23 ℃±2 ℃. Food and water were availableadlibitum. Mice were allowed to adapt to housing conditions for one week, and were handled daily by the same experimenter for three days prior to testing. All experimental procedures were performed strictly in accordance with the guidelines published in the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

    1.2 General experimental procedures

    The experiences of the mice were divided into two phases: administration and withdrawal phases. In the administration phase, mice received daily subcutaneous injections of 10.0 mg·kg-1morphine (ME,n=8) or 0.9% saline (SE,n=8) for 14 days. Following 14-day morphine exposure, mice underwent three experimental measurements in sequence: open field test,social novelty-seeking and food-seeking test. In the withdrawal phase, mice that had been treated for 14 days were given seven-day morphine withdrawal (n=8) or saline withdrawal (n=8). During the withdrawal phase, mice were left undisturbed in their home-cages, except for scheduled cage cleaning. Following the seven-day withdrawal, the second round of social novelty-seeking and food-seeking tests were conducted, but not the open field test. Fig. 1 represents a timeline depicting the experimental session.

    Fig. 1 Timeline of the experimental session

    The boxed text represents the components of the experimental session. Mice were given daily injections of morphine (10 mg·kg-1) or saline for 14 days, then open-field, social novelty-seeking and food-seeking behaviors were assessed. Mice then underwent a seven-day morphine or saline-free period, followed by a second test for social novelty- and food-seeking behavior.

    1.3 Drug administration

    Morphine-hydrochloride (Northwest Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd. Sinopharm, Xi’an, China) was used in this experiment, and was diluted in saline. During the administration phase, mice received a daily binge injection of morphine or saline for 14 days. The daily binge pattern consisted of two injections of an identical dose of morphine (subcutaneously, 10 mg·kg-1) in 12 h at 08∶00 and 14∶00. This morphine dose was chosen based on a previous study that demonstrated that chronic administration of 10 mg·kg-1of morphine increases neuronal activity in the areas of the brain that regulate appetitive behavior for both drugs and natural rewards when tested after six-day withdrawal (Krausetal., 1997).

    1.4 Open field test

    20 minutes after the last injection, motor activity and anxiety-like behaviors were measured for 5 min in an open field chamber. The chamber was a brightly and evenly illuminated square arena (l×w×h, 50 cm×50 cm×25 cm) made of white glacial polyvinyl chloride and illuminated with four 60 W lamps mounted 1.5 m above the arena. The area was divided into 16 quadrants (4 central and 12 peripheral) (Fiore & Ratti, 2007). A single mouse was placed in the center of the open field and was left to explore for 5 min. To assess anxiety-like behavior, the time spent in the center of the open field was measured during this period. The number of crossings between quadrants was used to assess locomotion. Additionally, rearing (raising on the hind legs and sniffing into the air or the wall of the box) and self-grooming behavior (licking own fur, sometimes using forepaws, passing them over the nose with a series of brief, horizontal movements) were recorded. All focal mice were videotaped for 5 min using a Sony camera. The frequency and total duration of these behaviors were later scored by a researcher blind to experimental treatment using Jwatcher 1.0. After each test was completed, the open-field was thoroughly cleaned with 70% ethanol solution.

    1.5 Social novelty-seeking test

    Social novelty-seeking test was performed in Makrolon chambers (l×w×h, 46 cm×31.5 cm×20 cm). A cylindrical wire cage containing a familiar mouse that was raised in a cage with the subject mouse was set in one of the chamber's corners. Another cage containing an unfamiliar mouse that had not previously encountered the subject mouse was set in another side corner in parallel. Placement of stimulus-cages within the chamber's corner was counterbalanced between each test. The wire holding cage was stainless steel, 17 cm high, and composed of a solid 11 cm diameter bottom with stainless steel bars spaced at 1 cm intervals. The experimental mice were individually placed in the center of the chamber arena and videotaped for 15 min. Measurement of total approaching or sniffing time at each stimulus-cage (measured by the animal having its nose within 1 cm of cage) was tabulated for each mouse within 15 min. The chamber arena was wiped clean with 70% ethanol between mice to eliminate olfactory cues. Videotapes were viewed by an observer that was blind to the treatment of each mouse.

    1.6 Food-seeking test

    The food-seeking test was performed in the same conditions as described above except a cylindrical wire cage containing mouse chow was set in the chamber’s corner. Another empty cylindrical wire cage was set in another side corner in parallel. The experimental mice were individually placed in the center of the chamber arena and videotaped for 15 min. The experiments were conducted after mice were food deprived for 2 h prior to the food-seeking test.

    1.7 Statistical analysis

    Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 13.0. All data were checked for normality using a one-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test and were found to be normally distributed. Independent samplet-tests were used to examine differences in the open-field behaviors. Social novelty- and food-seeking behaviors were compared using One-Way ANOVA. All data were presented as mean±standard error (SE) and the alpha was set at 0.05.

    2 Results

    2.1 Behavior in open field

    Morphine-treated mice showed a greater number of total transitions (t14=6.010,P<0.001) and spent more time in the central area than saline-treated mice (t14=4.986,P<0.001) (Fig.2: A, B). Compared to the saline controls, morphine-treated mice had less self-grooming behavior (t14=-3.849,P=0.002). No significant differences were found in rearing behavior between the two groups (t14=0.672,P=0.512) (Fig.2: C).

    Fig. 2 The behaviors of ICR mice in open-field test following 14-day saline or morphine exposure

    (A) the mean number of total transitions, (B) total time spent in the central area, and (C) duration of rearing and self-grooming behavior in mice injected with 0.9% saline (SE) or mice injected with 10 mg·kg-1morphine (ME);**P≤0.01; Error bars depict standard error.

