• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Social rank and cortisol among female rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)

    2013-09-20 03:39:30DongDongQINJoshuaDominicRizakXiaoLiFENGXunXunCHUShangChuanYANGChunLuLILongBaoLVYuanYeMAXinTianHU
    Zoological Research 2013年2期

    Dong-Dong QIN, Joshua Dominic Rizak, Xiao-Li FENG, Xun-Xun CHU Shang-Chuan YANG, Chun-Lu LI, Long-Bao LV, Yuan-Ye MA,5,6,* , Xin-Tian HU,5,6,*

    1. State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Kunming Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming 650223, China;2. University of the Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China;3. Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China;4. Kunming Primate Research Center of the Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming 650223,China;5. Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China;6. Kunming Primate Research Center, Kunming Institute of Zoology, the Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming 650223, China

    In many animal societies, stress generated within long-term dominant-subordinate relationships often predicts an increased vulnerability to an array of diseases,e.g., cardiovascular disease, rheumatoid arthritis,respiratory, reproductive, immune related disorders and mental illnesses (Adler et al, 2000; Kawachi & Kennedy,2006; Shively & Clarkson, 1994; Siegrist & Marmot,2004; Wilkinson, 2001; Cohen et al, 1997; Manuck et al,1995; Sapolsky & Share, 1994; Ader & Cohen, 2001;Dhabhar & McEwen, 1999). In response to varying stressors, animals release hormones and glucose into the bloodstream to provide extra energy, which also inhibits digestion, growth, tissue repair and reproduction. This secretion is more commonly referred to as the “fight or flight” reaction, or the stress response, which is accompanied by enhanced cognition, enhanced immune defenses,1blunted pain perception, and sharpened sensory thresholds (McEwen & Lasley, 2004; Sapolsky, 2004b).This stress response helps animals adapt to acute stress and/or short-term reactions to stress. Chronic stress,however, such as that induced by a social hierarchy, leads to excessive activation of this response, which, in turn,has been shown to have pathogenic ends (Sapolsky,2004a).

    A number of stress-related physiological studies have shown that animals experiencing more social stress due to dominance rank within a hierarchy exhibit hyperactivity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA)axis, including elevated basal levels of glucocorticoids,enlarged adrenal glands accompanied by increased hormone secretion, a repressed stress response to glucocorticoids when dealing with a challenge, an impaired sensitivity of the HPA axis to negative feedback regulation and a delayed recovery from stress (Sapolsky, 2005).

    The effects of dominance rank on subordinate and dominant individuals vary between species, and even within species. These variations largely depend on the dominance style, the means of maintaining a despotic dominance, the style of breeding system, rank stability,the availability of coping outlets for subordinates, the ease with which subordinates avoid dominant individuals,the availability of alternative strategies to overt competition, and even individual disposition or personality (Sapolsky, 2005). In some cases, dominant individuals are more socially stressed, such as when species maintain the hierarchy through frequent physical reassertion of dominance, in species that have a cooperative style of breeding system, those with transient periods of major rank instability, and species where dominants perceive neutral interactions as challenging while subordinates take advantage of coping strategies(Sapolsky, 2005). By contrast, these profiles are commonly observed in species with several defining characteristics: among subordinates in species where a despotic hierarchy is maintained through nonphysical intimidation, in species with consistently stable ranks,lower coping outlets available for subordinates, a lack alternative strategies to overt competition, and in species where dominants are adept at exerting social control and are highly affiliative whereas subordinates are poor at exploiting opportunities for social support, and likewise for species kept in an enclosure without sufficient space for subordinates to evade dominant animals (Sapolsky,2005).

