• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Avian sibling cannibalism: Hoopoe mothers regularly use their last hatched nestlings to feed older siblings

    2022-04-28 06:48:02JuanJosSolerManuelMartVivaldiSoNuhlkovCristinaRuizCastellanonicaMazorraAlonsoEsterMartnezRenauManfredEckenfellnerSvetlHerbertHoi
    Zoological Research 2022年2期

    Juan José Soler, Manuel Martín-Vivaldi, Soňa Nuhlí?ková, Cristina Ruiz-Castellano, Mónica Mazorra-Alonso,Ester Martínez-Renau, Manfred Eckenfellner, Ján Svetlík, Herbert Hoi,*

    1 Departamento de Ecología Funcional y Evolutiva, Estación Experimental de Zonas áridas (EEZA-CSIC), Almería E-04120, Spain

    2 Unidad Asociada Coevolución: Cucos, Hospedadores y Bacterias Simbiontes, Universidad de Granada, Granada E-18003, Spain

    3 Departamento de Zoología, Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad de Granada, Granada E-18003, Spain

    4 Department of Ecology, Faculty of Natural Sciences, Comenius University, Bratislava SK-84215, Slovakia

    5 Konrad Lorenz Institute of Ethology, Department of Interdisciplinary Life Sciences, University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna A-1160,Austria

    ABSTRACT

    Keywords: Brood reduction; Clutch size;Hatching asynchrony; Ice-box hypothesis;Infanticide; Siblicide; Sibling hierarchy; Upupa epops

    lNTRODUCTlON

    Cannibalism was originally deemed as an abnormal or unnatural behavior in the animal kingdom (Dawkins, 1976;Maynard Smith & Price, 1973).However, this view has changed dramatically over the past few decades, and it is now recognized to be adaptive, phylogenetically widespread, and relatively common in nature (Elgar & Crespi, 1992; Hausfater& Hrdy, 1984; Hrdy & Hausfater, 1984; Polis, 1981).

    Sibling cannibalism has been described in a wide range of animal taxa.In most cases, siblings are cannibalized to provide energy and resources during periods of low food abundance (Polis, 1981, 1984; Polis & Myers, 1985).Some invertebrate and arthropod females produce eggs that serve as the first source of postnatal nutrition (Crespi, 1992).In certain vertebrates, such as salamanders, sharks, and fish(Crump, 1992; Fitzgerald & Whoriskey, 1992), sibling cannibalism occurs during the embryonic phase within the mother’s body.Sibling cannibalism of newborn offspring is also phylogenetically widespread (Elgar & Crespi, 1992).Typically, the smallest individuals of a brood are at a greater risk of being eaten by the largest ones, and sibling cannibalism is related to within-brood birth and body size asynchrony (Baur & Baur, 1986; Eickwort, 1973; Polis, 1981).Thus, inducing birth asynchrony can be seen not only as a maternal strategy facilitating brood-size adjustment to environmental conditions, as suggested by the brood reduction hypothesis (Lack, 1954), but also favoring sibling cannibalism of particular offspring (Alexander, 1974), at least in the absence of physical constraints (see below).

    Although hatching asynchrony is common in birds, sibling cannibalism is very rare (Bortolotti et al., 1991; Ingram, 1962;Stanback & Koenig, 1992) and, thus, the use of last hatched nestlings as a food resource, as posited by the larder hypothesis (or ice-box hypothesis (Alexander, 1974)), is thought to be irrelevant for such species (Mock & Parker,1998).In general, it would be far easier for parent birds to increase provisioning efforts than to raise additional nestlings to serve as sibling food, thus the benefits to produce extra eggs as back-up food should hypothetically be restricted to species that do not feed their offspring (Stoleson & Beissinger,1995).Laying extra eggs to hatch asynchronously may not be due to their future use as food, but may serve several functions, including insurance against hatching failures or facilitation of brood-size adjustment (Lack, 1954; Stoleson &Beissinger, 1995).Then, once an “extra” nestling dies, parents or siblings could take advantage and use the body as food.However, even for avian species with obligate siblicide, such as found in raptors, skuas, and pelicans (Morandini & Ferrer,2015), where dead offspring could be consumed by surviving offspring (Bortolotti et al., 1991), there is very little evidence of sibling cannibalism (Stanback & Koenig, 1992).Three arguments have been proposed to account for the rare occurrence of sibling cannibalism in birds related to avianspecific anatomical, physiological, behavioral, or social characteristics (Mock, 1984; Stanback & Koenig, 1992).The first argument posits that the rapid developmental rate of most bird species reduces the difference in body size between the oldest and youngest nestlings, preventing the former from killing and feeding on the latter.The second argument predicts that sibling cannibalism needs parental complicity.Even when species show extreme asymmetries in nestling size, nestlings do not actively take food but are fed by their parents, at least during the hatching period when size asymmetries are greatest.The final argument suggests that most bird species cannot dismember prey with their bills, thus cannibalistic nestlings would have to swallow their siblings whole.These arguments would restrict sibling cannibalism to species with extreme hatching asynchrony, and thus extreme sibling size hierarchy, and with beaks that allow them to tear food apart or with a gape wide enough to swallow a nestling whole.Otherwise, sibling cannibalism during the nesting phase could only occur with the complicity of parents, who would have to commit infanticide to feed their older offspring with younger siblings (Mock, 1984; Stanback & Koenig, 1992).

    In this study, we investigated sibling cannibalism in the Eurasian hoopoe (Upupa epops) and explored possible influencing factors.Briefly, after quantifying the prevalence of sibling cannibalism in two different populations (Spain and Austria) during different breeding seasons, we explored predictive particularities of the breeding biology of hoopoes that may explain the high rate of sibling cannibalism.For instance, as hoopoes cannot tear prey apart, body size differences between nestlings must be large enough to mechanically allow the oldest to swallow the youngest whole(Stanback & Koenig, 1992).Time elapsed between the first and last hatchings (hatching span) is the main determinant of nestling size asymmetry.As such, we predicted a positive relationship between hatching span and the probability of sibling cannibalism.

    As mentioned above, sibling cannibalism in hoopoes may be a consequence of the presence of dead nestlings, or nestlings condemned to die.According to the brood reduction hypothesis (Lack, 1954; Stoleson & Beissinger, 1995),hoopoes adjust brood size to environmental conditions based on food availability (Hildebrandt & Schaub, 2018), thus nestlings that will or have died due to limited food availability could be used by adults to feed older nestlings.In this case,the intensity of brood reduction and occurrence and intensity of sibling cannibalism should be related to each other, and both should depend on variables related to food availability(i.e., latitude, time of breeding, and experimental food supplementation).The availability of food is expected to be higher in northern populations (Lundblad & Conway, 2021;Nuhlí?ková et al., 2016; Olson et al., 2009), while a decrease in food availability can be expected over the course of the breeding season (De Neve et al., 2004; Sorci et al., 1997).Consequently, we predicted that both the prevalence of sibling cannibalism and extent of brood reduction would be higher in Spain, particularly at the end of the breeding season.We also tested the effect of food availability on sibling cannibalism and brood reduction experimentally.The food supplementation experiment started one or two days prior to the end of the hatching period and ended two or three days after the last egg hatched, which is the period when sibling cannibalism occurs.Thus, we expected that the prevalence and intensity of sibling cannibalism and brood reduction would be lower in experimentally supplemented nests than in control nests.