    2.2 Social novelty-seeking behavior

    Saline-administrated mice spent significantly more time approaching the cage containing unfamiliar conspecifics than approaching the cage containing familiar conspecifics (Mean difference=131.37,P=0.032). However, morphine-administered mice spent similar amount of time approaching the cage containing unfamiliar conspecifics and the cage containing familiar conspecifics (Mean difference=114.35,P=0.059) (Fig.3). Mice undergoing withdrawal from morphine (Mean difference=171.72,P=0.001) or saline (Mean difference=92.67,P=0.044) spent more time approaching the cage containing unfamiliar conspecifics than approaching the cage containing familiar conspecifics. In comparison to the saline controls, there were no significant differences in the approach to the cage containing unfamiliar conspecifics in morphine administration phase (Mean difference=71.642,P=0.229) and withdrawal phase (Mean difference=-11.26,P=0.800) (Fig.3).

    2.3 Food-seeking behavior

    Both saline-administrated (t7=4.556,P=0.003) and morphine-administrated (t7=5.426,P=0.01) mice spent more time approaching the cage containing food rather than empty cage. Compared to saline treated mice, morphine treated mice did not show differences in the time approaching the cage containing food (t14=-0.330,P=0.746) (Fig. 4). Control mice undergoing saline withdrawal spent more time approaching the cage containing food rather than empty cage (t7=3.244,P=0.014). However, morphine- withdrawal mice spent less time approaching the cage containing food (t7=-4.081,P=0.005). Compared to the mice experienced saline withdrawal, the mice experienced morphine withdrawal reduced the time approaching the cage containing food (t14=6.245,P< 0.001) (Fig. 4).

    Fig. 3 Time spent approaching conspecifics in saline- and morphine-treated ICR mice following 14-day administration and seven-day withdrawal phases

    Graphs depict the mean time spent by saline-treated mice to unfamiliar conspecifics (SUC) and familiar conspecifics (SFC), and morphine-treated mice to unfamiliar conspecifics (MUC) and familiar conspecifics (MFC);*P≤ 0.05,**P≤ 0.01; Error bars depict standard error.

    Fig. 4 Time spent approaching food in saline-and morphine-treated ICR mice following 14-day administration and seven-day withdrawal phases

    Graphs depict mean time the saline-treated mice approached to the cage with food (SF) and without food (SN), and morphine-treated mice approached the cage with food (MF) and without food (MN);*P≤0.05,**P≤0.01; Error bars depict standard error.

    3 Discussion

    In this study, we assessed the effects of chronic morphine exposure and morphine-withdrawal on social novelty- and food-seeking behaviors. We observed an increase in locomotion in mice that had been administered morphine for 14 days, which suggested that the morphine exposure elicited pharmacologic effects. The morphine exposure decreased the mice's tendency to seek unfamiliar conspecifics, while mice preferred unfamiliar conspecifics after seven-day of morphine withdrawal. 14-day morphine administration did not affect the preference to food, but mice undergoing morphine withdrawal showed reduced appetitive behavior for food, indicating that there were different motive behaviors during morphine exposure and withdrawal phases.

    3.1 Effects of chronic morphine exposure and withdrawal on social novelty-seeking behavior

    Several studies indicated that mice exhibited a heightened sensitivity to the effects of morphine (Niuetal., 2013), and repeated morphine exposure decreases self-grooming behavior in female ICR mice (Zhanetal., 2015). We observed that chronic morphine administration enhanced locomotion and attenuated the levels of self-grooming behavior. A previous study showed that repeated morphine exposure did not affect locomotor activity in male ICR mice (Zhanetal., 2015), differing from our present results. This discrepancy may be due to the fact that the mice were exposed to the morphine for four days in Zhanetal. (2015), instead of 14 days, as was done in the present study. The time spent in the central zone, changes in rearing and self-grooming were generally considered to be indices of emotional behavioral processes (To & Bagdy, 1999; Careyetal., 2005). The increased time spent in the central area by mice treated with morphine implies a morphine-induced anxiolytic effect, suggesting that these mice experienced morphine induced-sensitization.

    Social reward-CPP can be obtained even when rats were separated by wire or mesh partitions with limited physical contact (Kummeretal., 2011; Peartreeetal., 2012). In our study, although the stimulus mice were in wire holding cages, saline-treated mice displayed robust approach toward the unfamiliar mice. Mice that were treated with morphine for 14 days did not show significant preference for unfamiliar mice. These results were consistent with a previous report showing diminished social motivation in opiate dependent rats during chronic morphine exposure (Mumford & Kumar, 1979). Seven-day morphine withdrawal did not alter the preference for unfamiliar mice, contrary to some results that prior morphine exposure enhances future social interest, with the effect consistently shown after three-day or two-week opiate withdrawal (Barr & Phillips, 1999; Nocjar & Panksepp, 2002, 2007). Opiate induced change in social interest in young rodents may depend on the animal’s social state while under the drug (Van den Bergetal., 1999a, 1999b; Brosetaetal., 2005; Nocjar & Panksepp, 2007). We noted that animals used in some previous work received chronic morphine while housed in isolation (Nocjar & Panksepp, 2007). Thus, one possibility for the discrepancies may be due to the animal’s social state in this study compared to previous studies.