    The two main influences of the stress response are the activation of the sympathetic nervous system and the release of glucocorticoids (such as cortisol or hydrocortisone in primates, and corticosterone in most rodent species) by the HPA axis (Sapolsky et al, 2000).In response to acute stressors, the sympathetic nervous system secrets catecholamine hormones within seconds,making it difficult to obtain accurate measures of the acute stress response. Meanwhile, glucocorticoid levels change over the course of 1-2 minutes, allowing for an extensive study of the correlation between social rank and glucocorticoid changes. While both glucocorticoids and catecholamines are essential for animals to survive acute stressors, they are pathogenic when secreted in excess (Sapolsky et al, 2000). As such, animals of low social rank have been found to present a pathological condition consisting of elevated basal glucocorticoid levels, a slowed on/off “switch” of stress response, and a blunted sensitivity to excessive levels of glucocorticoids.This pathological condition has been observed in cynomolgus monkeys (Adams et al, 1985), talapoin monkeys (Keverne et al, 1982), olive baboons (Sapolsky,1990), squirrel monkeys (Manogue et al, 1975) and lemurs (Schilling & Perret, 1987). Although stress in subordinates has been shown to be related to the hypersecretion of glucocorticoids, there are exceptions reported among macaques (Bercovitch & Clarke, 1995;Chamove & Bowman, 1976; Gust et al, 1991; Gust et al,1993; Van Schaik et al, 1991), squirrel monkeys (Coe et al, 1979; Mendoza et al, 1978; Steklis et al, 1986),marmosets (Saltzman et al, 1994), talapoin monkeys(Keverne et al, 1982) and ring-tailed lemurs (Cavogelli,1999).

    As the primary glucocorticoids in humans, nonhuman primates and many larger mammals, cortisol has been used to assess stress response levels in the plasma,saliva, urine, and feces of many species (Cattet et al,2003; Constable et al, 2006; Keay et al, 2006;Millspaugh et al, 2002; Wallner et al, 1999; Whitten et al,1999; Weingrill et al, 2004; Abbott et al, 2003). The most commonly used assays to detect cortisol levels in these samples include radioimmunoassays (RIAs), liquid chromatography-mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS) and enzyme-linked immunosorbent assays (ELISA) (Gatti et al, 2009). Although plasma samples provide a measurement of cortisol levels at a single point in time,they can only be used to assess acute stress, and are susceptible to physiological fluctuations. Similarly,cortisol levels measured in the plasma peak in the early morning and gradually decrease to their lowest levels in the evening, necessitating multiple blood samples being taken waking until of sleep in order to accurately assess stress levels. Though precise, this methodology is difficult to apply to larger populations, and the compliance of individual participants with the requisite sampling schedule varies (Russell et al, 2011). Likewise, the cortisol measured in blood samples reflect total cortisol,and can thus be affected by changes in levels of cortisolbinding globulin (e.g., by birth control pills or pregnancy), which give the illusion of increased stress levels. Furthermore, the act of blood sampling via venipuncture may be a source of stress and increase plasma cortisol levels (Vining et al, 1983), potentially yielding results that do not reflect an unbiased observation.

    Contrary to the more invasive methods used in plasma samples, salivary, urinal and fecal samples can be obtained relatively more easily and the measured cortisol reflects free cortisol. Unfortunately, the concentrations of cortisol still fluctuate significantly throughout the course of the day (Beerda et al, 1996; Cattet et al, 2003; Ekkel et al, 1996; Keay et al, 2006; Mormède et al, 2007;Owen et al, 2005; von der Ohe & Servheen, 2002). To overcome the issue of an animal’s diurnal rhythm,samples must be collected all day (Burch, 1982). More importantly, the cortisol levels measured in these media only reflect short-term stress occurring over hours to days, so without repeated sampling of animals, it is not possible to assess chronic stress levels that occur over weeks or months (Keay et al, 2006; Owen et al, 2005).

    Hair offers some interesting alternatives in that it has a fairly predictable growth rate of approximately 1 cm/month. Likewise, hair follicles can be used to track cortisol levels over time, with the most proximal 1 cm segment to the scalp approximates the last month’s cortisol production, the second most proximal 1 cm segment approximates the cortisol production during the month before that, and so on (Wennig, 2000). This makes cortisol measured in hair a biomarker of chronic stress,as demonstrated previously in two studies of rhesus macaques in which hair samples were obtained to assess long-term stress levels (Davenport et al, 2006; Feng et al,2011). Moreover, hair samples are easily transported and stored in envelopes or vials at room temperature for years, and gathering the samples does not affect stress levels of the subjects (Gow et al, 2010; Russell et al,2011).

    In the present study, we assessed long-term stress levels by measuring cortisol concentrations in hair samples from female rhesus macaques in order to test the predominant hypothesis of whether stress levels may be related or influenced by the strictness of the dominance hierarchy.