    The intensity of brood reduction depends not only on food availability, but also on brood size, which, in turn, depends on clutch size and hatching success.This is mainly because large clutches that hatch asynchronously may facilitate broodsize adjustment to environmental conditions (Brood Reduction Hypothesis; Lack, 1954)) and ensure an optimistic brood size in case of hatching failure (Insurance Hypothesis; Forbes,1990, 1991; Hardy, 1992).Hoopoes lay large clutches and experience high rates of hatching failure (Soler et al., 2008).Thus, it is possible that large clutches not only reflect the brood size that adults can raise under unrestricted food availability, but also include some “extra eggs” to prevent suboptimal brood sizes in the event of hatching failure.In this case, the intensity of brood reduction, and consequently sibling cannibalism, should be higher in clutches with lower hatching failures.We explored this possibility as it suggests that some cannibalized nestlings may act as insurance against hatching failure and that, without such failures, they are surplus.

    A final prerequisite for the occurrence of sibling cannibalism is that parents facilitate the intake of smaller offspring by their older siblings.In hoopoes, most brood reduction events occur at the end of the hatching period and a few days after (Ryser et al., 2016).During this period, females spend most of their time in the nest and are responsible for feeding the nestlings with food delivered to the nest by their mate (Díaz Lora et al.,2020).Thus, mothers not only determine clutch size and hatching span, but also control food allocation among the brood, including the use of the smallest to feed the older offspring.To distinguish between the above-described scenarios explaining the high rate of sibling cannibalism in hoopoes, it is important to know whether females use the smallest nestlings as offspring food just before or after their death.In addition, if females use live offspring as prey, it would be interesting to know whether cannibalized nestlings are in poor physical condition (i.e., runts) or just recent hatchlings of small size, rather than undernourished.Here, we recorded the behavior of females within 58 hoopoe nests in Spain to identify sibling cannibalism and gather information on the characteristics of nestlings used as prey by their mothers.

    MATERlALS AND METHODS

    Ethics statement

    The study was conducted according to relevant Spanish national (Decreto 105/2011, 19 de abril) and regional guidelines.All necessary permits for hoopoe manipulation were provided by Consejería de Medio Ambiente de la Junta de Andalucía, Spain (Ref: SGYB/FOA/AFR/CFS and SGMN/GyB/JMIF).The study area is not protected, but privately owned, and we obtained permission from the owners to work in the properties.Time spent in each hoopoe nest was the minimum necessary for the experiment.The Austrian research was carried out in accordance with ARRIVE guidelines and the EU Directive 2010/63/EU for animal experiments.The study also adhered to the requirements of Austrian welfare laws.Permission to work with the common hoopoes was provided by the government of Lower Austria(license number RU5-BE-7/010-2011; BD2-N-200/057-2005).Ethical approval and licenses were obtained under the institutional guidelines of the Austrian Academy of Sciences.

    Study species

    Hoopoes are migratory birds distributed throughout Europe,Asia, and Africa.They inhabit open woodlands or open steppes, grassland, pasture, semi-deserts, or crops with scattered trees, walls, or buildings providing holes for nesting and soil without tall vegetation for feeding (Barbaro et al.,2008; Rehsteiner, 1996; Schaub et al., 2010).Females usually lay one or two clutches of 6–9 (range=4–12) eggs during February–July, but only rarely do pairs succeed in producing more than six nestlings in a breeding attempt (Martín-Vivaldi et al., 1999; Plard et al., 2018).Asynchronous hatching is a consequence of females incubating from the laying of the first or second egg, which produces a considerable size hierarchy within the brood (Martín-Vivaldi et al., 1999).Only females incubate, but both sexes care for the offspring.Males feed females during incubation and early in the chick rearing phase(Díaz Lora et al., 2020).Once the oldest chick is about 10 days old, both male and female provision the offspring directly until fledging (24–30 days post-hatch) (Martín-Vivaldi et al.,2014).

    Study areas and general fieldwork

    The Spanish population is located in Guadix (N37°18′,W38°11′) in the south-eastern Iberian Peninsula, where hoopoes breed in nest boxes (5.5 cm (entrance diameter), 24 cm (bottom-to-hole height), 35 cm×18 cm×21 cm (internal height×width×depth)) made from cork and placed in trees or buildings.From early March to late July during the three study years (2017–2019), nest boxes were visited weekly until hoopoe eggs were detected, and then every third day until the end of egg-laying to detect clutch size.The study years represented typical conditions of the semiarid environment,with low average rainfall in May (0.01 mm in 2017 to 1.6 mm in 2018) (Huéneja Meteorological Station; N37°12′54′′,W02°57′48′′; see https://www.juntadeandalucia.es/agriculturay pesca/ifapa/riaweb/web/estacion/18/102#hist).Assuming that hoopoe females lay one egg per day, that incubation is initiated after laying the second egg, and that the incubation period is 18 days (Martín-Vivaldi et al., 1999), we visited the nests again 18–22 days after the day the first egg was laid to determine the hatching date of the first nestling.Once hatchlings were detected in the nest, we visually established nestling age and estimated the expected hatching date of the penultimate or last egg, at which time food supplementation and video recording inside nest boxes commenced (see Supplementary Materials for details on equipment installation)(2019, see below).Nests not subjected to food supplementation (2017–2018) were again visited 8 days after hatching to estimate brood size and hatching failures, and again at the end of the nestling period to estimate brood reduction.

    The Austrian population is located in the wine-growing region of Wagram (119 km2, 230–350 m a.s.l., N48°45′–48°47′; E15°66′–15°73′), Lower Austria.The region is a traditionally cultivated landscape comprised of southernexposed slopes covered mostly (80%) with vineyards.Theentire hoopoe population breeds in wooden nest boxes (6 cm entrance diameter and 24 cm bottom-to-hole height, 50 cm×35 cm×25 cm (height×width×depth)) (Nuhlí?ková et al.,2016).To simulate natural breeding site conditions, the nest boxes were placed in loess walls or hidden in abandoned wooden cottages (Kri?tofík et al., 2013).Nests were inspected regularly during April to July (2009–2010) for purposes other than detecting cannibalistic infanticide.As in the Spanish population, we visited nest boxes weekly until hoopoe eggs were detected inside, then inspected the nests after three weeks to detect hatching date, and a few days before fledging.These nest visits allowed us to estimate reproductive variables to compare with the Spanish population.Camera recording(see Supplementary Materials for details on equipment installation) began when the oldest nest-mate was able to move around the nest-box (~10 days old, when all eggs were usually hatched).The two study years (2009–2010)represented different environmental conditions.In the wet year(2010), measurable rainfall was about one and a half times that in 2009 (May 113 mm vs.79 mm, respectively)(Langenlois Meteorological Station; N48°28′ , E15°41′ ; see http://www.zamg.at).For the purposes of the current study, we followed 67 broods (29 nests in 2009 and 38 nests in 2010).

