• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Effects of biscuit-type feeding supplementation on the neurocognitive outcomes of HIV-affected school-age children: a randomized,double-blind, controlled intervention trial in Kenya

    2018-01-08 09:25:09KekKheeLooShemraRizzoQiaolinChenRobertWeissCatherineSugarGraceEttyangJudithErnstGoleenSamariCharlotteNeumann
    Family Medicine and Community Health 2017年4期

    Kek Khee Loo, Shemra Rizzo, Qiaolin Chen, Robert E. Weiss, Catherine A. Sugar,5, Grace Ettyang, Judith Ernst,Goleen Samari, Charlotte G. Neumann

    1. Kaiser Permanente Los Angeles Medical Center, Department of Pediatrics,1526 Edgemont Street, 5th Floor, Los Angeles, CA, USA

    2. Department of Statistics, University of California, Riverside, 1438 Olmsted Hall, 900 University Avenue, Riverside,CA, USA

    3. Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corporation, One Health Plaza, 337/A03.3C,East Hanover, NJ, USA

    4. Department of Biostatistics, UCLA Fielding School of Public Health, 650 Charles Young Drive South, Los Angeles, CA, USA

    5. Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA,USA

    6. School of Public Health, Moi University, P.O. Box 4606, Eldoret, Kenya

    7. Department of Nutrition and Dietetics, Indiana University School of Health and Rehabilitation, Coleman Hall, CF 224, 1140 W. Michigan Street, Indianapolis, IN, USA

    8. Population Research Center, University of Texas at Austin, 305 E. 23rd Street, Stop G1800, Austin, TX, USA

    9. Department of Community Health Sciences, UCLA Fielding School of Public Health, 650 Charles Young Drive South, Los Angeles, CA, USA

    Effects of biscuit-type feeding supplementation on the neurocognitive outcomes of HIV-affected school-age children: a randomized,double-blind, controlled intervention trial in Kenya

    Kek Khee Loo1, Shemra Rizzo2, Qiaolin Chen3, Robert E. Weiss4, Catherine A. Sugar4,5, Grace Ettyang6, Judith Ernst7,Goleen Samari8, Charlotte G. Neumann9

    1. Kaiser Permanente Los Angeles Medical Center, Department of Pediatrics,1526 Edgemont Street, 5th Floor, Los Angeles, CA, USA

    2. Department of Statistics, University of California, Riverside, 1438 Olmsted Hall, 900 University Avenue, Riverside,CA, USA

    3. Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corporation, One Health Plaza, 337/A03.3C,East Hanover, NJ, USA

    4. Department of Biostatistics, UCLA Fielding School of Public Health, 650 Charles Young Drive South, Los Angeles, CA, USA

    5. Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA,USA

    6. School of Public Health, Moi University, P.O. Box 4606, Eldoret, Kenya

    7. Department of Nutrition and Dietetics, Indiana University School of Health and Rehabilitation, Coleman Hall, CF 224, 1140 W. Michigan Street, Indianapolis, IN, USA

    8. Population Research Center, University of Texas at Austin, 305 E. 23rd Street, Stop G1800, Austin, TX, USA

    9. Department of Community Health Sciences, UCLA Fielding School of Public Health, 650 Charles Young Drive South, Los Angeles, CA, USA

    Objective:To determine if meat or soy protein dietary supplementation will enhance the neurocognitive performance of HIV-affected children at-risk of malnutrition and food insecurity.Methods:A randomized, double-blind, controlled intervention trial evaluated the effect of nutritional supplementation on the neurocognitive outcomes of 49 HIV-affected school-age children in western Kenya. The intervention consisted in providing the mother, target child, and siblings with one of three isocaloric biscuit-type supplements — soy, wheat, or beef — on 5 days per week for 18 months. Neurocognitive outcomes of the target children were assessed by a battery of eight measures and followed up longitudinally for up to 24 months.Results:Mixed effects modeling demonstrated significant differences in the rates of increase over time among all three groups (Ftest degrees of freedom of 2,P<0.05) for Raven’s progressive matrices performance, but not for verbal meaning, arithmetic, digit span backward, forward, and total, embedded figure test, and Beery visual—motor integration scores.Conclusion:HIV-affected school-age children provided with soy protein supplementation showed greater improvement in nonverbal cognitive ( fluid intelligence) performance compared with peers who received isocaloric beef or wheat biscuits. Soy nutrients may have an enhancing effect on neurocognitive skills in HIV-affected school-age children.

    Neurocognition; neurodevelopment; malnutrition; HIV; nutrition; Africa; animal source foods; meat; soy; proteins

    Introduction

    Mothers with human immunodeficiency virus(HIV) infection and their offspring, HIV positive and HIV negative, are among those most susceptible to the effects of malnutrition and disease [1,2]. Most HIV-affected families live in impoverished areas of the world, where an adverse spiral of food insecurity, poor dietary quality,opportunistic infections, lethargy, and encephalopathy can impact both the caregivers’ and the child’s health, as well as deplete caregivers’abilities to cope with and attend to the child’s developmental care, ultimately affecting the neurocognitive outcomes of the child [3—5].

    While it is known that pediatric HIV infection is associated with a range of cognitive problems, and brain alterations are detectable even in the early postnatal period [6—8], children and youths who are not infected by vertical transmission (but whose families are affected by HIV) are nonetheless shown to have cognitive and school achievement deficits compared with same-age peers without HIV exposure [9, 10]. Children living in less economically developed areas are particularly at risk. A literature review indicated that in comparison with resource-rich settings, in resource-poor settings infants and young children born to HIV-positive mothers demonstrated greater neurodevelopmental delay compared with HIV-unexposed counterparts, but data on older children remain lacking [11].

    In sub-Saharan Africa, where maternal HIV infection remains pervasive, malnutrition further undermines the neurodevelopmental status of children in HIV-affected families[12]. In this geographic terrain, the diet is mainly plant based,low in animal-source foods (ASFs), low in energy density,and deficient in vitamin B12, and contains high amounts of phytate and fiber that reduce the bioavailability of micronutrients such as iron, calcium, and zinc [13, 14]. Iron deficiency is the most prevalent micronutrient deficiency worldwide, and leads to microcytic anemia, lethargy, and impaired cognitive,immune, and endocrine function [15]. Intestinal parasites and high-phytate diets also contribute to iron deficiency. Zinc deficiency is associated with immune dysfunction, diarrhea, and acute respiratory infections [16, 17]. Vitamin B12deficiency is classically associated with pernicious anemia, but on a global scale, deficiency due to food-bound cobalamin malabsorption is more common [18]. Severe vitamin B12deficiency results in neurologic deficits, impairs brain development [19], and adversely impacts school performance [20, 21].

    ASFs can provide a variety of micronutrients that are deficient and poorly absorbed from plant-source diets, especially vitamin B12[22]. There is evidence that relatively small amounts of ASFs, added to a vegetarian diet, can substantially increase nutrient adequacy, particularly of bioavailable iron and zinc [22]. School breakfast programs have been associated with improved academic performance [23, 24]. The Child Nutrition Project (CNP) feeding intervention study provided evidence on the impact of an ASF nutrition intervention on learning and cognitive development outcomes in Kenyan school-age children. This randomized study provided three types of isocaloric school snacks 5 days a week to schoolchildren in rural Kenya (plaingitheri(a vegetable porridge) plus added oil,githeriplus milk, orgitheriplus meat) and had a control (no feeding) group. Children in the CNP study in the meat group significantly outperformed their counterparts in the milk, energy, and control groups on Raven’s progressive matrices (RPM) [25]. The CNP feeding intervention also affected end-of-term school test scores over time. The meat group children showed greater improvements compared with the control group children in English, Kiembu, Kiswahili,geography, and arithmetic test scores [26]. Intake of riboflavin, available iron, iron, energy per body weight, zinc, and vitamin B12were positively associated with the changes in the school test scores [27]. Greater gains in RPM scores were also associated with intake of available iron. However, a soy porridge was not included in this intervention trial.

    Soy is a rich source of protein, and includes all essential amino acids. It is also replete with isoflavones, which constitute one class of phytoestrogens found largely in soybeans and soy-derived products, with the aglycone genistein and daidzein being the most potent of the isoflavones present in soy foods [28]. Isoflavones from soy foods are more bioavailable in children than in adults [29], and at concentrations present in soy infant formula inhibit rotavirus infectionin vitro[30].However, soy-protein products contain appreciable amounts of phytate and fiber, which inhibit iron absorption. Soy protein itself is still relatively inhibitory to iron absorption, even with phytate removal [31].