    3.2 Effects of chronic morphine exposure and withdrawal on food-seeking behavior

    Food preference remained high even after 14 days of morphine exposure. However, when mice were undergoing a seven-day morphine withdrawal showed lower food-seeking behavior. Although chronic exposures to drugs induce a complex series of changes in appetitive behavior, there is no consensus on how it affects natural rewards. For example, Vanhilleetal. (2015) proposed that drug addiction was associated with a relative devaluation of natural or socially-valued reinforcers that were unable to divert addicts from seeking and consuming the drug. Most rats preferred natural rewards, such as saccharin, over cocaine before protracted drug exposure. Galajetal. (2013) found repeated heroin exposure decreased the attractiveness of food and reduced motivation to work for natural rewards. Morphine, d-amphetamine or methamphetamine withdrawal results in decreased motivation to obtain the natural reinforcement (Zhangetal., 2007), yet withdrawal from chronic opiates or amphetamine treatments increased food-seeking (Nocjar & Panksepp, 2002, 2007). These seemingly conflicting results on natural reward processing appear to relate with the nature of the reward-directed behavior (Galajetal., 2013). Additionally, rats that were treated with morphine for five-days followed by a seven-day morphine-withdrawal period had significantly reduced consumption of 2.5% sucrose solution (Baietal., 2014); however, the rats appeared to show increased interest in high-fat food after two-week morphine withdrawal, but not after a three-day short-term withdrawal (Nocjar & Panksepp, 2007), indicating that food-consuming behavior and food-seeking behavior may be controlled by different neurochemical circuits in the brain. These discrepancies were related with the length of morphine pretreatment and withdrawal as well. Additionally, male rhesus monkeys treated with high cocaine dose showed preference for cocaine over food, therefore the reinforcer values included in the experiment can influence the seeking-behavior (Banks & Negus, 2010). In investigating the effect of drugs on animal behavior, we also cannot exclude differences in species (mice vs. rats) because of the importance of genetic variability in opioid modulation of natural reinforcement (Dymetal., 2007).

    In the present study, we found that morphine exposure and withdrawal had different effects on social novelty- and food-seeking behavior. These results supported that independent motivational systems mediated the rewarding effects of opioids in the nondependent state, and in the physically dependent/withdrawal state (Becharaetal., 1998). Repeated drug exposure induced short- and long-term neuroadaptations in brain reward circuitries that were normally involved in the regulation of motivation (Barnea-Ygaeletal., 2014). Morphine exposure induced an anxiolytic effect as shown in our results, while withdrawal from morphine may lead to the appearance of anxiety-like behavior (Castilhoetal., 2008; Miladi-Gorjietal., 2012), though it was not directly measured in the current study. These neuroadaptations were associated with emotional changes that could affect the incentives for social novelty and food.

    In conclusion, morphine exposure and withdrawal differently altered the pursuit of social novelty and food, indicating that morphine can significantly impair the appetitive motivations to two types of natural rewards: social-novelty and food. Morphine-induced different neuroadaptations may regulate these motivational changes. Additional research will be required to understand where and how morphine acts within the brain to alter the incentives for social novelty and food.

    Aragona BJ, Detwiler JM, Wang ZX. 2007. Amphetamine reward in the monogamous prairie vole[J]. Neuroscience Letter, 418(2): 190-194.

    Bai Y, Li Y, Lv Y,etal. 2014. Complex motivated behaviors for natural rewards following a binge-like regimen of morphine administration: mixed phenotypes of anhedonia and craving after short-term withdrawal[J]. Fronters in Behavavioral Neuroscience, 8: 23.

    Banks ML, Negus SS. 2010. Effects of extended cocaine access and cocaine withdrawal on choice between cocaine and food in rhesus monkeys[J]. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(2): 493-504.

    Barnea-Ygael N, Gal R, Zangen A. 2014. Chronic cocaine administration induces long-term impairment in the drive to obtain natural reinforcers in high- but not low-demanding tasks[J]. Addiction Biology, doi: 10.1111/adb.12196.

    Barr AM, Phillips AG. 1999. Withdrawal following repeated exposure to damphetamine decreases responding for a sucrose solution as measured by a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement[J]. Psychopharmacology, 141(1): 99-106.

    Bechara A, Nader K, van der Kooy D. 1998. A two-separate-motivational-systems hypothesis of opioid addiction[J]. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 59(1): 1-17.

    Broseta I, Rodriguez-Arias M, Aguilar MA,etal. 2005. Isolation decreases physical and motivational aspects of morphine withdrawal[J]. Behavioural Pharmacology, 16(3): 131-138.

    Calcagnetti DJ, Schechter MD. 1992. Place conditioning reveals the rewarding aspect of social-interaction in juvenile rats[J]. Physiology & Behavior, 51(4): 667-672.

    Carey RJ, Depalma G, Damianopoulos E. 2005. Evidence for Pavlovian conditioning of cocaine-induced responses linked to emotional behavioral effects[J]. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior, 80(1): 123-134.

    Castilho VM, Borelli KG, Brand?o ML,etal. 2008. Anxiety-like symptoms induced by morphine withdrawal may be due to the sensitization of the dorsal periaqueductal grey[J]. Physiology & Behavior, 94(4): 552-562.

    Cooper SJ, Kirkham TC. 1993. Opioid mechanisms in the control of food consumption and taste preferences[M]// Her A. Handbook of experimental pharmacology. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 104(II): 239-262.

    Crowder WF, Hutto CW. 1992. Operant place conditioning measures examined using 2 non-drug reinforcers[J]. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 41(4): 817-824.

    Duarte RB, Patrono E, Borges AC,etal. 2014. Consumption of a highly palatable food induces a lasting place-conditioning memory in marmoset monkeys[J]. Behavioural Processes, 107: 163-166.