    During the 2017–2018 breeding seasons, we frequently recorded sibling cannibalism in the Spanish population in late hatching (i.e., June-July) hoopoe nests with brood sizes larger than seven nestlings (50.5% (n=6) and 54.5% (n=11) of the recorded hoopoe nests in 2017 and 2018, respectively).During the 2019 breeding season, we evaluated the intensity and prevalence of sibling cannibalism in the entire Spanish hoopoe population.We recorded videos of all available hoopoe nests during the time period when sibling cannibalism occurred (i.e., at the end and few days after the hatching period) and gathered information to explore the possible influencing factors predicted to affect both the extent of brood reduction during that period and the occurrence of sibling cannibalism (i.e., clutch size, hatching date, hatching failures,brood size, hatching span, and experimentally supplemented food).

    In 2019, when food was supplemented and most Spanish hoopoe nests were video recorded (n=58), we directly observed sibling cannibalism in 21 out of the 30 nests in which cannibalism was assumed it has occurred.In the nine nests where sibling cannibalism was not physically recorded, we observed females handling small nestlings as prey, with nestlings found to have disappeared from those nests after video recording failure.In Austria, of the 48 hoopoe nests that showed brood reduction, the bodies of small nestlings that died a few days after hatching were found in 42 nests at the end of the nestling period.Of the six nests where dead nestlings were not detected, sibling cannibalism was directly observed in two.Thus, in Austria, indirect inferences of sibling cannibalism were based on the sudden disappearance of nestlings between consecutive nest visits.

    Food supplementation experiment

    For the food supplementation experiment, we added 20(approximately 15 mL) crickets (Gryllus campestris) to the experimental nests, with the remaining nests maintained as the control.Fresh crickets were frozen and stored at -20 °C for a maximum of 15 days.The head and feet of the frozen crickets were removed, and they were maintained at room temperature for a minimum of 3 h before being introduced into the hoopoe nests.Hoopoe females in the experimental nests rapidly detected experimental crickets in the nest box and used all of them to feed their offspring and themselves(Supplementary Video S1).Supplemented crickets were usually only available for 1–2 h.

    Nest boxes under experimental and control treatment were visited daily to add the crickets to the nests and to renew batteries and SD memory cards.The experiment started one or two days before the last hoopoe egg hatched and continued until sibling cannibalism was directly detected or inferred from the disappearance of smaller nestlings, or the second or third day after the end of hatching, when body mass of the small living nestlings was close to 10 g, or dead nestlings in the nest boxes were decaying.On the last day of recording, we weighed all nestlings in the nests with a digital balance(Ascher, China; accuracy 0.01 g) and established body-mass hierarchy (body mass of largest nestling divided by body mass of smallest sibling).When sibling cannibalism occurred, we conservatively assumed 4 g (weight of healthy one-day-old nestling) for those nestlings that disappeared when estimating the level of brood hierarchy.Based on daily nest visits and video recordings, we determined the hatching date of the last egg and estimated hatching span (number of days elapsed from hatching of first to last egg), hatching failure (number of eggs that failed to hatch), and brood reduction (number of nestlings that died during the experiment, i.e., approximately 10 days after the older nestlings hatched).

    We assigned hoopoe nests to experimental treatments sequentially depending on hatching date and brood size, i.e.,we concentrated on nests with more than four successfully hatched eggs.We did so because broods smaller than five rarely occurred in nests of the Spanish population (data from non-experimental hoopoe nests followed in 2017–2018:2.86%,n=105), where we observed brood reduction only sporadically (one out of three nests with brood size lower than five).Thus, 22 and 26 hoopoe nests with a brood size larger than four were assigned to the experimental and control groups, respectively.It should be noted that the experimental effects did not vary qualitatively (i.e., statistical significance)when including the nine nests with brood sizes less than four in analyses, nor when restricting analyses to nests where sibling cannibalism was directly observed (results not shown).Moreover, we included first and second breeding attempts in 10 nest boxes.In four of these, brood size was lower than five in one of the breeding attempts, and thus was excluded from one set of analyses.In the remaining six nest boxes, the first and second breeding attempts received different experimental treatment, with four showing different experimental outcomes between the first and second reproductive events.Thus, nestbox identity did not predict experimental outcome, and therefore first and second experimental events were considered statistically independent.Finally, we did not detect any bias associated with clutch size, hatching date, hatching failure, hatching span, and brood size of experimental and control nests (allF1,46<1.11, allP>0.29).

    Video recordings during the 2019 breeding season were also used to study female provisioning behavior and evaluate whether they neglected the small last-hatched chicks in the nest (as previously suggested by direct observations (Martín-Vivaldi et al., 1999)).Whenever possible, we used recordings from the day when the last eggs hatched, and when experimental crickets were not available in the nests.Evaluation of female provisioning behavior started 1 h after the recording equipment was recharged.Briefly, we divided the brood into small-, medium-, and large-sized nestlings that could be visually differentiated, and then counted the number of feeds that the different-sized nestlings received over a 2 h and 15 min period.We then assessed the average number of feeds for each group of nestlings by considering the number of nestlings within each group and estimated the feeding rates of the different-sized nestlings as the average value of each group divided by the sum of the averages of all three groups.Due to recording problems, we were unable to visualize female feeding behavior in 10 of the 58 filmed nests.

    Statistical analyses

    To explore changes in variables describing hoopoe breeding biology based on study year and population, we used information collected from the Spanish population during the 2017–2018 breeding seasons and Austrian population during the 2009–2010 breeding seasons.Factors affecting the probability of sibling cannibalism, including those related to the experimental food supply, were explored using data collected from the Spanish population in 2019, when we focused on frequency of sibling cannibalism in the population.

    Between-population comparisons were carried out using univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) and Chi-square statistical tests.The effects of food supplementation on the probability of cannibalism and brood reduction were explored using generalized linear models with binomial distribution and logit link function (GLZ).The models included laying date and hatching span as additional continuous independent factors.Likelihood type 3 tests were used to estimate two-tailedP-values.The general linear models were used to explore the effects of experimental treatment, hatching date, and hatching span on the intensity of brood reduction and sibling cannibalism in the experimental hoopoe nests.Pearson correlation was employed to explore the associations between preference for feeding smaller nestlings and magnitude of brood reduction, hatching date, and hatching span.Statistical analyses were performed using Statistica (v13, Dell-Inc,2015).