    The wheat biscuit was developed to be equal in energy to the soy and meat biscuits, to serve as the comparison product in this randomized controlled trial. The wheat biscuit was lower in protein content and quality, and the source of protein was solely derived from the wheat flour ingredient, whereas the meat and soy biscuits had dried beef powder or soy flour respectively added to the basic recipe made of wheat flour.Caloric supplementation without the addition of protein did not improve children’s cognitive performance in the aforementioned CNP study [25].

    Previous studies have demonstrated positive effects of soyprotein supplementation in alleviating malnutrition. Fortified blended flours, such as corn—soy blend (CSB), prepared as porridge are the most widely used foods in supplementary feeding programs [32]. In children with moderate acute malnutrition,however, recovery rates remain below 75%, lower than the rate achieved with peanut-paste-based ready-to-use supplementary foods (RUSFs) [33]. Hence, concerns about the nutritional adequacy of these blended foods and problems in home preparation of CSB (e.g., overdilution of the porridge or inadequate boiling of water) have led to the development of fortified CSBs. In a prospective, randomized, investigator-blinded,controlled noninferiority trial involving rural Malawian children aged 6—59 months with moderate acute malnutrition,a novel, locally produced, fortified blended flour (CSB++)that included dry skim milk and that was more energy dense proved to be not inferior to a locally produced soy RUSF and an imported soy/whey RUSF in facilitating recovery from moderate acute malnutrition [34]. A randomized study of 81 children with moderate acute malnutrition in Cameroon showed comparable recovery rates in children who were treated with an improved CSB (CSB+) versus RUSF [35]. A school feeding program involving 383 schoolchildren in Ghana demonstrated that the use of a multiple-micronutrient-fortified CSB was a key contributor to micronutrient adequacy [36].

    Soybean-enhanced supplemental snacks may also provide an appropriate, affordable, and sustainable solution to the problem of malnutrition, since they are is energy dense and can be made with locally available foods in low-income countries in sub-Saharan Africa [37, 38]. Biscuit-based soy supplementation has been found to be accepted by pregnant women [39], but its efficacy in this form has not been assessed in children. In HIV-affected families, the ‘biscuit model’ confers further advantages in that it can be provided in a blinded and randomized fashion, safely and privately in a home under directly observed consumption by a highly stigmatized population [40].

    The current study was a three-arm randomized, doubleblind nutrition intervention trial of the effects of ASF versus soy supplementation on the neurocognitive performance of HIV-affected, nutritionally at risk school-age children in Kenya. The intervention consisted in providing one of three isocaloric biscuit-type supplements — soy, wheat, or beef — to the mother, target child, and siblings. It was hypothesized that children in the meat group would have better neurocognitive outcomes than those randomized to the soy and wheat groups.

    Materials and methods

    Study design

    This was a three-arm randomized, double-blind nutrition intervention trial with a sample of 49 school-age children born to HIV-positive drug-na?ve women in three communities in western Kenya (Turbo, Mautuma, and Soi). The uniform nutrition intervention consisted in feeding the mother and children biscuits 5 days per week for 18 months. Women,with HIV and also free from opportunistic or other serious infections, and the target children between 4 and 8 years old were randomly assigned to receive one of three isocaloric intervention biscuits that contained either beef, roasted soy flour, or wheat flour (used as the control biscuit). The study was a three-armed intervention trial to ascertain if the addition of soy and meat protein to a basic recipe of wheat will improve the cognitive performance of HIV-affected children.At enrollment, these women were not yet ill enough to warrant treatment with antiretroviral therapy (ART), and therefore it was hypothesized that a food intervention may have a positive impact on their health and delay the need for ART. Groups were matched for the distance of participant households from the treatment clinic and a government health center. The location of the households was determined by Global Positioning System mapping. The intervention spanned 18 months to ensure inclusion of all participants during all of the seasons,including the dry season from November to March, when there was the greatest vulnerability to food scarcity. Subsequently,the mother and child/children were followed up at 6 months after intervention. Participants were enrolled over a 1.5-year period (December 2008 to June 2010). Data collection began in December 2008 and ended in June 2012.

    Population

    The study population of women received medical care either at a local government health center or at the Academic Model Providing Access to Health care partnership clinics in western Kenya, supported in part by United States Agency for International Development [41, 42]. This partnership operates under the joint direction of Moi Teaching and Referral Hospital and the Moi University and Indiana University schools of medicine and cares for more than 160,000 HIV-infected adults and children at more than 500 clinical sites throughout western Kenya. Women were included if they were drug na?ve and classified as having WHO stage 1 or 2 HIV infection. At the beginning of the study, women were excluded if the initial CD4 cell count was less than 250/μL, or if the participant had one or more opportunistic infections, was pregnant, was allergic to meat, soy, or wheat, and/or did not have permission from her spouse/family to participate in the study.During the time of the study, however, the recommendations for ART initiation were liberalized in Kenya from a CD4 cell count of 200/μL or less to 350/μL or less. Therefore, the inclusion criteria regarding baseline CD4 cell count was changed from more than 250/μL to more than 400/μL. During the course of the study and after the study, it was discovered that some women received ART and/or were pregnant at the baseline, and even though they received the intervention, their data and the data from their child were excluded from the intervention effect analyses. Some target children were also found to be HIV positive and receiving ART at the baseline. The data from these children were excluded from the intervention effect analyses. HIV-positive target children who were drug na?ve were not excluded.

    Intervention food

    Details on the ‘biscuit model’ and nutritional composition of the intervention biscuits for this study have been published[40]. The research team developed the intervention foods to be isocaloric biscuits made with wheat flour. Dried beef powder or soy flour was added to the basic recipe in amounts to provide similar amounts of total protein in the beef and soy biscuits; 4.0 g protein per 100 kcal. Dried beef strips produced by Farmer’s Choice butcheries in Nairobi were processed into a powder in a Vitamix commercial blender.Soy flour was purchased solely through Nakumat supermarket in Eldoret, Kenya, and then roasted after purchase with a consistent method. Refined unfortified wheat flour manufactured in Kenya (EXE) was used as an ingredient in all of the biscuit recipes. The biscuits were prepared, packaged in opaque wrappers, weighed, labeled, and stored in a research bakery specifically designed with standardized mixing,weighing, and baking and storage equipment that allowed the production of a reliable, safe, and reproducible product.The production bakery was set up and operated by research project staff specifically trained in quantity food production,with oversight for quality control and safety by co-principal investigators and the field research project coordinator. Food preparation staff members were required to wear clean uniforms, aprons, and hair nets, and have initial and periodic medical examinations, testing for tuberculosis, and stool examinations for parasites. They were required to wash their hands and work with disposable gloves. The kitchen was inspected by the local department of public health for sanitation and cleanliness. Nutrient and bacterial analyses of the foods developed were performed in a reliable food laboratory(Covance Laboratories, Madison, Wisconsin, United States),and repeated quarterly for quality control of macronutrient,micronutrient, phytate, and fiber contents.

    Delivery of biscuits and direct observation of treatment

    Biscuits, ready for distribution, were delivered to the field twice per week. Directly observed treatment (DOT) was used to ensure adherence and to quantify intake. A DOT field worker delivered the supplement daily and returned any leftovers to a central location for quantification. The reasons for any leftovers were recorded daily for each of the participants.For young children or those children with difficulty chewing,a fixed amount of water (boiled and filtered) was added to biscuits when they were served in the home to make them into a porridge consistency. Adherence was assessed daily as part of the DOT method, whereby the individual dispensing the intervention watches the participant consume the biscuit. This modality has been reliable in the dispensing of medication for treatment of tuberculosis [43].

    Socioeconomic status measure

    The socioeconomic status (SES) score adapted for the local population was previously validated [44], and was further updated for the study population in 2013. The SES score comprises 33 binary items and three scaled items: land score, animal score, and goods score.

    Outcome measures

    There were eight measures: digit span forward, digit span backward, digit span total, RPM, verbal meaning, arithmetic,embedded figure test, and Beery test of visual—motor integration. Children were assessed every 6 months starting from the time that they were enrolled in the study.