    Dym CT, Pinhas A, Ginzberg M,etal. 2007. Genetic variance contributes to naltrexone-induced inhibition of sucrose intake in inbred and outbred mouse strains[J]. Brain Research, 1135(1): 136-145.

    Fiore L, Ratti G. 2007. Remote laboratory and animal behaviour: an interactive open field system[J]. Computers & Education, 49(4): 1299-1307.

    Galaj E, Cruz I, Schachar J,etal. 2013. Differential effects on natural reward processing in rats after repeated heroin[J]. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 229(1): 125-132.

    Goeldner C, Lutz PE, Darcq E,etal. 2011. Impaired emotional-like behavior and serotonergic function during protracted abstinence from chronic morphine[J]. Biological Psychiatry, 69(3): 236-244.

    Kelley AE. 2004. Memory and addiction: shared neural circuitry and molecular mechanisms[J]. Neuron, 44(1): 161-179.

    Kraus MA, Piper JM, Kornetsky C. 1997. Persistent increases in basal cerebral metabolic activity induced by morphine sensitization[J]. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 57(1-2): 89-100.

    Kummer K, Klement S, Eggart V,etal. 2011. Conditioned place preference for social interaction in rats: contribution of sensory components[J]. Fronters in Behavavioral Neuroscience, 5: 80.

    Lutz PE, Reiss D, Ouagazzal AM,etal. 2013. A history of chronic morphine exposure during adolescence increases despair-like behaviour and strain-dependently promotes sociability in abstinent adult mice[J]. Behavioural Brain Research, 243: 44-52.

    Miladi-Gorji H, Rashidy-Pour A, Fathollahi Y. 2012. Anxiety profile in morphine-dependent and withdrawn rats: effect of voluntary exercise[J]. Physiology & Behavior, 105(2): 195-202.

    Moles A, Cooper SJ. 1995. Opioid modulation of sucrose intake in CD-1 mice: effects of gender and housing conditions[J]. Physiology & Behavior, 58(4): 791-796.

    Moles A, Valsecchi P, Cooper SJ. 1999. Opioid modulation of socially transmitted and spontaneous food preferences in female mice[J]. Behavioural Processes, 44(3): 277-285.

    Monclaro AV, Sampaio AC, Ribeiro NB,etal. 2014. Time-of-day effect on a food-induced conditioned place preference task in monkeys[J]. Behavioural Brain Research, 259: 336-341.

    Motevasseli T, Rezayof A, Zarrindast MR,etal. 2010. Role of ventral hippocampal NMDA receptors in anxiolytic-like effect of morphine[J]. Physiology & Behavior, 101(5): 608-613.

    Mumford L, Kumar R. 1979. Sexual behaviour of morphine-dependent and abstinent male rats[J]. Psychopharmacology, 65(2): 179-185.

    Nadler JJ, Moy SS, Dold G,etal. 2004. Automated apparatus for rapid quantitation of social approach behaviors in mice[J]. Genes Brain and Behavior, 3(5): 303-314.

    Niu H, Zheng Y, Huma T,etal. 2013. Lesion of olfactory epithelium attenuates expression of morphine-induced behavioral sensitization and reinstatement of drug-primed conditioned place preference in mice[J]. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior, 103(3): 526-534.

    Nocjar C, Panksepp J. 2002. Chronic intermittent amphetamine pretreatment enhances future appetitive behavior for drug- and natural-reward: interaction with environmental variables[J]. Behavioural Brain Research, 128(2): 189-203.

    Nocjar C, Panksepp J. 2007. Prior morphine experience induces long-term increases in social interest and in appetitive behavior for natural reward[J]. Behavioural Brain Research, 181(2): 191-199.

    Panksepp J, Herman BH, Villberg T,etal. 1980. Endogenous opioids and social behavior[J]. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 4(4): 473-487.

    Panksepp J, Nelson E, Bekkedal M. 1997. Brain systems for the mediation of social separation-distress and social-reward. Evolutionary antecedents and neuropeptide intermediaries[J]. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 807: 78-100.

    Peartree NA, Hood LE, Thiel KJ,etal. 2012. Limited physical contact through a mesh barrier is sufficient for social reward-conditioned place preference in adolescent male rats[J]. Physiology & Behavior, 105(3): 749-756.

    Qi RL, Qu JG, Chen YM,etal. 2011. Differences in morphine-induced and food-induced conditioned place preference between adolescent and adult mice[J]. Zoological Research, 32(5): 528-532.

    Rezayof A, Assadpour S, Alijanpour S. 2013. Morphine-induced anxiolytic-like effect in morphine-sensitized mice: involvement of ventral hippocampal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors[J]. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 103(3): 460-466.

    Shin IC, Kim HC, Swanson J,etal. 2003. Anxiolytic effects of acute morphine can be modulated by nitric oxide systems[J]. Pharmacology, 68(4): 183-189.

    Thiel KJ, Okun AC, Neisewander JL. 2008. Social reward-conditioned place preference: a model revealing an interaction between cocaine and social context rewards in rats[J]. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 96(3): 202-212.

    To CT, Bagdy G. 1999. Anxiogenic effect of central CCK administration is attenuated by chronic fluoxetine or ipsapirone treatment[J]. Neuropharmacology, 38(2): 279-282.

    Van den Berg CL, Kitchen I, Gerrits MAFM,etal. 1999a. Morphine treatment during juvenile isolation increases social activity and opioid peptides release in the adult rat[J]. Brain Research, 830(1): 16-23.

    Van den Berg CL, Pijlman FT, Koning HA,etal. 1999b. Isolation changes the incentive value of sucrose and social behaviour in juvenile and adult rats[J]. Behavioural Brain Research, 106(1-2): 133-142.