    RESULTS

    Breeding biology and sibling cannibalism: betweenpopulation and year comparisons

    The Austrian and Spanish hoopoes laid clutches of similar size, and although significant among-year variation was detected in both populations, hatching failure (i.e., number of eggs that failed to hatch) was more frequent and intense in the Austrian nests than in the Spanish nests (Table 1).Consequently, at the end of the hatching period, the Austrian broods were significantly smaller than the Spanish broods.Although significant annual variation in brood reduction intensity was detected in both populations, it was significantly higher in the Spanish population (Table 1).Interestingly, the prevalence of brood reduction in the Spanish and Austrian populations did not differ significantly (Table 1).We also detected between-population differences in brood size(number of hatched chicks) and brood-reduction intensity(number of nestlings that died during nestling period), but not in fledgling production (Table 1).

    In the Austrian population, sibling cannibalism(Supplementary Video S2) was slightly, but non-significantly,more frequent in 2010 (13.16%,n=38) than in 2009 (3.33%,n=30) (χ21=2.01,P=0.156).Moreover, independent of the study year, the prevalence of sibling cannibalism was consistently higher in the Spanish (51.72%,n=58, data from 2019 breeding season) than in the Austrian population (2009:χ21=20.29,P<0.000 1; 2010:χ21=14.74,P=0.000 1).Interestingly, when sibling cannibalism occurred in the Spanish population (8–10 days after the first egg hatched),and as a consequence of the extreme hatching span (mean(CI 95%)=5.62 (5.18–6.06) days), the body mass of the oldest nestling (mean (CI 95%)=37.5 (34.9–40.2) g, minmax=16.0–63.0 g,n=54) was almost an order of magnitude greater than that of the youngest nestling (mean (CI 95%)=5.5(4.5–6.6) g, min-max=1.7–23.4 g,n=54).

    Factors affecting sibling cannibalism and brood reduction in Spain

    Hatching span was the factor most closely associated with the prevalence and intensity of sibling cannibalism and brood reduction that occurred during the first 10 days after the first egg hatched (Table 2).Supplementation of food at the end of the hatching period significantly reduced the probability of sibling cannibalism (Figure 1), but not its intensity (Table 2).Experimental food did not affect the prevalence (Figure 1) or intensity of brood reduction (Table 2).Finally, brood reduction intensity, but not sibling cannibalism, increased as the season progressed (Table 2).

    Table 1 Life history traits of Spanish and Austrian hoopoes in two different study years

    Table 2 Factors affecting sibling cannibalism and brood reduction

    In addition to the 30 (n=58) nests in which sibling cannibalism was detected in Spain during the 2019 breeding season, females themselves were observed cannibalizing offspring in three nests (Supplementary Video S3), and one male was recorded entering another nest and picking up a small nestling as if it was prey (Supplementary Video S4).The intensity of cannibalism by either sibling or adult increased with clutch size (Beta (SE)=0.36 (0.12),F1,55=9.41,P=0.003)and decreased as hatching failures increased (Beta(SE)=–0.29 (0.12),F1,55=6.09,P=0.017, Figure 2).The same patterns were detected when trying to explain brood reduction intensity (Clutch size:Beta (SE)=0.43 (0.12),F1,55=13.70,P<0.001; hatching failures:Beta (SE)=–0.25 (0.12),F1,55=4.73,P=0.034, Figure 2).Moreover, clutch size did not predict hatching failures (R=–0.06,n=58,P=0.64), and thus brood reduction intensity was closely related to cannibalism intensity(R=0.77,n=58,P<0.000 1) (Figure 2).

    Figure 1 Sibling cannibalism and experimental food supply

    Figure 2 Cannibalism and life history traits

    Female provisioning behavior, infanticide, brood reduction, and cannibalism

    Only 10.4% (CI 95%=8.7%–13.0%,n=50 nests) of feedingevents were directed towards the third of the brood that included the smallest nestlings.Moreover, the rate of feeding the smallest nestlings was negatively correlated with the subsequently observed brood reduction intensity (R=–0.32,t48=2.31,P=0.025), but not with the probability of sibling cannibalism (logistic regression,χ21=1.45,P=0.228).These results suggest that females allocate fewer resources to the smallest nestlings when they are going to starve, not necessarily because they are going to use them as prey to feed older siblings.

    In all hoopoe nests with evidence of sibling cannibalism,females were recorded handling some of the smallest offspring as prey, even when they were begging for food and did not display outward signs of poor physical condition, i.e.,when not fed for several hours.Moreover, in several recorded cannibalism events, a female was observed directly removing a hatchling from an eggshell to feed to an older offspring(Supplementary Video S5).Thus, hoopoe mothers areobviously responsible for brood reduction, including sibling cannibalism events, but the presence of nestlings in poor condition does not necessarily trigger this behavior, i.e., a high likelihood of starving does not necessarily precede sibling cannibalism.

    DlSCUSSlON

    Hoopoes lay large clutches, start incubation with the first eggs,and produce large and asynchronously hatched broods, with the last hatched chicks almost invariably dying soon after hatching.In the current study, video recordings confirmed that mothers were responsible for nestling death, either indirectly by discarding the smallest nestlings when provisioning, or directly by treating them as prey items to feed older offspring,which occurred before the nestling died and independent of its physical condition.Moreover, as expected for species that are unable to dismember prey and for which sibling cannibalism is constrained by the relative size of consumed and consumer nestlings, our results showed that hatching span was the mainpredictor of the probability and intensity of sibling cannibalism.However, many other species, such as owls (e.g.,Athene noctuaandOtus scops) and rollers (Coracias garrulus),experience longer hatching spans, but, as far as we know, do not exhibit sibling cannibalism.

    Most observations of sibling cannibalism in birds occur after episodes of siblicide due to intra-brood competition under restricted nutritional conditions (Bortolotti et al., 1991; Mock,1984; Morandini & Ferrer, 2015; Stanback & Koenig, 1992).However, this was not the case for hoopoe nestlings, as they did not show any noticeable within-brood aggression and infanticide was entirely dependent on female behavior.Killing one’s own nestlings is an extreme form of parental favoritism(Caro et al., 2016; Mock, 2004; Moreno, 2012).Our results clearly showed that sibling cannibalism is a general strategy in the European hoopoe orchestrated by females, who are responsible for feeding offspring during the hatching period and for a few days afterwards.On the one hand, we detected a negative relationship between the rate of feeding the smallest nestlings and subsequently observed intensity of brood reduction, but no correlation with the probability of sibling cannibalism.This implies that sibling cannibalism, as part of the brood reduction strategy, depends less on females feeding the smallest, last-hatched nestlings than whole brood reduction.On the other hand, all recorded cases of sibling cannibalism were preceded by females treating the to-becannibalized nestling as prey, even when it was alive and still begging for food.In a few cases, mothers even fed older siblings with offspring just after hatching.Thus, sibling cannibalism events are not necessarily the consequence of females using nestlings in poor condition to feed older siblings, but rather the consequence of females using extra nestlings condemned to die as food to satiate largest siblings.Whether these extra nestlings are the consequence of laying extra eggs as a way to store food reserves for bad times (i.e.,Larder Hypothesis (Alexander, 1974)) is a matter of debate(see below).