    This battery of tests had been used in previous research in rural Kenya involving school-age children [25, 26], had been shown to be appropriate for the local cultural context,and the individual measures were demonstrated to capture multiple cognitive constructs associated with micronutrient status. Construct validity and reliability have been measured in the Kenyan population [25, 27, 45]. The cognitive testing team consisted of a supervisor and three enumerators, who were trained by a Kenyan psychologist who served as the lead field psychologist in the CNP study and is well versed in administering neurocognitive testing for both research and clinical purposes. Each enumerator was assigned a panel of children to follow up, so serial assessments were administered by the same enumerator familiar with the child and the family, in the home setting. Oversight for reliability training and quality control were further provided by a developmental pediatrician and a neuropsychologist. The cognitive team members, including training personnel, were blinded to the randomization group, as well as the history of the child and the family.

    RPM [46] is a nonverbal test of cognitive performance,abstract reasoning, and problem-solving ( fluid intelligence).Each child was presented with a matrix-like arrangement of symbols and asked to select the correct symbol to complete the pattern in multiple-choice format. No time limit was set to solve each problem. Thirty-six items were administered to each child. The verbal meaning test measures expressive language abilities and verbal skills. The test was designed in East Africa and had previously been used in Kenya, Uganda, and Tanzania [47]. The child was presented with four pictures and asked to point to the one named by the tester. Thirty-six items were administered to the children, with simpler items requiring recognition of simple nouns, while the most advanced items tested more abstract concepts. No time limit was set to identify each picture. The arithmetic test was adapted from the revised edition of the Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children [48] and assesses basic knowledge of arithmetic. The child was asked to add and subtract simple numbers and then proceed to more difficult items involving division,multiplication, and decimals. The test consisted of 19 orally presented arithmetic word problems and a time limit of 30 s to reply verbally with the answer to each question. The digit span tests are subsections of the revised edition of the Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children, and measure the child’s ability to remember and orally repeat a sequence of numbers. The test consisted of 14 items for forward and backward recall.The tester read increasingly longer strings of numbers, which the child was asked to recall and repeat in forward order for the digit span forward test, and in reverse order for the digit span backward test. Although both forward and backward digit recall are reliant on short-term concentration, each has been noted to also require different mental processes: forward recall requires basic short-term auditory memory, while backward recall invokes more complex processes involving mental transformations. Digit span scores were thus analyzed in two different ways: separately and together as a digit span total score. The Beery—Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual—Motor Integration, sixth edition [49] was used to assess the extent to which individuals can integrate their visual and motor abilities. The participant is instructed to copy 30 geometric designs in increasing order of difficulty without a time limit. The designs are scored according to the visual—motor integration manual on the basis of accuracy and quality standards. Discontinuation item is reached when the participant fails to copy three consecutive designs that can be scored.

    Ethics

    This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving human participants were approved by the institutional and ethics review boards at the University of California, Los Angeles, Indiana University, and Moi University. Informed and written consent was obtained, by specifically trained staff,from female participants and from parents on behalf of their children. Children aged 7 years or older were also given the opportunity to give consent.

    This study was part of a main study. The main study was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT00562874). CONSORT guidelines were followed in the design and reporting of this randomized controlled trial.

    Statistical analyses

    Descriptive statistics were used to summarize baseline characteristics of the study population. Frequencies were reported for categorical variables, and the mean and standard deviation were reported for continuous variables. Chi-square tests were used to test for significant differences at the baseline across the three groups, and Fisher’s exact tests were used in cases with small cell counts. Linear mixed effects models with random intercepts were used to model the covariance structures of the outcomes. Longitudinal analyses included only observations for school-age children older than 48 months.We plotted data in profile plots and histograms and tabulated summaries of covariates by treatment group and location. The data were longitudinal, with repeated measures nested within participants. Analysis generally followed methods described by Weiss [50]. Other covariates included indicators for study location (Mautuma, Soi, and Turbo), sex, baseline age, and the treatment group by time interaction. An age-squared covariate was found to be not significant, so it was not included in the final models. Participants were randomized to the three treatment groups after the baseline so no main effect for the treatment group is included to increase power. Data analysis was performed with the statistical software package Stata/SE 14.Validity of the models was confirmed by standard statistical methods. Differences in the rates of change of neurocognitive measurements by biscuit types were tested with anFtest. We consideredP<0.05 to be statistically significant.

    Results

    The flow of participants is depicted in Fig. 1. Forty-nine children were enrolled as school-age children, age 4—8 years, with 18 (37%), 20 (41%), and 11 (22%) in the soy, beef, and wheat groups respectively. Forty-four percent were male. There were 196 visits. The attrition at each stage of the study is also shown in Fig. 1. For example, at 6 months, three children in the soy group had dropped out of the biscuit intervention: one child who did not remain in the intervention after the baseline, and two children who stopped their biscuit intake after 5.5 and 3 months respectively, leaving 15 children from the baseline count of 18.

    Table 1 shows the baseline characteristics of age, sex,height, weight, BMI, head circumference, hemoglobin level and CD4 cell count, which did not differ significantly among children in the three randomized groups. Maternal characteristics are presented in Table 2. One mother had two children in the study. Maternal age, education, SES score, CD4 cell count, and hemoglobin level did not differ significantly across the three groups.

    Baseline cognitive scores are presented in Table 3. There were no significant differences among soy, beef, and wheat groups in all but one outcome. The wheat group had an average verbal meaning score lower than the other two groups at the baseline.

    Mixed effects model results

    Table 4 shows the results from the mixed effects models for the eight outcomes. Outcome variables did not differ by study location or sex. There were significant increases in the outcomes’ scores over time for all three biscuit groups, as would be expected through the developmental maturation of the children. TheFtests reveal that these increases over time were not significantly different across biscuit groups for most outcomes. However, there were significant differences between biscuit groups in the RPM scores (P=0.012), with the scores of children in the soy group being almost two times higher than those in the beef and wheat groups. In pairwise tests, there was no significant difference between the beef and wheat groups(P=0.849). A complete list of pairwise test results for all outcomes is given in Table 5.

    Discussion

    This study was a randomized, double-blind, controlled intervention trial to assess the effects of beef, soy, or wheat biscuit-type supplements on the neurocognitive outcomes of HIV-affected school-age children in Kenya. Mixed modeling analysis revealed that neurocognitive performance improved over time in all three groups. The rate of increase in nonverbal cognitive scores ( fluid intelligence) was greater for children who received soy protein compared with children provided with beef or wheat supplementation.

    Fig. 1. Numbers of children randomized to receive a biscuit with soy (S), beef (B), or wheat (W) at enrollment who remained in the study or stopped participating in the study during or after the 18-month intervention phase.

    Findings from our previous studies (see the introductory section) demonstrated improved academic and cognitive functioning among school-age children in Kenya receiving ASF supplementation [25—27]. The main differences in the interventions between previous studies and the current research were the lack of soy supplementation in the previous school feeding studies, extension of feeding supplementation to mothers and siblings, and use of the biscuit model for supplementation rather thangitheri-based mid-morning snacks.Hence, the present study offers valuable information on the effects of ASF versus soy supplementation in HIV-affected families and established the feasibility of biscuit-type supplementation using local resources.

    The child biscuit intervention provided 14 g of protein per day [51]. Protein intake from the meat and soy biscuits provided children with 64—108% of their recommended levels,in contrast to the wheat biscuit supplement, which provided a lesser amount (25—42% of the recommendation) of incomplete protein, which is also of inferior quality [52, 53]. Children in the meat and soy groups both received lysine in amounts that met 80—135% of the recommended intake, while those in the wheat group did not receive lysine from the supplement [54,55]. Vitamin B12was provided primarily to those in the meat group in an amount of 0.88 mg/day (1—8 years), representing 73—98% of the recommended intake. Higher amounts of absorbable zinc were available to those receiving meat, and themeat biscuit supplements were also lower in fiber and phytate,thus increasing bioavailability of the micronutrient. Children in the soy biscuit intervention group received greater amounts of absorbable iron because of the high amount of iron in soy flour, but the soy biscuit also contained fiber as well as phytate,with ratios with respect to iron that are known to inhibit mineral absorption. It is possible that higher iron content in the soy biscuit had a noticeable effect on children’s cognitive performance, despite the inhibitory effects of high fiber and phytate in the soy biscuits.