    Vanhille N, Belin-Rauscent A, Mar AC,etal. 2015. High locomotor reactivity to novelty is associated with an increased propensity to choose saccharin over cocaine: new insights into the vulnerability to addiction[J]. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(3): 577-589.

    Walter H, Abler B, Ciaramidaro A,etal. 2005. Motivating forces of human actions: neuroimaging reward and social interaction[J]. Brain Research Bulletin, 67(5): 368-381.

    Zhan B, Ma HY, Wang JL,etal. 2015. Sex differences in morphine-induced behavioral sensitization and social behaviors in ICR mice[J]. Zoological Research, 36(2): 103-108.

    Zhang D, Zhou X, Wang X,etal. 2007. Morphine withdrawal decreases responding reinforced by sucrose self-administration in progressive ratio[J]. Addiction Biology, 12(2): 152-157.

    2015-07-21 接受日期:2015-11-04

    國家自然科學基金項目(31260513, 31460565); 北方民族大學研究生創(chuàng)新基金項目(YCX1526); 國家大學生創(chuàng)新實驗項目(201411407039)

    古再麗努爾·買買提(1990—), 女, 碩士研究生

    Q959.837

    A

    1000-7083(2016)01-0044-08

    慢性嗎啡處理及戒斷對社會新穎性和食物尋求行為的影響

    古再麗努爾·買買提, 王建禮*, 詹泊, 劉朝寶, 陳墨軒

    (北方民族大學生物科學與工程學院,銀川750021)

    阿片肽在調控社會性和非社會性天然獎賞中具有重要作用。尋求天然獎賞的動機與動物的情緒狀態(tài)有關。嗎啡注射和戒斷會引起動物不同的情緒狀態(tài)。本研究探討了嗎啡處理期和戒斷期是否會影響動物對2種類型天然獎賞(社會新穎性和食物)的尋求行為。雄性小鼠連續(xù)注射14 d的嗎啡(10 mg·kg-1,生理鹽水作對照組),在最后一次注射20 min后,依次檢測了小鼠的運動性及對熟悉個體、陌生個體和食物的接近時間。戒斷7 d后,以上行為變量(除運動性)被再次檢測。結果表明,鹽水處理及戒斷的小鼠對陌生個體的接近時間明顯多于熟悉個體,且表現出對食物的偏好。嗎啡處理的小鼠運動性增加,接近陌生個體和熟悉個體的時間差異無統計學意義,但仍表現出對食物的偏好。與鹽水組相比,嗎啡戒斷降低了小鼠對食物的偏好,但不影響對陌生個體的接近時間。這些結果表明,嗎啡會損害尋求社會新穎性和食物的動機;慢性嗎啡處理和戒斷對這2種天然獎賞的尋求行為有不同的影響,這種差異可能與嗎啡經驗誘導的不同神經適應有關。