    The intensity and prevalence of hatching failure, brood reduction, and fledging production, but not brood size, varied among study years.Austrian and Spanish hoopoes laid similar clutch sizes, resulting in similar fledging production.However,while hatching failure occurred more frequently in the northern population, brood reduction was more intense in the southern population.Sibling cannibalism, as part of brood reduction,was also more frequent in the south than in the north.Thus, it is possible that the lower rate of hatching failure makes insurance less necessary in the south, where the relatively large number of extra-nestlings are most times used as a resource to feed older nestlings.Below, we discuss the experimental and empirical results from the Spanish population concerning factors that may explain sibling cannibalism, brood reduction, and hatching failure to determine whether the first two are at least partly the result of the latter.

    The supplementation of hoopoe nests with crickets(preferred hoopoe prey in the study area (Martín-Vivaldi et al.,2014)) at the end of the nestling period resulted in a reduction in the use of the smallest nestlings as food, strongly suggesting a link between food availability and sibling cannibalism.However, the supplementation experiment did not affect the prevalence or magnitude of brood reduction during the days of food supply.The experiment only manipulated food availability for a few days during the period when most sibling cannibalism and brood reduction occurs.Moreover, the daily added crickets were consumed rapidly,and thus manipulation may not be strong enough to induce females to misinterpret environmental conditions to adjust brood size.In accordance with this possibility, hatching date,which is commonly used as a proxy of environmental conditions in general and of food availability in particular (De Neve et al., 2004; Sorci et al., 1997), showed a negative relationship with brood reduction intensity, but not with sibling cannibalism.This evidence suggests that the smallest nestlings subject to brood reduction would die anyway, and that only in the case of food scarcity would they be consumed by older siblings.

    The cannibalized nestlings were mainly consumed by siblings, but also occasionally by the adult females and males.As cannibalizing results in death, not surprisingly, the intensity of cannibalism was strongly positively related to the intensity of brood reduction.In his pioneering work (1954), David Lack noted that inter- and intra-specific variations in clutch size and hatching asynchrony are adaptive, allowing birds to adjust brood size to the availability of resources at the time of peak nestling demand.Accordingly, brood reduction resulted in a positive relationship with clutch size and hatching span.In addition, large and asynchronous clutches may also function as insurance against hatching failure (Forbes, 1990, 1991;Hardy, 1992), while variation in hatching failure influences brood size and the level of brood reduction necessary to adjust the number of nestlings to environmental conditions.In agreement with this scenario, brood reduction was negatively related to the number of eggs that failed to hatch.Interestingly, clutch size did not predict hatching failure, but showed a positive relationship with intensity of cannibalism.These last two results could be interpreted as hoopoes laying extra eggs to be used as food resources after hatching, the key point of the larder hypothesis (Alexander, 1974).Producing extra eggs may be relatively cheap for hoopoes,given that females lay relatively small eggs at times of relatively high resource availability, and when they are additionally fed by their males (Díaz Lora et al., 2020).The relatively high proportion of clutches with more than six eggs,in comparison to the rare occurrence of broods with more than six fledglings in Palearctic hoopoes (Arlettaz et al., 2000;Fournier & Arlettaz, 2001; Martín-Vivaldi et al., 1999; Rieder &Schulze, 2010), could be at least partially explained by investment in the last eggs as a food cache.However, our data cannot distinguish this interpretation from the possibility that hoopoes are just using condemned-to-die nestlings as a food resource.

    In cases of reduced hatching failure, the cannibalized extra nestlings may be the consequence of surplus hatchlings from“insurance” eggs.In accordance, we found that lower hatching failure was associated with a greater number of cannibalized nestlings.Thus, it is possible that hoopoe sibling cannibalism may just be the consequence of taking advantage of resources that were adaptively employed but not used as“insurance”.However, this interpretation assumes that, at the population level, clutch size should be adjusted to the expected hatching failure level, which is not supported in the Spanish population, nor when comparing clutch size and hatching failure in hoopoes breeding in Austria and Spain.Since Spanish hoopoes laid similar clutch sizes as Austrian hoopoes, but hatching failures were more frequent in the latter, selection pressures for clutch size adjustment to environmental conditions (Sanz & Moreno, 1995) should have reduced clutch size in the Spanish population, but this did not occur.Considering that some extra-nestlings are consumed by older offspring, it is possible that selection for a smaller brood size in Spain is partially counteracted by the advantages of eating last hatchlings.

    Whatever the evolutionary explanation, the most parsimonious argument for cannibalistic behavior in hoopoes is that they take advantage of nestlings that are condemned to die, because older nestlings can consume their smallest siblings.Contrary to what has been assumed in the literature,our results showed that cannibalism of last-hatched nestlings occurs frequently in hoopoes.It may also occur at similar rates in species with similar life history characteristics or in phylogenetically close species, such as the Bucerotiformes,for which anecdotic evidence of sibling cannibalism exists(Chan et al., 2007; Engelbrecht, 2013; Ng et al., 2011).Our findings may encourage research on this behavior in other bird species, while providing the opportunity to test exclusive predictions of the larder hypothesis in hoopoes.Experimental manipulations of food availability during the laying stage,together with removing or adding last-hatched nestlings to hoopoe nests, may serve this purpose.If extra eggs are laid as food caches, food-supplemented nests should contain larger clutches, while cannibalizing experimentally added nestlings should influence fledging success.

    DATA AVAlLABlLlTY

    Data used in this paper can be found at https://figshare.com/s/55775bd4f5e31a464fd2.

    SUPPLEMENTARY DATA

    Supplementary data to this article can be found online.

    COMPETlNG lNTERESTS

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    AUTHORS’ CONTRlBUTlONS

    M.M.-V.detected the first recorded evidence of cannibalism in Spain in 2017 and, together with J.J.S., designed the study of the Spanish population, including the experimental approach in 2019.J.J.S., M.M.-V., M.M.-A., C.R.-C., and E.M.-R.carried out fieldwork in the Spanish population and visualized videos.J.J.S.performed statistical analyses and wrote a first version of the manuscript with great help from M.M.-V., S.N., and H.H.S.N.designed the study of the Austrian population and,together with M.E.and J.S., performed the fieldwork and wrote a first draft based on results from the Austrian population.All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    We thank Carmen Zamora, Manuel Soler Cruz, Juan Moreno Klemming, Anders Pape M?ller, and Graham Tebb for discussion and comments on a preliminary version of the manuscript.Alberto Ruiz Moreno and Enrique Cortés Sánchez helped with the recording equipment.Natalia Juárez García Pelayo and Lola Barón helped with fieldwork.The Spanish research group was supported by the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades and European (FEDER)funds (CGL2017-83 103-P), and from facilities, including an apartment, provided by the city authorities of Guadix, where a small lab to process samples quickly was installed.Finally, we would like to thank Renate Hengsberger for revising earlier drafts.