    Table 1. Children’s characteristics by intervention group (n=49)

    Table 2. Characteristics of mothers by intervention group (n=48)

    Table 3. Baseline cognitive scores by intervention group (n=49)

    Table 4. Mixed effects models for cognitive outcomes (n=49)

    Table 5. Pairwise differences of test scores for cognitive outcomes by biscuit group in mixed models (n=49)

    There is evidence to suggest that flavonoids, abundant in soy, have the potential to enhance human memory and neurocognitive performance through their ability to protect vulnerable neurons, enhance existing neuronal function, and stimulate neurogenesis [56]. Flavonoids may exert effects on long-term potentiation, and consequently strengthen memory and cognitive performance, through their interactions with these neuronal signaling pathways, which include the phosphatidylinositol-3 kinase—protein kinase B, protein kinase C, protein kinase A, Ca—calmodulin kinase, and mitogenactivated protein kinase mechanisms [57—59]. Isoflavones can also increase choline acetyltransferase and messenger RNA levels of neurotrophins in the hippocampus and frontal cortex[60]. Animal studies have shown that flavonoids improve visuospatial memory (or visuospatial working memory) [61, 62].Human clinical studies on neurocognitive-enhancing effects of flavonoids, however, have been mostly focused on older adults, including individuals with HIV dementia, and postmenopausal women, with little research focused on children[63—65]. After 6 weeks of soy supplementation, postmenopausal women demonstrated improved frontal lobe function,specifically in mental flexibility and planning ability, compared with women in the placebo group [28]. Neurocognitive improvements from a high-soy diet were found to extend to young adults and spatial memory of men as well [57]. A study on student volunteers who received a high-soy diet showed significant improvements in short-term memory (immediate recall of prose and 4-s delayed matching to sample of patterns) and long-term memory (picture recall after 20 min) and in mental flexibility (rule shifting and reversal) [66].

    Soy isoflavones are also implicated in immune functioning [58]. Specifically, genistein is structurally similar to 17β-estradiol, and it suppresses antigen-specific immune responsein v ivoand lymphocyte proliferation response in vitro [59]. Soy isoflavones may also adjunctively preserve neuronal functioning and sustain neurocognitive abilities of HIV-1-infected persons, by diminishing apoptotic signaling induced by the HIV-1 viral protein Tat [67]. It is thus possible that soy nutrients, including soy isoflavones, may have a role in improving the mother’s immune functioning and mental capacities, and these effects may be carried over to the developmental stimulation of the child. Siblings may also have benefitted from the feeding intervention, and older siblings may have the capacity to provide caretaker support and academic fostering as well.

    Fluid intelligence is a major component of intellect, central to executive functioning, logical reasoning, and other frontal functions [68—70]. There is some evidence from optical topography to suggest that visuospatial working memory shares a common neural system with general fluid intelligence within the lateral prefrontal cortex during the preschool years [71].Although there is a paucity of data on the effects of soy isoflavones on the cognitive functioning of children, it is possible that the improvements in spatial memory, mental flexibility,and working memory noted in adults receiving high-soy diets may similarly apply to improvement of fluid intelligence in HIV-affected school-age children provided with soy supplementation, mediated by neural pathways in the frontal and prefrontal regions.

    The strengths of this study include the randomized, double-blind controlled design; the provision of nutritional supplementation to the mother and siblings as well as the target child to study the effects in HIV-affected families; the use of detailed, longitudinal assessments based on the age of enrollment; the use of DOT to ensure adherence and to quantify intake; rigorous quality control and safety standards for intervention food production and delivery; reliability in measurement of outcome variables; and access to the networking and collaborative framework afforded by the Academic Model Providing Access to Health care partnership in western Kenya.Limitations included the relatively small sample size of 49 school-age children, which may limit the power to detect differences in the test scores across biscuits. The dropout rate was high in working with families in this resource-poor area.Families’ willingness to be followed up over time could not be ensured, and some fathers might not have wanted the mother and child to continue in the study. Some families, particularly those who dropped out within the first 6 months, were wary because of the stigma associated with HIV infection [72].Inclement weather conditions affected contact with families,as road travel to meet in person was the only means of contact without telephone or digital access. Families might not have been present at the appointed day/time (e.g., went to town for medical appointment or to run an errand), necessitating another road trip to meet the family for follow-up. If a family missed an appointment, other logistical issues had to be solved (e.g., the assessment team might not be able to meet other families as scheduled). Another limitation is that it was not possible to elucidate mechanisms by which the food supplement modulated neurocognitive and academic skills in this study. Pathways mediated by the effects of supplemental feeding for mothers and siblings as well as mother—child and child—siblings interactions could not be clarified.

    In conclusion, the results from this three-arm randomized,double-blind nutrition intervention trial of the neurocognitiveenhancing effects of ASF supplementation on HIV-affected,nutritionally at risk school-age children in Kenya indicated that nonverbal cognitive ( fluid intelligence) performance improvements in school-age children over time were greater for children who received soy protein than those who received ASFs. It is possible that soy nutrients, including soy isoflavones, may augment nonverbal cognitive skills in HIV-affected school-age children, and these effects may be mediated by the mother’s health improvement and her ability to provide developmental and educational stimulation. Further studies are needed to elucidate the neurocognitive-enhancing effects of soy biscuit intervention on HIV-affected children. Soy protein may be a good candidate for future nutrition interventions as it can be grown locally and may be less expensive than meat in poor rural areas.

    The authors honor the contributions of the late Professor Marian Sigman (UCLA), who worked with families in Kenya,and served as mentor and/or colleague to various members of the research team. The authors are grateful for the consultancy of Susan D’Sousa, a psychologist who is bilingual in Swahili and English, who joined efforts with Professor Mirian Sigman in adapting the assessment measures for the local cultural context. The authors also thank Mari Davies, Ph.D., for assisting in field work oversight and training of enumerators; Maria Pia Chaparro and Natalie Drobaugh for literature review and administration assistance. The authors are most grateful to the field staff (including enumerators and supervisors), and the endearing children and families who participated in the study.

    Conflicts of interest

    The authors declare no conflict of interest.

    Funding

    The research was supported by USAID Grant No. PCEG-00-98-00036-00; National Institutes of Health-Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development NIH-(NICHD) 1R01HD57646-01A1(CFDA #93.865), 5R01HD057646-04 “Increasing Animal Source Foods in Diets of HIV-infected Kenyan Women and Their Children”; Center for HIV Identification, Prevention,and Treatment (CHIPTS) NIMH grant P30MH058107; UCLA Center for AIDS Research (CFAR) grant 5P30AI028697,Core H; Beef Checkoff, Heifer Project International, as well as internal support from Indiana University, UCLA, and Moi University.

    Significance statement

    Research on neurocognitive functioning of children prenatally exposed to HIV, and at risk of malnutrition, has been lacking.This study employs locally-manufactured isocaloric biscuits made with wheat, soy and beef, to deliver nutritional supplementation in a standardized, quantifiable and sustainable way in poor rural areas. The main finding from this 3-arm randomized, double-blind controlled intervention trial, was that school-aged HIV-affected children provided with soy protein supplementation attained non-verbal cognitive scores that were almost two times higher than those in the beef and wheat groups. The finding that soy was advantageous to beef was unanticipated, but there is evidence from the literature that soy nutrients, including soy isoflavones, may play a role in augmenting fluid intelligence.

    1. Abubakar A, Van Baar A, Van de Vijver FJ, Holding P, Newton CR. Paediatric HIV and neurodevelopment in sub-Saharan Africa:a systematic review. Trop Med Int Health 2008;13(7):880—7.

    2. Le Doaré K, Bland R, Newell M-L. Neurodevelopment in children born to HIV-infected mothers by infection and treatment status. Pediatrics 2012;130(5):e1326—44.

    3. Antelman G, Msamanga GI, Spiegelman D, Urassa EJ, Narh R,Hunter DJ, et al. Nutritional factors and infectious disease contribute to anemia among pregnant women with human immunodeficiency virus in Tanzania. J Nutr 2000;130(8):1950—7.

    4. Reddi A, Powers MA, Thyssen A. HIV/AIDS and food insecurity: deadly syndemic or an opportunity for healthcare synergism in resource-limited settings of sub-Saharan Africa? AIDS 2012;26(1):115—7.

    5. Weiser SD, Tsai AC, Gupta R, Frongillo EA, Kawuma A, Senkungu, J, et al. Food insecurity is associated with morbidity and patterns of healthcare utilization among HIV-infected individuals in a resource-poor setting. AIDS 2012;26(1):67—75.

    6. Cohen S, ter Stege JA, Geurtsen GJ, Scherpbier HJ, Kuijpers TW,Reiss P, et al. Poorer cognitive performance in perinatally HIV-infected children versus healthy socioeconomically matched controls. Clin Infect Dis 2015;60(7):1111—9.

    7. Ragin AB, Wu Y, Gao Y, Keating S, Du H, Sammet C, et al. Brain alterations within the first 100 days of HIV infection. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 2015;2(1):12—21.