    嗎啡; 戒斷; 天然獎賞; 社會刺激; 食物

    10.11984/j.issn.1000-7083.20150237

    *通信作者Corresponding author, 男, 博士, 副教授, 主要從事行為生理學研究, E-mail:wang_jianli@163.com

    猜你喜歡
    買買提努爾新穎性
    《天山牧人家》
    新疆藝術(2023年5期)2023-10-30 05:21:58
    《黑馬》(油畫)
    新疆藝術(2023年1期)2023-10-11 17:54:23
    阿依努爾——獻給一位哈薩克支教女孩
    心聲歌刊(2021年2期)2021-07-16 07:05:56
    外觀新穎性對消費者購買意愿的影響:自我建構與產品類型的調節(jié)效應
    心理學報(2020年11期)2020-11-13 05:41:28
    日本計劃將新穎性寬限期延長至12個月
    艾力江·買買提國畫作品
    新疆藝術(2017年1期)2017-10-27 08:15:18
    最笨公主的間諜人生(下)
    五千個買買提
    希努爾著力于轉型重組
    《國防專利條例》新穎性標準應當及時進行修改
    中國軍轉民(2013年9期)2013-08-15 00:43:44
    国产伦人伦偷精品视频| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 色哟哟·www| 中国美女看黄片| 亚洲精品456在线播放app | 亚洲国产欧美人成| 国产成人aa在线观看| 精品久久久久久久久久免费视频| 夜夜看夜夜爽夜夜摸| 一区福利在线观看| 欧美黑人巨大hd| 极品教师在线视频| 两性午夜刺激爽爽歪歪视频在线观看| 2021天堂中文幕一二区在线观| 亚洲熟妇熟女久久| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 亚洲在线观看片| 亚洲中文字幕日韩| 亚洲国产欧洲综合997久久,| 好男人在线观看高清免费视频| 国产在视频线在精品| 亚洲色图av天堂| 一级a爱片免费观看的视频| 噜噜噜噜噜久久久久久91| 1024手机看黄色片| 精品久久久久久久久久免费视频| 99久久久亚洲精品蜜臀av| 免费不卡的大黄色大毛片视频在线观看 | 我要搜黄色片| 韩国av在线不卡| 99久久精品一区二区三区| 永久网站在线| 国产精品三级大全| 国内精品久久久久久久电影| 欧美+日韩+精品| av天堂在线播放| 国产精品日韩av在线免费观看| 久久久久九九精品影院| 亚洲欧美日韩高清在线视频| 国产精品综合久久久久久久免费| 亚洲熟妇熟女久久| 日本撒尿小便嘘嘘汇集6| 中文字幕高清在线视频| 欧美日本亚洲视频在线播放| 国产黄色小视频在线观看| 最新在线观看一区二区三区| 日韩,欧美,国产一区二区三区 | 国产精品人妻久久久影院| 搡老妇女老女人老熟妇| 一本久久中文字幕| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 欧美绝顶高潮抽搐喷水| 男女做爰动态图高潮gif福利片| 91在线精品国自产拍蜜月| 麻豆av噜噜一区二区三区| 在线观看舔阴道视频| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 99九九线精品视频在线观看视频| 色av中文字幕| 亚洲成人免费电影在线观看| 亚洲国产欧美人成| 国产精品伦人一区二区| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av| 国产高清激情床上av| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 极品教师在线视频| 在线观看免费视频日本深夜| 国产欧美日韩精品亚洲av| 精品欧美国产一区二区三| 精品久久久久久久久久免费视频| 男人舔奶头视频| 国产精品自产拍在线观看55亚洲| 桃色一区二区三区在线观看| 看免费成人av毛片| 久久亚洲精品不卡| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 啦啦啦观看免费观看视频高清| 亚洲一区二区三区色噜噜| 国产亚洲91精品色在线| 夜夜夜夜夜久久久久| 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| www.色视频.com| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 色尼玛亚洲综合影院| 小说图片视频综合网站| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av| 嫁个100分男人电影在线观看| 国产成人a区在线观看| 亚洲中文字幕一区二区三区有码在线看| 日韩欧美三级三区| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 毛片女人毛片| 久久久久久久久中文| 人人妻人人澡欧美一区二区| 嫩草影院新地址| 久久草成人影院| 久久久久国产精品人妻aⅴ院| 精品国产三级普通话版| 给我免费播放毛片高清在线观看| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 色在线成人网| 97热精品久久久久久| 最近最新中文字幕大全电影3| 欧美日韩综合久久久久久 | 极品教师在线视频| 全区人妻精品视频| 淫秽高清视频在线观看| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 亚洲专区国产一区二区| 国产精品一区二区三区四区久久| aaaaa片日本免费| 特级一级黄色大片| 久久亚洲精品不卡| 国产成人aa在线观看| 韩国av一区二区三区四区| 亚洲国产精品sss在线观看| 色播亚洲综合网| 一级a爱片免费观看的视频| 亚洲黑人精品在线| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 精品国产三级普通话版| 午夜精品久久久久久毛片777| 看黄色毛片网站| 最好的美女福利视频网| 成人无遮挡网站| 淫妇啪啪啪对白视频| 91久久精品电影网| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 精品99又大又爽又粗少妇毛片 | 美女 人体艺术 gogo| 欧美+日韩+精品| 亚洲成人中文字幕在线播放| 欧美成人a在线观看| 久久人妻av系列| 免费在线观看影片大全网站| 丰满乱子伦码专区| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看| 国产一区二区三区视频了| 淫秽高清视频在线观看| 久久久久精品国产欧美久久久| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 国产黄a三级三级三级人| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| 三级男女做爰猛烈吃奶摸视频| 97人妻精品一区二区三区麻豆| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 制服丝袜大香蕉在线| 99精品久久久久人妻精品| а√天堂www在线а√下载| 露出奶头的视频| 国产精品爽爽va在线观看网站| 九九热线精品视视频播放| 99国产精品一区二区蜜桃av| 高清毛片免费观看视频网站| 免费看日本二区| 老师上课跳d突然被开到最大视频| 国产在线男女| 韩国av一区二区三区四区| 久久久午夜欧美精品| 亚洲一级一片aⅴ在线观看| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 精品无人区乱码1区二区| 老司机午夜福利在线观看视频| 亚洲三级黄色毛片| 欧美高清成人免费视频www| 亚洲性久久影院| 欧美日韩综合久久久久久 | 丰满乱子伦码专区| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产 | 又紧又爽又黄一区二区| 亚洲 国产 在线| 悠悠久久av| 久久这里只有精品中国| 色哟哟·www| а√天堂www在线а√下载| 亚洲成人久久爱视频| 人妻久久中文字幕网| 亚洲最大成人中文| 国内精品美女久久久久久| 亚洲美女视频黄频| 变态另类丝袜制服| 欧美成人性av电影在线观看| 国产亚洲欧美98| 国产成人a区在线观看| 国产精品98久久久久久宅男小说| 国产精品av视频在线免费观看| 亚洲最大成人av| 99精品久久久久人妻精品| 九九爱精品视频在线观看| 尾随美女入室| 成人国产一区最新在线观看| 国产一区二区三区av在线 | 