    91精品三级在线观看| 97精品久久久久久久久久精品| 国产亚洲欧美精品永久| 国产精品香港三级国产av潘金莲 | 亚洲中文av在线| 午夜激情av网站| 日韩三级伦理在线观看| 寂寞人妻少妇视频99o| 丁香六月天网| 婷婷色综合www| 亚洲精品久久久久久婷婷小说| 一区二区日韩欧美中文字幕| 一级黄片播放器| 精品少妇内射三级| 亚洲成国产人片在线观看| 精品人妻熟女毛片av久久网站| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 九九爱精品视频在线观看| 秋霞伦理黄片| 9热在线视频观看99| 青青草视频在线视频观看| 婷婷色麻豆天堂久久| 99国产精品免费福利视频| 曰老女人黄片| 亚洲 欧美一区二区三区| 一级片'在线观看视频| 国产福利在线免费观看视频| 亚洲伊人色综图| 99九九在线精品视频| 国产麻豆69| 欧美日本中文国产一区发布| 韩国av在线不卡| 久久综合国产亚洲精品| 男人操女人黄网站| 日韩电影二区| 丝袜脚勾引网站| 亚洲欧洲精品一区二区精品久久久 | 国产成人精品久久久久久| 日韩制服丝袜自拍偷拍| 免费观看性生交大片5| 久久久久国产网址| 欧美日韩av久久| 日韩av不卡免费在线播放| 少妇的逼水好多| 中文乱码字字幕精品一区二区三区| 婷婷色麻豆天堂久久| 成年女人在线观看亚洲视频| 国产免费一区二区三区四区乱码| 又粗又硬又长又爽又黄的视频| 欧美亚洲 丝袜 人妻 在线| 亚洲成av片中文字幕在线观看 | 蜜桃在线观看..| 女人高潮潮喷娇喘18禁视频| 亚洲婷婷狠狠爱综合网| 日韩人妻精品一区2区三区| 丁香六月天网| 99久久中文字幕三级久久日本| 亚洲国产精品999| 欧美成人午夜精品| 国产一区二区在线观看av| 精品少妇黑人巨大在线播放| 亚洲成人一二三区av| 天堂8中文在线网| 国产免费福利视频在线观看| 久久这里有精品视频免费| 午夜91福利影院| 亚洲国产av影院在线观看| 成人毛片60女人毛片免费| 亚洲国产毛片av蜜桃av| a级毛片黄视频| 国产人伦9x9x在线观看 | 成人毛片a级毛片在线播放| 欧美黄色片欧美黄色片| 777久久人妻少妇嫩草av网站| 精品国产一区二区久久| 岛国毛片在线播放| 在线观看一区二区三区激情| 国产欧美日韩综合在线一区二区| 国产精品熟女久久久久浪| 男女边摸边吃奶| 成年女人在线观看亚洲视频| 精品视频人人做人人爽| 久久精品夜色国产| 亚洲av.av天堂| 亚洲国产欧美在线一区| 爱豆传媒免费全集在线观看| 观看美女的网站| 一二三四中文在线观看免费高清| 热99国产精品久久久久久7| 国产片特级美女逼逼视频| 国产精品免费视频内射| 久久国内精品自在自线图片| 在线观看美女被高潮喷水网站| 少妇猛男粗大的猛烈进出视频| 在线观看免费高清a一片| videossex国产| 久久久久精品性色| 女人高潮潮喷娇喘18禁视频| 欧美黄色片欧美黄色片| 亚洲第一av免费看| h视频一区二区三区| 久久久久久久久久久免费av| 国产极品天堂在线| 久久97久久精品| 18禁动态无遮挡网站| www.av在线官网国产| 欧美bdsm另类| 亚洲欧美清纯卡通| 80岁老熟妇乱子伦牲交| 波多野结衣一区麻豆| 激情五月婷婷亚洲| 三级国产精品片| 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 街头女战士在线观看网站| 亚洲情色 制服丝袜| 欧美日韩av久久| 女人高潮潮喷娇喘18禁视频| 边亲边吃奶的免费视频| 欧美日韩视频高清一区二区三区二| 久久久久久免费高清国产稀缺| 久久久久精品人妻al黑| 日韩精品有码人妻一区| 99久国产av精品国产电影| 精品国产一区二区三区四区第35| av在线观看视频网站免费| 久久综合国产亚洲精品| 99久久人妻综合| 美女脱内裤让男人舔精品视频| 亚洲精品乱久久久久久| 少妇熟女欧美另类| 国产白丝娇喘喷水9色精品| 亚洲国产av新网站| 国产精品99久久99久久久不卡 | 亚洲精品国产色婷婷电影| 中国国产av一级| 超碰97精品在线观看| 国产精品国产av在线观看| 大香蕉久久成人网| 久久午夜福利片| 一个人免费看片子| 亚洲人成电影观看| 99久久综合免费| 日日啪夜夜爽| 丝袜人妻中文字幕| 国产精品 欧美亚洲| 一边摸一边做爽爽视频免费| 亚洲成av片中文字幕在线观看 | 亚洲精品国产av成人精品| 久久久国产欧美日韩av| 18在线观看网站| xxxhd国产人妻xxx| 亚洲欧美色中文字幕在线| 伊人亚洲综合成人网| 国产精品一区二区在线不卡| 丝瓜视频免费看黄片| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看 | 久久这里有精品视频免费| 2022亚洲国产成人精品| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 午夜福利一区二区在线看| 丝袜人妻中文字幕| 9191精品国产免费久久| 免费少妇av软件| 天天操日日干夜夜撸| 久久ye,这里只有精品| 亚洲av综合色区一区| 性高湖久久久久久久久免费观看| 一级毛片电影观看| 久久精品国产亚洲av天美| 欧美日韩一区二区视频在线观看视频在线| 精品一区在线观看国产| 狠狠精品人妻久久久久久综合| 女人久久www免费人成看片| 十分钟在线观看高清视频www| 男人爽女人下面视频在线观看| 日韩精品免费视频一区二区三区| 亚洲熟女精品中文字幕| 亚洲婷婷狠狠爱综合网| 国产成人午夜福利电影在线观看| 99久久中文字幕三级久久日本| 国产精品久久久久久精品电影小说| 欧美激情 高清一区二区三区| 国产极品粉嫩免费观看在线| 水蜜桃什么品种好| 人人妻人人澡人人爽人人夜夜| 精品午夜福利在线看| 少妇熟女欧美另类| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在| 亚洲欧洲精品一区二区精品久久久 | 在线 av 中文字幕| www.av在线官网国产| 可以免费在线观看a视频的电影网站 | 一边摸一边做爽爽视频免费| 肉色欧美久久久久久久蜜桃| 校园人妻丝袜中文字幕| 国产淫语在线视频| 亚洲欧美成人综合另类久久久| 国产精品熟女久久久久浪| 日韩在线高清观看一区二区三区| 国产97色在线日韩免费| 婷婷色av中文字幕| 欧美精品一区二区大全| 亚洲成人手机| 久久亚洲国产成人精品v| 亚洲欧美成人综合另类久久久| a 毛片基地| 国产毛片在线视频| 午夜激情久久久久久久| 成年av动漫网址| 天堂中文最新版在线下载| 国产精品一区二区在线不卡| 国产xxxxx性猛交| 