    8. Bagenda D, Nassali A, Kalyesubula I, Sherman B, Drotar D,Boivin MJ, et al. Health, neurologic, and cognitive status of HIV-infected, long-surviving, and antiretroviral-naive Ugandan children. Pediatrics 2006;117(3):729—40.

    9. Garvie PA, Zeldow B, Malee K, Nichols SL, Smith RA, Wilkins ML, et al. Discordance of cognitive and academic achievement outcomes in youth with perinatal HIV exposure. Pediatr Infect Dis J 2014;33(9):e232—8.

    10. Chase C, Ware J, Hittelman J, Blasini I, Smith R, Llorente A,et al. Early cognitive and motor development among infants born to women infected with human immunodeficiency virus. Pediatrics 2000;106(2):E25.

    11. Le Doare K, Bland R, Newell ML. Neurodevelopment in children born to HIV-infected mothers by infection and treatment status. Pediatrics 2012;130(5):e1326—44.

    12. Anabwani G, Navario P. Nutrition and HIV/AIDS in sub-Saharan Africa: an overview. Nutrition 2005;21(1):96—9.

    13. Walker AR, Walker BF, Vorster HH, Glatthaar II. Fiber, phytic acid, and mineral metabolism. Nutr Rev 1992;50(8):246—7.

    14. Bwibo NO, Neumann CG. The need for animal source foods by Kenyan children. J Nutr 2003;133(11 Suppl 2):3936S—40S.

    15. Bailey RL, West KP Jr, Black RE. The epidemiology of global micronutrient deficiencies. Ann Nutr Metab 2015;66 Suppl 2:22—33.

    16. Black MM. Zinc deficiency and child development. Am J Clin Nutr 1998;68(2 Suppl):464S—9S.

    17. Brooks WA, Yunus M, Santosham M, Wahed MA, Nahar K,Yeasmin S, et al. Zinc for severe pneumonia in very young children: double-blind placebo-controlled trial. Lancet 2004;363(9422):1683—8.

    18. Shipton MJ, Thachil J. Vitamin B12 deficiency — a 21st century perspective. Clin Med 2015;15(2):145—50.

    19. Black MM. Effects of vitamin B12 and folate deficiency on brain development in children. Food Nutr Bull 2008;29(2 Suppl):S126—31.

    20. Healton EB, Savage DG, Brust JC, Garrett TJ, Lindenbaum J.Neurologic aspects of cobalamin deficiency. Medicine (Baltimore) 1991;70(4):229—45.

    21. Rasmussen S, Fernhoff P, Scanlon K. Vitamin B12 deficiency in children and adolescents. J Pediatr 2001;138(1):10—7.

    22. Murphy SP, Gewa C, Liang LJ, Grillenberger M, Bwibo NO,Neumann CG. School snacks containing animal source foods improve dietary quality for children in rural Kenya. J Nutr 2003;133(11 Suppl 2):3950S—6S.

    23. Adolphus K, Lawton CL, Dye L. The effects of breakfast on behavior and academic performance in children and adolescents.Front Hum Neurosci 2013;7:425.

    24. Hoyland A, Dye L, Lawton CL. A systematic review of the effect of breakfast on the cognitive performance of children and adolescents. Nutr Res Rev 2009;22(2):220—43.

    25. Whaley SE, Sigman M, Neumann C, Bwibo N, Guthrie D, Weiss RE, et al. The impact of dietary intervention on the cognitive development of Kenyan school children. J Nutr 2003;133(11 Suppl 2):3965S—71S.

    26. Hulett JL, Weiss RE, Bwibo NO, Galal OM, Drorbaugh N, Neumann CG. Animal source foods have a positive impact on the primary school test scores of Kenyan schoolchildren in a cluster-randomised, controlled feeding intervention trial. Br J Nutr 2013;111(5):875—86.

    27. Gewa CA, Weiss RE, Bwibo NO, Whaley S, Sigman M, Murphy SP, et al. Dietary micronutrients are associated with higher cognitive function gains among primary school children in rural Kenya. Br J Nutr 2009;101(9):1378—87.

    28. File SE, Hartley DE, Elsabagh S, Duffy R, Wiseman H. Cognitive improvement after 6 weeks of soy supplements in postmenopausal women is limited to frontal lobe function. Menopause 2005;12(2):193—201.

    29. Halm BM, Ashburn LA, Franke AA. Isoflavones from soya foods are more bioavailable in children than adults. Br J Nutr 2007;98(5):998—1005.

    30. Andres A, Donovan SM, Kuhlenschmidt TB, Kuhlenschmidt MS. Isoflavones at concentrations present in soy infant formula inhibit rotavirus infection in vitro. J Nutr 2007;137(9):2068—73.

    31. Hurrell RF, Juillerat MA, Reddy MB, Lynch SR, Dassenko SA,Cook JD. Soy protein, phytate, and iron absorption in humans.Am J Clin Nutr 1992;56(3):573—8.

    32. Dewey KG, Adu-Afarwuah S. Systematic review of the efficacy and effectiveness of complementary feeding interventions in developing countries. Matern Child Nutr 2008;4 Suppl 1:24—85.

    33. Karakochuk C, van den Briel T, Stephens D, Zlotkin S. Treatment of moderate acute malnutrition with ready-to-use supplementary food results in higher overall recovery rates compared with a corn-soya blend in children in southern Ethiopia: an operations research trial. Am J Clin Nutr 2012;96(4):911—6.

    34. LaGrone LN, Trehan I, Meuli GJ, Wang RJ, Thakwalakwa C,Maleta K, et al. A novel fortified blended flour, corn-soy blend“plus-plus,” is not inferior to lipid-based ready-to-use supplementary foods for the treatment of moderate acute malnutrition in Malawian children. Am J Clin Nutr 2012;95(1):212—9.

    35. Medoua GN, Ntsama PM, Ndzana AC, Essa’a VJ, Tsafack JJ,Dimodi HT. Recovery rate of children with moderate acute malnutrition treated with ready-to-use supplementary food (RUSF)or improved corn-soya blend (CSB+): a randomized controlled trial. Public Health Nutr 2016;19(2):363—70.

    36. Abizari AR, Buxton C, Kwara L, Mensah-Homiah J,Armar-Klemesu M, Brouwer ID. School feeding contributes to micronutrient adequacy of Ghanaian schoolchildren. Br J Nutr 2014;112(6):1019—33.

    37. Abiodun PO. Use of soya-beans for the dietary prevention and management of malnutrition in Nigeria. Acta Paediatr Scand Suppl 1991;374:175—82.

    38. Alarcon P, Montoya R, Perez F, Dongo JW, Peerson JM, Brown KH.Clinical trial of home available, mixed diets versus a lactose-free,soy-protein formula for the dietary management of acute childhood diarrhea. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1991;12(2):224—32.

    39. Gewa CA, Frankenfeld CL, Slavin M, Omondi M. Fishenhanced and soybean-enhanced supplemental snacks are acceptable among pregnant women in rural Kenya. Food Nutr Bull 2014;35(4 Suppl):S180—7.

    40. Ernst J, Ettyang G, Neumann CG. High-nutrition biscuits to increase animal protein in diets of HIV-infected Kenyan women and their children: a study in progress. Food Nutr Bull 2014;35(4 Suppl):S198—204.

    41. Diero LO, Shaffer D, Kimaiyo S, Siika AM, Rotich JK, Smith FE,et al. Characteristics of HIV infected patients cared for at “Academic Model for the Prevention and Treatment of HIV/AIDS”clinics in western Kenya. East Afr Med J 2006;83(8):424—33.

    42. Mamlin J, Kimaiyo S, Lewis S, Tadayo H, Jerop FK, Gichunge C, et al. Integrating nutrition support for food-insecure patients and their dependents into an HIV care and treatment program in western Kenya. Am J Public Health 2009;99(2):215—21.

    43. Falzon D, Jaramillo E, Schunemann HJ, Arentz M, Bauer M, Bayona J, et al. WHO guidelines for the programmatic management of drugresistant tuberculosis: 2011 update. Eur Respir J 2011;38(3):516—28.

    44. Neumann C, McDonald MA, Sigman M, Bwibo N. Medical illness in school-age Kenyans in relation to nutrition, cognition, and playground behaviors. J Dev Behav Pediatr 1992;13(6):392—8.

    45. Daley TC, Whaley SE, Sigman MD, Espinosa MP, Neumann C.IQ on the rise: the Flynn effect in rural Kenyan children. Psychol Sci 2003;14(3):215—9.