国产白丝娇喘喷水9色精品| 精品久久久久久,| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 亚洲人成伊人成综合网2020| 久久久久免费精品人妻一区二区| 午夜影院日韩av| 亚洲七黄色美女视频| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类 | 最新中文字幕久久久久| 亚洲无线在线观看| 99热这里只有精品一区| 91久久精品电影网| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 国产精品免费一区二区三区在线| 又爽又黄无遮挡网站| 欧美三级亚洲精品| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 大又大粗又爽又黄少妇毛片口| 午夜福利18| 美女cb高潮喷水在线观看| 91在线观看av| 亚洲av免费在线观看| 又爽又黄无遮挡网站| 日本-黄色视频高清免费观看| 亚洲av日韩精品久久久久久密| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 日本免费a在线| 精品久久久久久久久久免费视频| 亚洲美女视频黄频| 久久久国产成人精品二区| 亚洲成人免费电影在线观看| 欧美激情在线99| 成人欧美大片| 在线免费观看的www视频| 又爽又黄无遮挡网站| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 亚洲av熟女| 大又大粗又爽又黄少妇毛片口| 欧美bdsm另类| 干丝袜人妻中文字幕| 国内精品美女久久久久久| 两个人视频免费观看高清| 欧美潮喷喷水| 亚洲综合色惰| 欧美成人免费av一区二区三区| av天堂中文字幕网| 国产精品自产拍在线观看55亚洲| 啦啦啦韩国在线观看视频| 国产主播在线观看一区二区| 夜夜夜夜夜久久久久| 国产一区二区三区视频了| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 免费电影在线观看免费观看| 亚洲国产精品久久男人天堂| 美女免费视频网站| 亚洲欧美激情综合另类| 三级国产精品欧美在线观看| 啪啪无遮挡十八禁网站| 欧美绝顶高潮抽搐喷水| 我要看日韩黄色一级片| 国产伦精品一区二区三区四那| 久久久成人免费电影| 免费在线观看成人毛片| 日韩一本色道免费dvd| 成人无遮挡网站| 两性午夜刺激爽爽歪歪视频在线观看| a级毛片a级免费在线| 午夜久久久久精精品| 成人三级黄色视频| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 亚洲一区二区三区色噜噜| 一个人观看的视频www高清免费观看| 欧美高清成人免费视频www| 国产成年人精品一区二区| 99热这里只有精品一区| www日本黄色视频网| 国产成人aa在线观看| 在线天堂最新版资源| 最近中文字幕高清免费大全6 | 深夜精品福利| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 搡女人真爽免费视频火全软件 | 级片在线观看| 久久精品夜夜夜夜夜久久蜜豆| 日本撒尿小便嘘嘘汇集6| 我的老师免费观看完整版| videossex国产| 欧美日韩国产亚洲二区| 亚洲最大成人av| 亚洲三级黄色毛片| 亚洲av成人av| 人妻丰满熟妇av一区二区三区| 久久婷婷人人爽人人干人人爱| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 免费观看的影片在线观看| 久久久久国产精品人妻aⅴ院| 中文字幕熟女人妻在线| 日韩一区二区视频免费看| 美女黄网站色视频| 最近最新中文字幕大全电影3| 在线观看一区二区三区| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区| 国产男靠女视频免费网站| 能在线免费观看的黄片| 91久久精品电影网| 99精品久久久久人妻精品| 日韩中文字幕欧美一区二区| 久久久久九九精品影院| 中文亚洲av片在线观看爽| 免费人成视频x8x8入口观看| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 午夜亚洲福利在线播放| 伦精品一区二区三区| 91久久精品国产一区二区三区| 午夜精品一区二区三区免费看| 国产免费av片在线观看野外av| 亚洲欧美精品综合久久99| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 国产真实乱freesex| 国产精品久久久久久久电影| 春色校园在线视频观看| 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 少妇高潮的动态图| 99久久无色码亚洲精品果冻| 国产乱人伦免费视频| 69人妻影院| av专区在线播放| 亚洲av美国av| 久久草成人影院| 国产熟女欧美一区二区| 色精品久久人妻99蜜桃| 亚洲性久久影院| 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 中文字幕免费在线视频6| 色在线成人网| 国产v大片淫在线免费观看| 久久人人精品亚洲av| 亚洲人成伊人成综合网2020| 亚洲真实伦在线观看| 国产精品美女特级片免费视频播放器| 国内少妇人妻偷人精品xxx网站| 久久久色成人| 老熟妇仑乱视频hdxx| 国产欧美日韩一区二区精品| 91久久精品电影网| bbb黄色大片| 午夜福利高清视频| 日韩 亚洲 欧美在线| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看 | 亚洲黑人精品在线| 免费av不卡在线播放| 欧美黑人巨大hd| 伦理电影大哥的女人| 精品免费久久久久久久清纯| 国产毛片a区久久久久| 两个人视频免费观看高清| 免费看光身美女| 亚洲av一区综合| 国产午夜精品论理片| 嫩草影院精品99| 国产精品人妻久久久久久| 国产一区二区激情短视频| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 久久精品综合一区二区三区| 波多野结衣巨乳人妻| 欧美日韩中文字幕国产精品一区二区三区| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 我要看日韩黄色一级片| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱 | 麻豆国产97在线/欧美| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| a级毛片免费高清观看在线播放| 联通29元200g的流量卡| 色哟哟·www| 天堂av国产一区二区熟女人妻| 国产av不卡久久| 免费观看精品视频网站| 丰满乱子伦码专区| av在线亚洲专区| 最近视频中文字幕2019在线8| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验 | 国产精品一及| 国产精品一区二区免费欧美| 有码 亚洲区| 99热精品在线国产| 国产一区二区亚洲精品在线观看| netflix在线观看网站| 亚洲性夜色夜夜综合| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验 | 有码 亚洲区| 美女被艹到高潮喷水动态| 国产精品三级大全| av在线亚洲专区| 男女之事视频高清在线观看| 成人高潮视频无遮挡免费网站| 国产久久久一区二区三区| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线| 尤物成人国产欧美一区二区三区| 在线免费观看的www视频| 亚洲在线观看片| 99在线人妻在线中文字幕| 美女被艹到高潮喷水动态| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品| 91狼人影院| 欧美日韩乱码在线| 亚洲最大成人中文| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 亚洲成人免费电影在线观看| 成人性生交大片免费视频hd| 此物有八面人人有两片| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 亚洲美女视频黄频| 亚洲在线观看片| 国产精品三级大全| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| 国产毛片a区久久久久| 亚洲精华国产精华精| 亚洲成人免费电影在线观看| 久99久视频精品免费| 亚洲精品在线观看二区| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| 