国产精品国产三级专区第一集| 亚洲成人手机| 久久久欧美国产精品| 视频区图区小说| 国产精品久久久久成人av| 久久精品aⅴ一区二区三区四区 | 中文字幕精品免费在线观看视频| 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看| 国产亚洲午夜精品一区二区久久| 亚洲少妇的诱惑av| 成人黄色视频免费在线看| 最近手机中文字幕大全| 国产在线免费精品| 国产高清国产精品国产三级| 亚洲欧美色中文字幕在线| 香蕉国产在线看| 亚洲精品国产av成人精品| 人人妻人人爽人人添夜夜欢视频| 国产亚洲午夜精品一区二区久久| 日本91视频免费播放| 超色免费av| 中文字幕精品免费在线观看视频| 久久精品人人爽人人爽视色| 人人澡人人妻人| 丝袜喷水一区| 日韩伦理黄色片| 亚洲国产精品成人久久小说| 久久国产精品大桥未久av| 男女国产视频网站| 精品国产一区二区三区四区第35| 七月丁香在线播放| 欧美日韩亚洲高清精品| 18在线观看网站| av卡一久久| 热99久久久久精品小说推荐| 精品久久蜜臀av无| 久久精品国产a三级三级三级| 日韩av在线免费看完整版不卡| av卡一久久| 熟女av电影| 国产精品亚洲av一区麻豆 | 亚洲在久久综合| 在线观看三级黄色| 国产乱人偷精品视频| 这个男人来自地球电影免费观看 | 男女午夜视频在线观看| 五月伊人婷婷丁香| 国产精品国产三级国产专区5o| 丝袜美腿诱惑在线| 国产成人免费无遮挡视频| 少妇被粗大的猛进出69影院| 精品午夜福利在线看| 熟妇人妻不卡中文字幕| 一区二区三区精品91| 欧美人与性动交α欧美软件| 久久久国产精品麻豆| 成人影院久久| 国产精品香港三级国产av潘金莲 | 中文欧美无线码| 2021少妇久久久久久久久久久| 不卡视频在线观看欧美| 中国国产av一级| 国产色婷婷99| 观看av在线不卡| 黄色视频在线播放观看不卡| 黄频高清免费视频| 国产免费一区二区三区四区乱码| 亚洲中文av在线| 久久ye,这里只有精品| 日本欧美视频一区| 99久久中文字幕三级久久日本| 国产精品蜜桃在线观看| 国产精品无大码| 曰老女人黄片| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 一本久久精品| 久久亚洲国产成人精品v| 2018国产大陆天天弄谢| 欧美av亚洲av综合av国产av | 人人妻人人添人人爽欧美一区卜| 国产淫语在线视频| 啦啦啦在线观看免费高清www| 国产爽快片一区二区三区| 最新的欧美精品一区二区| 我要看黄色一级片免费的| 国产在线免费精品| 亚洲色图综合在线观看| 欧美+日韩+精品| 国产激情久久老熟女| 伊人久久国产一区二区| 汤姆久久久久久久影院中文字幕| 中文乱码字字幕精品一区二区三区| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 国产av国产精品国产| 国产不卡av网站在线观看| 亚洲成国产人片在线观看| 三上悠亚av全集在线观看| 亚洲人成电影观看| 国产精品国产三级国产专区5o| 欧美精品一区二区大全| 国产片特级美女逼逼视频| 香蕉丝袜av| 黄色毛片三级朝国网站| 美女高潮到喷水免费观看| 久久久久人妻精品一区果冻| 成人午夜精彩视频在线观看| 国产精品免费大片| 精品一区二区三卡| 在线观看三级黄色| 亚洲国产看品久久| 在线观看三级黄色| 亚洲国产看品久久| 国产激情久久老熟女| 天天影视国产精品| 可以免费在线观看a视频的电影网站 | 又大又黄又爽视频免费| av线在线观看网站| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验| 国产精品一区二区在线观看99| 国产男人的电影天堂91| 免费日韩欧美在线观看| 80岁老熟妇乱子伦牲交| 日韩人妻精品一区2区三区| 亚洲国产av影院在线观看| 人人澡人人妻人| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 精品亚洲成国产av| 大香蕉久久网| 在线观看免费日韩欧美大片| 一级片'在线观看视频| videossex国产| 18禁观看日本| 人妻一区二区av| 日韩人妻精品一区2区三区| 中文字幕色久视频| 男男h啪啪无遮挡| 国产一级毛片在线| 欧美精品高潮呻吟av久久| 日韩熟女老妇一区二区性免费视频| 黄片无遮挡物在线观看| 久久热在线av| 人妻系列 视频| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看| 亚洲av欧美aⅴ国产| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 一级爰片在线观看| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看| 最近最新中文字幕免费大全7| 好男人视频免费观看在线| 男人舔女人的私密视频| 黄色配什么色好看| 五月伊人婷婷丁香| 黄色毛片三级朝国网站| 久久久久久人妻| 国产精品.久久久| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| xxxhd国产人妻xxx| 纯流量卡能插随身wifi吗| 欧美日本中文国产一区发布| 欧美日韩视频精品一区| 青青草视频在线视频观看| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 亚洲第一青青草原| 在线观看免费日韩欧美大片| 国产综合精华液| 国产精品久久久久久精品古装| 久久av网站| 国产精品无大码| 亚洲精品av麻豆狂野| 久久久久久久国产电影| 亚洲成色77777| 日韩一卡2卡3卡4卡2021年| 热re99久久国产66热| 久久久久久久国产电影| 人人妻人人澡人人爽人人夜夜| 亚洲综合色惰| 又黄又粗又硬又大视频| 男人添女人高潮全过程视频| 亚洲一码二码三码区别大吗| 精品久久蜜臀av无| 国产精品久久久久久精品电影小说| 精品人妻在线不人妻| 亚洲av中文av极速乱| av天堂久久9| 国产无遮挡羞羞视频在线观看| 久久久久精品人妻al黑| av在线观看视频网站免费| 日韩成人av中文字幕在线观看| 999久久久国产精品视频| 国产探花极品一区二区| 制服诱惑二区| 午夜免费鲁丝| 麻豆精品久久久久久蜜桃| 视频在线观看一区二区三区| 毛片一级片免费看久久久久| 一区二区日韩欧美中文字幕| 欧美人与性动交α欧美精品济南到 | 久久综合国产亚洲精品| 亚洲国产看品久久| 