    46. Raven TC. Raven’s progressive matrices. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1960.

    47. Sigman M, Neumann C, Baksh M, Bwibo N, McDonald MA.Relationship between nutrition and development in Kenyan toddlers. J Pediatr 1989;115(3):357—64.

    48. Weschler D. Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children — revised.New York: Psychological Corporation; 1974.

    49. Beery KE, Beery NA. Administration, scoring, and teaching manual for the Beery-VMI. 6th ed. San Antonio: Pearson; 2010.

    50. Weiss RE. Modeling longitudinal data. New York: Springer; 2005.

    51. Grillenberger M, Neumann CG, Murphy SP, Bwibo NO, van’t Veer P, Hautvast JG, et al. Food supplements have a positive impact on weight gain and the addition of animal source foods increases lean body mass of Kenyan schoolchildren. J Nutr 2003;133(11 Suppl 2):3957S—64S.

    52. Food Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, World Health Organization. Energy and protein requirements: report of a joint FAO/WHO/UNU expert consultation. Report no.9241207248. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1985.

    53. National Research Council. Recommended dietary allowances.Washington: National Academy of Sciences Press; 1989.

    54. Kurpad AV, Young VR. What is apparent is not always real: lessons from lysine requirement studies in adult humans. J Nutr 2003;133(4):1227—30.

    55. Young VR, Borgonha S. Nitrogen and amino acid requirements:the Massachusetts Institute of Technology amino acid requirement pattern. J Nutr 2000;130(7):1841s—9s.

    56. Spencer JP. Food for thought: the role of dietary flavonoids in enhancing human memory, learning and neuro-cognitive performance. Proc Nutr Soc 2008;67(2):238—52.

    57. Lee YB, Lee HJ, Sohn HS. Soy isoflavones and cognitive function. J Nutr Biochem 2005;16(11):641—9.

    58. Masilamani M, Wei J, Sampson HA. Regulation of the immune response by soybean isoflavones. Immunol Res 2012;54(1—3):95—110.

    59. Sakai T, Kogiso M. Soy isoflavones and immunity. J Med Invest 2008;55(3—4):167—73.

    60. Pan Y, Anthony M, Clarkson TB. Evidence for up-regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor mRNA by soy phytoestrogens in the frontal cortex of retired breeder female rats. Neurosci Lett 1999;261(1):17—20.

    61. Stackman RW, Eckenstein F, Frei B, Kulhanek D, Nowlin J,Quinn JF. Prevention of age-related spatial memory deficits in a transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease by chronic Ginkgo biloba treatment. Exp Neurol 2003;184(1):510—20.

    62. Sun SW, Yu HQ, Zhang H, Zheng YL, Wang JJ, Luo L. Quercetin attenuates spontaneous behavior and spatial memory impairment in d-galactose-treated mice by increasing brain antioxidant capacity. Nutr Res 2007;27(3):169—75.

    63. Kim H, Xia H, Li L, Gewin J. Attenuation of neurodegenerationrelevant modifications of brain proteins by dietary soy. Biofactors 2000;12(1—4):243—50.

    64. Kritz-Silverstein D, Von Mühlen D, Barrett-Connor E, Bressel MA. Isoflavones and cognitive function in older women: the SOy and Postmenopausal Health in Aging (SOPHIA) study. Menopause 2003;10(3):196—202.

    65. Macready A, Kennedy O, Ellis J, Williams C, Spencer JE, Butler L. Flavonoids and cognitive function: a review of human randomized controlled trial studies and recommendations for future studies. Genes Nutr 2009;4(4):227—42.

    66. File SE, Jarrett N, Fluck E, Duffy R, Casey K, Wiseman H. Eating soya improves human memory. Psychopharmacology (Berl)2001;157(4):430—6.

    67. Adams SM, Aksenova MV, Aksenov MY, Mactutus CF, Booze RM. Soy isoflavones genistein and daidzein exert anti-apoptotic actions via a selective ER-mediated mechanism in neurons following HIV-1 Tat 1-86 exposure. PLoS One 2012;7(5):e37540.

    68. Blair C. How similar are fluid cognition and general intelligence? A developmental neuroscience perspective on fluid cognition as an aspect of human cognitive ability. Behav Brain Sci 2006;29(2):109—25; discussion 125—60.

    69. Fry AF, Hale S. Relationships among processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence in children. Biol Psychol 2000;54(1—3):1—34.

    70. Huepe D, Roca M, Salas N, Canales-Johnson A, Rivera-Rei AA,Zamorano L, et al. Fluid intelligence and psychosocial outcome:from logical problem solving to social adaptation. PLoS One 2011;6(9):e24858.

    71. Kuwajima M, Sawaguchi T. Similar prefrontal cortical activities between general fluid intelligence and visuospatial working memory tasks in preschool children as revealed by optical topography. Exp Brain Res 2010;206(4):381—97.

    72. Verdun D, Siika A, Sawe C, Ernst J. Experience and challenges in the recruitment and retention of HIV-infected rural Kenyan women and their Children into a randomized nutrition intervention study. Research brief 10-02-HNP. Davis: Global Livestock Collaborative Research Support Program, University of California, Davis; 2010.

    Kek Khee Loo, MD

    Kaiser Permanente Los Angeles Medical Center, Department of Pediatrics, 1526 Edgemont Street, 5th Floor, Los Angeles, CA 90027, USA