在线观看一区二区三区| av在线老鸭窝| 综合色av麻豆| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 亚洲精品久久国产高清桃花| 国产高清视频在线播放一区| 两人在一起打扑克的视频| 精华霜和精华液先用哪个| 91精品国产九色| 久久这里只有精品中国| 久久久精品大字幕| 我要看日韩黄色一级片| 成人国产综合亚洲| 亚洲va在线va天堂va国产| 国产一区二区三区视频了| 麻豆成人午夜福利视频| 村上凉子中文字幕在线| 欧美国产日韩亚洲一区| 免费不卡的大黄色大毛片视频在线观看 | 真人做人爱边吃奶动态| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 久久久久久久久久成人| 一个人观看的视频www高清免费观看| 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 一区福利在线观看| 日韩欧美国产在线观看| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 99精品在免费线老司机午夜| 国产精品女同一区二区软件 | 亚洲四区av| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| aaaaa片日本免费| 亚洲成人中文字幕在线播放| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 欧美性猛交黑人性爽| 欧美日韩国产亚洲二区| 国产精品久久视频播放| 成人毛片a级毛片在线播放| 久久久久性生活片| 国产高清激情床上av| 精品一区二区三区av网在线观看| 色综合站精品国产| 国产探花极品一区二区| 嫁个100分男人电影在线观看| 一进一出抽搐gif免费好疼| 夜夜夜夜夜久久久久| 午夜a级毛片| 真实男女啪啪啪动态图| 哪里可以看免费的av片| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区| 日韩av在线大香蕉| 精品一区二区三区视频在线观看免费| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| 亚洲美女视频黄频| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| 午夜亚洲福利在线播放| 亚洲成人免费电影在线观看| 亚洲人成伊人成综合网2020| 国产高清激情床上av| 国产色婷婷99| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| h日本视频在线播放| 精品欧美国产一区二区三| av在线观看视频网站免费| 亚洲精品乱码久久久v下载方式| 亚洲av免费在线观看| 国产亚洲精品综合一区在线观看| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 亚洲成人久久性| 亚洲成a人片在线一区二区| 男女下面进入的视频免费午夜| 淫妇啪啪啪对白视频| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 免费无遮挡裸体视频| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 午夜影院日韩av| 高清在线国产一区| a级毛片免费高清观看在线播放| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 国产午夜精品久久久久久一区二区三区 | 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 午夜福利欧美成人| 免费人成在线观看视频色| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 亚洲成人免费电影在线观看| xxxwww97欧美| 高清日韩中文字幕在线| 中文字幕高清在线视频| 男女视频在线观看网站免费| 日本免费一区二区三区高清不卡| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看| 亚洲国产欧洲综合997久久,| 免费看光身美女| 国产亚洲av嫩草精品影院| 免费看a级黄色片| 级片在线观看| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 国产高清不卡午夜福利| 18禁黄网站禁片午夜丰满| 久久精品久久久久久噜噜老黄 | 69人妻影院| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在 | 亚洲性夜色夜夜综合| 国产高清不卡午夜福利| 亚洲人成伊人成综合网2020| 亚洲综合色惰| 搡老熟女国产l中国老女人| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| av在线观看视频网站免费| 国产在视频线在精品| 舔av片在线| 久久香蕉精品热| 夜夜夜夜夜久久久久| 美女黄网站色视频| 久久久久免费精品人妻一区二区| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频| 最好的美女福利视频网| 国产精品一区二区免费欧美| 国产黄色小视频在线观看| 不卡一级毛片| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| 变态另类丝袜制服| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 三级毛片av免费| 国产主播在线观看一区二区| 人人妻人人看人人澡| 久久午夜福利片| 日日啪夜夜撸| 老女人水多毛片| 免费观看人在逋| 精品一区二区三区人妻视频| 欧美成人免费av一区二区三区| 国产av不卡久久| 亚洲av五月六月丁香网| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 国产免费av片在线观看野外av| 亚洲成av人片在线播放无| 97热精品久久久久久| 人人妻人人澡欧美一区二区| 精品久久久久久久久久免费视频| 91在线精品国自产拍蜜月| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 日韩欧美在线二视频| 综合色av麻豆| 一区二区三区激情视频| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类 | 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 亚洲四区av| 黄片wwwwww| 国产在线男女| 亚洲va在线va天堂va国产| 国产真实伦视频高清在线观看 | 91在线精品国自产拍蜜月| 老熟妇仑乱视频hdxx| 国产精品永久免费网站| 国产精品女同一区二区软件 | 一级av片app| 九色成人免费人妻av| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 在线看三级毛片| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av| 国产探花极品一区二区| 能在线免费观看的黄片| 久久久久久伊人网av| 熟妇人妻久久中文字幕3abv| 久久午夜亚洲精品久久| 成人国产麻豆网| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区| 久久久色成人| 男女做爰动态图高潮gif福利片| 搡老熟女国产l中国老女人| 亚洲欧美日韩高清专用| 精品国产三级普通话版| 中国美白少妇内射xxxbb| 精品欧美国产一区二区三| 热99在线观看视频| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验 | 午夜福利在线观看吧| 午夜影院日韩av| 欧美潮喷喷水| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 91av网一区二区| 赤兔流量卡办理| 日本一二三区视频观看| www日本黄色视频网| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 久久午夜亚洲精品久久| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 伦精品一区二区三区| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 亚洲一级一片aⅴ在线观看| 国产精品一区二区三区四区免费观看 | 亚洲图色成人| 极品教师在线免费播放|