又大又黄又爽视频免费| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 免费高清在线观看日韩| 高清不卡的av网站| 国产精品香港三级国产av潘金莲 | 欧美日本中文国产一区发布| 精品国产一区二区三区四区第35| 国产免费现黄频在线看| 免费av中文字幕在线| 一级爰片在线观看| 日韩一卡2卡3卡4卡2021年| 狠狠精品人妻久久久久久综合| 青草久久国产| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 久久99一区二区三区| videossex国产| 一本一本久久a久久精品综合妖精 国产伦在线观看视频一区 | 一本大道久久a久久精品| 成人亚洲精品一区在线观看| 黑人欧美特级aaaaaa片| 亚洲人成网站在线观看播放| 久久久久视频综合| 久久精品国产鲁丝片午夜精品| 久久精品久久久久久噜噜老黄| 欧美 日韩 精品 国产| 91aial.com中文字幕在线观看| 青春草国产在线视频| 老鸭窝网址在线观看| 亚洲av免费高清在线观看| 国产有黄有色有爽视频| 天天躁日日躁夜夜躁夜夜| 波野结衣二区三区在线| 青春草亚洲视频在线观看| 一个人免费看片子| 91aial.com中文字幕在线观看| 免费日韩欧美在线观看| 99国产综合亚洲精品| 美国免费a级毛片| 热re99久久国产66热| 午夜福利视频在线观看免费| 最黄视频免费看| 秋霞在线观看毛片| 91午夜精品亚洲一区二区三区| 亚洲av免费高清在线观看| 99国产综合亚洲精品| 9191精品国产免费久久| 久久久国产精品麻豆| 男男h啪啪无遮挡| 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区| av在线播放精品| 春色校园在线视频观看| 王馨瑶露胸无遮挡在线观看| 成人免费观看视频高清| 久久国产亚洲av麻豆专区| 伦理电影大哥的女人| 精品卡一卡二卡四卡免费| 成人免费观看视频高清| 久久午夜福利片| 90打野战视频偷拍视频| 毛片一级片免费看久久久久| 90打野战视频偷拍视频| 久久99热这里只频精品6学生| 一本一本久久a久久精品综合妖精 国产伦在线观看视频一区 | 国产日韩一区二区三区精品不卡| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱| 欧美国产精品一级二级三级| 热99久久久久精品小说推荐| 亚洲av电影在线观看一区二区三区| kizo精华| 亚洲欧洲日产国产| 美女中出高潮动态图| 国产亚洲欧美精品永久| 天天躁狠狠躁夜夜躁狠狠躁| 亚洲成国产人片在线观看| 日日啪夜夜爽| 日韩一卡2卡3卡4卡2021年| 黑人巨大精品欧美一区二区蜜桃| 亚洲美女黄色视频免费看| 少妇人妻精品综合一区二区| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 日本欧美国产在线视频| 狠狠婷婷综合久久久久久88av| 亚洲成人手机| 欧美最新免费一区二区三区| 久久韩国三级中文字幕| 久久鲁丝午夜福利片| 精品少妇久久久久久888优播| 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 黄色怎么调成土黄色| 国产毛片在线视频| 青青草视频在线视频观看| 高清视频免费观看一区二区| 久久午夜综合久久蜜桃| 另类亚洲欧美激情| 久久ye,这里只有精品| 国产不卡av网站在线观看| 制服人妻中文乱码| kizo精华| 我的亚洲天堂| 国产欧美亚洲国产| 午夜福利视频精品| 亚洲成色77777| 女性被躁到高潮视频| 制服诱惑二区| 国产色婷婷99| 日本av手机在线免费观看| 国产熟女欧美一区二区| 久久人人爽人人片av| 99香蕉大伊视频| 国产成人精品一,二区| 久久影院123| 午夜福利在线免费观看网站| 亚洲五月色婷婷综合| 国产国语露脸激情在线看| 秋霞伦理黄片| 亚洲成人手机| 一级片免费观看大全| 性色av一级| 亚洲熟女精品中文字幕| 国产 精品1| 久久久国产欧美日韩av| 最近最新中文字幕免费大全7| 免费少妇av软件| 国产精品欧美亚洲77777| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看| 欧美精品av麻豆av| 亚洲视频免费观看视频| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图| 亚洲中文av在线| 高清欧美精品videossex| 国产女主播在线喷水免费视频网站| 亚洲精品日本国产第一区| 美女大奶头黄色视频| 中文字幕精品免费在线观看视频| 久久鲁丝午夜福利片| 欧美日韩国产mv在线观看视频| 久久精品国产亚洲av高清一级| 国产精品免费视频内射| 中文欧美无线码| 另类精品久久| 欧美精品一区二区免费开放| av线在线观看网站| 亚洲欧美成人综合另类久久久| 中文字幕亚洲精品专区| 最近最新中文字幕免费大全7| 欧美97在线视频| 国产精品 欧美亚洲| 少妇被粗大的猛进出69影院| 免费av中文字幕在线| 国产午夜精品一二区理论片| 国产高清国产精品国产三级| 在线精品无人区一区二区三| 久久韩国三级中文字幕| 精品国产乱码久久久久久男人| 亚洲精品日本国产第一区| 精品福利永久在线观看| 精品少妇一区二区三区视频日本电影 | 国产精品女同一区二区软件| 国产精品国产av在线观看| 国产成人av激情在线播放| 90打野战视频偷拍视频| 又黄又粗又硬又大视频| 1024视频免费在线观看| 精品国产超薄肉色丝袜足j| 日本色播在线视频| 精品亚洲乱码少妇综合久久| 自拍欧美九色日韩亚洲蝌蚪91| 亚洲第一av免费看| 欧美精品av麻豆av| 黄色配什么色好看| 中文字幕精品免费在线观看视频| 午夜日本视频在线| 久久久久久人人人人人| 精品久久久久久电影网| 天天躁狠狠躁夜夜躁狠狠躁| 欧美精品av麻豆av| 久久久久久久久久久免费av| 欧美国产精品va在线观看不卡| av网站免费在线观看视频| 九色亚洲精品在线播放| 超色免费av| 亚洲av在线观看美女高潮| 熟女少妇亚洲综合色aaa.| 日韩在线高清观看一区二区三区| 婷婷色综合大香蕉| 亚洲婷婷狠狠爱综合网| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 美女主播在线视频| 在线观看人妻少妇| 两个人看的免费小视频| 亚洲精品美女久久av网站| 国产精品久久久久成人av| 亚洲精品美女久久久久99蜜臀 | 搡老乐熟女国产| 少妇的逼水好多| 亚洲情色 制服丝袜| 叶爱在线成人免费视频播放| 97精品久久久久久久久久精品| 制服诱惑二区|