    E-mail: kloo@ucla.edu

    17 May 2017;Accepted 27 June 2017

    美女福利国产在线| 久久香蕉精品热| 成人影院久久| 国产精品久久电影中文字幕| 99精品久久久久人妻精品| 久久精品亚洲av国产电影网| 精品福利永久在线观看| 黄频高清免费视频| 长腿黑丝高跟| 99在线人妻在线中文字幕| 亚洲精品美女久久av网站| 久久中文看片网| 日韩欧美一区视频在线观看| 99久久人妻综合| 亚洲国产欧美日韩在线播放| 三上悠亚av全集在线观看| 久久久久久免费高清国产稀缺| 色老头精品视频在线观看| 9热在线视频观看99| 国产精品国产高清国产av| 黑人操中国人逼视频| 精品国产美女av久久久久小说| 丰满的人妻完整版| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 美女午夜性视频免费| 亚洲av美国av| 久久久水蜜桃国产精品网| 国产精品免费一区二区三区在线| 99久久人妻综合| 亚洲精品一区av在线观看| 亚洲成人免费av在线播放| 久久久久亚洲av毛片大全| 久热爱精品视频在线9| 亚洲一区中文字幕在线| 一区二区三区激情视频| 好男人电影高清在线观看| 国产在线观看jvid| 久久精品国产清高在天天线| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 久久人妻福利社区极品人妻图片| 免费人成视频x8x8入口观看| 91国产中文字幕| 免费在线观看亚洲国产| 美女扒开内裤让男人捅视频| 国产片内射在线| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添小说| 欧美人与性动交α欧美软件| 变态另类成人亚洲欧美熟女 | 中出人妻视频一区二区| 日本三级黄在线观看| 久久久久久久久免费视频了| 黑人操中国人逼视频| 777久久人妻少妇嫩草av网站| 欧美日韩黄片免| 国产野战对白在线观看| 天天影视国产精品| 国产精品香港三级国产av潘金莲| 淫秽高清视频在线观看| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 欧美成狂野欧美在线观看| 亚洲少妇的诱惑av| 国产一区在线观看成人免费| 亚洲久久久国产精品| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 一进一出抽搐动态| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| 日韩欧美一区视频在线观看| 亚洲午夜精品一区,二区,三区| 国产成人免费无遮挡视频| 亚洲在线自拍视频| 国产精品一区二区在线不卡| 亚洲在线自拍视频| 在线观看免费高清a一片| 欧美乱码精品一区二区三区| 日韩精品中文字幕看吧| 777久久人妻少妇嫩草av网站| 不卡av一区二区三区| 亚洲午夜精品一区,二区,三区| 激情视频va一区二区三区| 亚洲 欧美 日韩 在线 免费| 亚洲五月色婷婷综合| 欧美精品亚洲一区二区| 国产精品99久久99久久久不卡| 午夜精品国产一区二区电影| 午夜a级毛片| 精品人妻在线不人妻| 午夜免费观看网址| 88av欧美| 国产三级黄色录像| 国产欧美日韩精品亚洲av| 亚洲成人免费av在线播放| 亚洲成人久久性| 亚洲伊人色综图| 久久精品91蜜桃| 欧美精品啪啪一区二区三区| 看片在线看免费视频| 成人影院久久| 91老司机精品| 久久国产精品人妻蜜桃| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添小说| 9色porny在线观看| 麻豆一二三区av精品| 50天的宝宝边吃奶边哭怎么回事| 热re99久久国产66热| 免费在线观看视频国产中文字幕亚洲| 国产亚洲精品久久久久久毛片| 亚洲av成人不卡在线观看播放网| 91字幕亚洲| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av在线| 免费看a级黄色片| 日本五十路高清| 国产不卡一卡二| av视频免费观看在线观看| 欧美激情 高清一区二区三区| 国产精品九九99| 亚洲黑人精品在线| 国产精品偷伦视频观看了| 女警被强在线播放| 性少妇av在线| 女人精品久久久久毛片| 国产成人欧美在线观看| 一级片'在线观看视频| 精品一区二区三区av网在线观看| 美女福利国产在线| 黄色怎么调成土黄色| 亚洲精品美女久久av网站| 纯流量卡能插随身wifi吗| 国产一区在线观看成人免费| 人人妻人人爽人人添夜夜欢视频| 99精国产麻豆久久婷婷| 欧美日韩视频精品一区| 国产精品永久免费网站| 精品第一国产精品| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 淫秽高清视频在线观看| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| 桃红色精品国产亚洲av| 日本vs欧美在线观看视频| 国产成人啪精品午夜网站| 久久久久国产一级毛片高清牌| 欧美亚洲日本最大视频资源| 两个人看的免费小视频| 久热爱精品视频在线9| 99精品久久久久人妻精品| 法律面前人人平等表现在哪些方面| 一个人观看的视频www高清免费观看 | 大型av网站在线播放| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 无人区码免费观看不卡| 亚洲一区高清亚洲精品| 亚洲一卡2卡3卡4卡5卡精品中文| 欧美人与性动交α欧美精品济南到| 久久这里只有精品19| 中文字幕色久视频| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区 | 99国产极品粉嫩在线观看| 午夜福利一区二区在线看| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 日日干狠狠操夜夜爽| 亚洲人成电影观看| 女警被强在线播放| 精品欧美一区二区三区在线| 午夜久久久在线观看| 窝窝影院91人妻| 欧美日韩精品网址| 不卡一级毛片| 国产真人三级小视频在线观看| 精品人妻在线不人妻| 十八禁网站免费在线| 久久人妻福利社区极品人妻图片| 国产伦人伦偷精品视频| 波多野结衣一区麻豆| 国产精品亚洲av一区麻豆| 制服人妻中文乱码| 国产熟女xx| 久久精品91蜜桃| 男女做爰动态图高潮gif福利片 | 国产精品99久久99久久久不卡| 欧美成人午夜精品| 国产一区二区三区视频了| 久久国产乱子伦精品免费另类| 国产不卡一卡二| 国产精品爽爽va在线观看网站 | 国产单亲对白刺激| 黄色丝袜av网址大全| 亚洲午夜理论影院| 又紧又爽又黄一区二区| 伊人久久大香线蕉亚洲五| 日韩 欧美 亚洲 中文字幕| 亚洲av美国av| 淫秽高清视频在线观看| 这个男人来自地球电影免费观看| 国产精品秋霞免费鲁丝片| 高清在线国产一区| 国产精品成人在线| 亚洲伊人色综图| 99久久国产精品久久久| 日韩欧美国产一区二区入口| 亚洲精品国产精品久久久不卡| 十八禁人妻一区二区| 夜夜看夜夜爽夜夜摸 | 国产精品电影一区二区三区| www.精华液| e午夜精品久久久久久久| 在线观看免费午夜福利视频| 最近最新免费中文字幕在线| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清 | 一区二区三区精品91| 日韩大码丰满熟妇| av片东京热男人的天堂| 欧美久久黑人一区二区| 日韩免费高清中文字幕av| 国产99白浆流出| av在线播放免费不卡| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 久久天躁狠狠躁夜夜2o2o| bbb黄色大片| 一级毛片精品| 国产无遮挡羞羞视频在线观看| 一区二区日韩欧美中文字幕| 亚洲精品一二三| 狠狠狠狠99中文字幕| 视频区图区小说| 亚洲 欧美一区二区三区| 国产一区二区三区视频了| 精品国产美女av久久久久小说| 亚洲精华国产精华精| 十分钟在线观看高清视频www| 国产aⅴ精品一区二区三区波| av电影中文网址| 天堂俺去俺来也www色官网| 精品一区二区三区四区五区乱码| 久久影院123| 久热爱精品视频在线9| 国产麻豆69| 精品日产1卡2卡| 久久午夜综合久久蜜桃| 免费日韩欧美在线观看| 国产高清视频在线播放一区| 十八禁网站免费在线| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 女人被狂操c到高潮| 精品国产国语对白av| 国产欧美日韩一区二区三区在线| 国产精品一区二区三区四区久久 | 大陆偷拍与自拍| 一级,二级,三级黄色视频| 亚洲成国产人片在线观看| 色播在线永久视频| 18禁观看日本| 中文字幕人妻丝袜一区二区| 国产成人精品无人区| 老司机福利观看| 国产亚洲精品一区二区www| 精品国产超薄肉色丝袜足j| 在线观看一区二区三区| 国产有黄有色有爽视频| av福利片在线| 免费在线观看日本一区| 亚洲欧美日韩另类电影网站| av欧美777| 亚洲av熟女| 国产无遮挡羞羞视频在线观看| 亚洲专区国产一区二区| 涩涩av久久男人的天堂| 成人免费观看视频高清| 波多野结衣一区麻豆| 久久国产精品人妻蜜桃| 久久国产精品影院| 精品一品国产午夜福利视频| 黄频高清免费视频| 久久久国产成人免费| 国产在线精品亚洲第一网站| 女同久久另类99精品国产91| 久热这里只有精品99| 欧美日韩乱码在线| 久久精品国产99精品国产亚洲性色 | 无遮挡黄片免费观看| 欧美日韩中文字幕国产精品一区二区三区 | 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 久久精品aⅴ一区二区三区四区| 91成年电影在线观看| 国产国语露脸激情在线看| 久久国产亚洲av麻豆专区| 国产欧美日韩一区二区三区在线| 长腿黑丝高跟| 别揉我奶头~嗯~啊~动态视频| 大码成人一级视频| 亚洲一区中文字幕在线| 国产又色又爽无遮挡免费看| 悠悠久久av| av有码第一页| 久99久视频精品免费| 欧美最黄视频在线播放免费 | 国产免费av片在线观看野外av| 18美女黄网站色大片免费观看| 黑人猛操日本美女一级片| 亚洲色图av天堂| 国产主播在线观看一区二区| 1024视频免费在线观看| 伦理电影免费视频| 一级黄色大片毛片| 国产一卡二卡三卡精品| a级毛片在线看网站| 男女高潮啪啪啪动态图| 亚洲熟女毛片儿| 亚洲精品美女久久久久99蜜臀| 香蕉国产在线看| 可以在线观看毛片的网站| 日本免费一区二区三区高清不卡 | av有码第一页| 很黄的视频免费| 免费在线观看视频国产中文字幕亚洲| 亚洲第一av免费看| 这个男人来自地球电影免费观看| 国产精品一区二区免费欧美| 999精品在线视频| 国产午夜精品久久久久久| 日本wwww免费看| www日本在线高清视频| 国产真人三级小视频在线观看| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放 | 欧美人与性动交α欧美软件| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 精品一区二区三区av网在线观看| 99国产精品一区二区三区| 亚洲伊人色综图| 久久精品国产亚洲av高清一级| 欧洲精品卡2卡3卡4卡5卡区| 精品少妇一区二区三区视频日本电影| 欧美午夜高清在线| 大香蕉久久成人网| 黄色毛片三级朝国网站| 最好的美女福利视频网| 国产成人精品久久二区二区91| 法律面前人人平等表现在哪些方面| 精品福利观看| 人人妻人人爽人人添夜夜欢视频| 男女下面插进去视频免费观看| 性欧美人与动物交配| 两性午夜刺激爽爽歪歪视频在线观看 | 欧美成人免费av一区二区三区| 日韩精品中文字幕看吧| 在线永久观看黄色视频| 国产精华一区二区三区| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 久久久久久人人人人人| 亚洲狠狠婷婷综合久久图片| 成年女人毛片免费观看观看9| 一级a爱视频在线免费观看| 99精品欧美一区二区三区四区| 亚洲人成电影免费在线| 亚洲色图综合在线观看| 91成人精品电影| 69av精品久久久久久| 久久热在线av| 欧美日韩亚洲高清精品| 嫩草影院精品99| 成人18禁在线播放| 999精品在线视频| 丝袜美腿诱惑在线| 熟女少妇亚洲综合色aaa.| 黑人巨大精品欧美一区二区mp4| 日韩三级视频一区二区三区| 国产精品亚洲av一区麻豆| 9热在线视频观看99| 18禁国产床啪视频网站| 香蕉国产在线看| 国产熟女xx| 午夜福利一区二区在线看| 一级作爱视频免费观看| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清 | 欧美日韩中文字幕国产精品一区二区三区 | 亚洲精品中文字幕在线视频| 色综合站精品国产| 精品午夜福利视频在线观看一区| 久热爱精品视频在线9| 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看| 国产亚洲欧美98| 欧美日韩av久久| 欧美日本中文国产一区发布| av片东京热男人的天堂| 757午夜福利合集在线观看| 欧美黄色片欧美黄色片| 嫁个100分男人电影在线观看| 精品高清国产在线一区| 在线观看一区二区三区激情| 18美女黄网站色大片免费观看| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 国产精品香港三级国产av潘金莲| 黄片大片在线免费观看| 日韩大尺度精品在线看网址 | 三上悠亚av全集在线观看| 国产精品爽爽va在线观看网站 | 精品国产超薄肉色丝袜足j| 女性生殖器流出的白浆| 人妻久久中文字幕网| 丰满饥渴人妻一区二区三| 午夜福利免费观看在线| 国产精品 欧美亚洲| 动漫黄色视频在线观看| 日韩三级视频一区二区三区| 在线av久久热| 国产亚洲精品久久久久久毛片| 淫秽高清视频在线观看| 婷婷丁香在线五月| 午夜激情av网站| 91麻豆精品激情在线观看国产 | 不卡一级毛片| 日韩视频一区二区在线观看| 国产激情久久老熟女| 丰满饥渴人妻一区二区三| 久久精品人人爽人人爽视色| 欧美中文综合在线视频| 少妇粗大呻吟视频| 91av网站免费观看| 人人澡人人妻人| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 啪啪无遮挡十八禁网站| 国产成人av激情在线播放| 天天躁狠狠躁夜夜躁狠狠躁| 一级,二级,三级黄色视频| 中文字幕av电影在线播放| 一区福利在线观看| 久久久国产成人免费| 不卡av一区二区三区| 天堂√8在线中文| 自拍欧美九色日韩亚洲蝌蚪91| 国产精品免费视频内射| 国产精品一区二区三区四区久久 | 中文字幕最新亚洲高清| 午夜精品国产一区二区电影| av在线天堂中文字幕 | 色老头精品视频在线观看| 国产成年人精品一区二区 | 国产野战对白在线观看| 国产精华一区二区三区| 超碰成人久久| 两性午夜刺激爽爽歪歪视频在线观看 | 午夜两性在线视频| 成人免费观看视频高清| 国产高清国产精品国产三级| 久久久国产欧美日韩av| 欧美成狂野欧美在线观看| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看 | 黄频高清免费视频| 黄色丝袜av网址大全| 午夜影院日韩av| 精品熟女少妇八av免费久了| 午夜激情av网站| 亚洲九九香蕉| 免费观看精品视频网站| 两个人免费观看高清视频| 亚洲免费av在线视频| 热re99久久精品国产66热6| 国产成人精品无人区| 看黄色毛片网站| 国产91精品成人一区二区三区| 亚洲avbb在线观看| 最好的美女福利视频网| 波多野结衣一区麻豆| 老熟妇乱子伦视频在线观看| 久久久精品欧美日韩精品| 国产精品一区二区免费欧美| 国产又色又爽无遮挡免费看| 日本撒尿小便嘘嘘汇集6| 丰满饥渴人妻一区二区三| 精品福利永久在线观看| 欧美乱色亚洲激情| 女性生殖器流出的白浆| 人人妻人人添人人爽欧美一区卜| 在线观看免费视频网站a站| 首页视频小说图片口味搜索| 老熟妇仑乱视频hdxx| 日韩高清综合在线| 岛国在线观看网站| 午夜福利在线免费观看网站| 日日夜夜操网爽| 亚洲,欧美精品.| 国产激情久久老熟女| 亚洲国产毛片av蜜桃av| 别揉我奶头~嗯~啊~动态视频| 国产视频一区二区在线看| cao死你这个sao货| 久久精品亚洲熟妇少妇任你| 午夜福利在线免费观看网站| 高清在线国产一区| 男人舔女人下体高潮全视频| 久久精品国产99精品国产亚洲性色 | 国产日韩一区二区三区精品不卡| 涩涩av久久男人的天堂| 男人操女人黄网站| 热re99久久国产66热| 日本黄色日本黄色录像| 制服人妻中文乱码| 一区二区日韩欧美中文字幕| 国产亚洲欧美98| 又紧又爽又黄一区二区| 精品人妻在线不人妻| 午夜老司机福利片| 免费在线观看影片大全网站| 国产精华一区二区三区| 一边摸一边做爽爽视频免费| 精品免费久久久久久久清纯| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 午夜精品国产一区二区电影| 久久九九热精品免费| 久久久久久久久中文| 免费高清视频大片| 色尼玛亚洲综合影院| 乱人伦中国视频| 别揉我奶头~嗯~啊~动态视频| 91九色精品人成在线观看| 国产精品日韩av在线免费观看 | 老汉色∧v一级毛片| 人成视频在线观看免费观看| 精品午夜福利视频在线观看一区| 亚洲色图av天堂| 国产又色又爽无遮挡免费看| 国产区一区二久久| 免费一级毛片在线播放高清视频 | 丰满迷人的少妇在线观看| 日本免费一区二区三区高清不卡 | 99热只有精品国产| 一级作爱视频免费观看| 成人特级黄色片久久久久久久| 91成年电影在线观看| 久久草成人影院| 99国产极品粉嫩在线观看| 中文字幕另类日韩欧美亚洲嫩草| 精品高清国产在线一区| 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 一级片'在线观看视频| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 国产野战对白在线观看| 日韩一卡2卡3卡4卡2021年| 一边摸一边做爽爽视频免费| 一边摸一边抽搐一进一小说| 精品高清国产在线一区| 五月开心婷婷网| 色尼玛亚洲综合影院| 亚洲国产精品sss在线观看 | 成人黄色视频免费在线看| 欧美成人午夜精品| 国产三级黄色录像| 脱女人内裤的视频| 久久久国产一区二区| 999久久久国产精品视频| av网站免费在线观看视频| 成年版毛片免费区| 色综合站精品国产| 久久精品国产综合久久久| 日本 av在线| √禁漫天堂资源中文www| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| 欧美日韩福利视频一区二区| 热99国产精品久久久久久7| 国产一区二区激情短视频| 中文字幕最新亚洲高清| 精品国产美女av久久久久小说| 亚洲av成人av| 夫妻午夜视频| 午夜福利欧美成人| 久99久视频精品免费| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 极品人妻少妇av视频| 超碰97精品在线观看| 日本免费a在线| 久久午夜综合久久蜜桃| 日韩欧美三级三区| 成人18禁高潮啪啪吃奶动态图| 美女高潮喷水抽搐中文字幕| 人人妻人人添人人爽欧美一区卜| 在线免费观看的www视频| www国产在线视频色| 国产三级在线视频| 日本vs欧美在线观看视频| 国产一区二区三区视频了| 99精品久久久久人妻精品| 精品电影一区二区在线| 亚洲av熟女| 色婷婷久久久亚洲欧美| 国产亚洲欧美在线一区二区| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 国产精品国产av在线观看| 搡老乐熟女国产| 一级a爱视频在线免费观看| 一级,二级,三级黄色视频| 9191精品国产免费久久| av在线天堂中文字幕 | 1024视频免费在线观看| 动漫黄色视频在线观看| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 欧美精品亚洲一区二区| 一边摸一边抽搐一进一小说| 老熟妇乱子伦视频在线观看| 欧美成人性av电影在线观看| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 啦啦啦 在线观看视频| 成人18禁在线播放| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出 | 亚洲熟女毛片儿| 日韩欧美三级三区| 亚洲国产欧美网| 午夜亚洲福利在线播放| 成熟少妇高潮喷水视频| 国产精品国产av在线观看| 精品欧美一区二区三区在线| avwww免费| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 亚洲中文字幕日韩| 一级作爱视频免费观看| 动漫黄色视频在线观看|