    MATERIALS AND METHODS

    Subjects

    In total, 20 female rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) living in five breeding groups (n=5 in each group, where one monkey in the group was male) at the Kunming Primate Research Center, Chinese Academy of Sciences were used in this study. The monkeys ranged from 12 to 20 years of age (14.75±1.89 years), and were housed in colonies with access to a connected indoor(2.61×2.46×2.58 m)-outdoor (2.67×2.66×2.67 m) cage.All animals were given commercial monkey biscuits twice a day with tap water ad libitum, and were fed with fruits and vegetables once daily. All subjects had lived in their respective social groups at least 1 year prior to initial observation. All animal procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Kunming Institute of Zoology and were carried out in accordance with the National Institute of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

    Experimental design

    Animal behaviors were video recorded using a focal follow technique and analyzed to calculate the linearity of hierarchies and dominance rank for each monkey(Altmann, 1974). After completion of the video recordings, hair samples were obtained to measure the cortisol levels. Afterward, correlations between social rank and hair cortisol levels were calculated.

    Behavior sampling

    The monkeys were given 7 days to acquaint themselves with the observers and cameras prior to recording and sampling, at which time a digital camera fixed on a tripod was set up in front of the colony to record one of the monkeys in the cage. Observers kept as far away as possible (minimum of 5 meters) from enclosures to avoid disturbing the animals during recording. Fourteen 1-hour recordings were collected for each monkey throughout a 7 day period. Two recordings were collected per day, one taken for two hours in the morning (9:00-11:00) and another segment in the afternoon (14:00-16:00). All video recordings were stored on a hard disk before being reviewed and interpreted by three technicians. The three viewers analyzed each video recording simultaneously and came to a consensus regarding the behavior classification.

    Social rank determination

    The social ranks for monkeys in each group were calculated according to David’s score (DS), which is based on the consistent outcomes of agonistic encounters(Gammell et al, 2003; Zhao et al, 2011). Aggressive behaviors were divided into extreme aggression and mild aggression. Behaviors that involved physical contact (a bite, slap or grab)were classified as extreme aggression.A stare threat, open-mouth threat, chase, or displacement was considered a mild aggression. Submissive behaviors were also sub-divided into extreme and mild. Extreme submissive behaviors included a scream, a scream threat,crouch, or fleeing. Mild submissions included a lip smack, grimace, submissive present, or moving away(Shively et al, 2005).

    The averaged frequencies of agonistic behaviors were used to assign a hierarchal rank for each monkey based on their individual DS. A detailed method for calculating a DS has been described previously(Gammell et al, 2003). Standardized DS scores (DSS)between each group were then produced in the following manner: the smallest DS value (the largest minus value)in a group was added to its absolute value to create a zero value, then other DS values in the same group were added with the same value. The new DS values were then divided by the largest new DS value. For each group, this process created DSSvalues ranging from 0 to 1,indicating the lowest rank (DSS= 0) to the highest rank(DSS= 1) in the respective groups.

    Furthermore, an H value calculated using a previously described equation (Singh et al, 1992; Singh et al, 2003) to determine the linearity of the social hierarchy. Calculated H values ranged from 0 to 1,indicating the social hierarchy on a continuum from the total absence of a ranking system (H=0) to a perfect linear order (H=1).

    Hair sampling and cortisol extraction

    Hair samples from all monkeys were collected between 13:30 and 15:00. At 13:30, each monkey was captured by an experienced technician using a net and removed from the colony. The hair was then taken from the back of the monkey’s neck using a pair of scissors,with particular attention made by technicians to not break or damage the skin. Hair was placed into a small pouch of aluminum foil for protection and stored as previously described (Davenport et al, 2006; Wennig,2000).

    The hair cortisol extraction was done as described in detail in previous studies (Davenport et al, 2006; Feng et al, 2011). Briefly, 500 mg of hair sample was washed twice for 3 min each time in 10 mL isopropanol to remove surface contaminants, dried at 37 °C for 8 hours,and then pulverized using a Retsch ball mill (Retsch M400) at 26 Hz for 2.5 min. After, 400 mg of the powdered hair was weighed and incubated in 8 mL of methanol at room temperature for 24 hours with a slow rotation to extract cortisol. Samples were then centrifuged at 8 000 r/min for 5 minutes, and 4 mL of the supernatant was pipetted into a centrifuge tube and dried under a stream of nitrogen gas. The precipitate was reconstituted with 0.5 mL of phosphate buffered saline solution and stored at ?20 °C until assayed. The cortisol concentration in each sample was quantified with a radioimmunoassay kit(Cortisol RIA DSL-2000, America). The cortisol RIA was performed at the Radioimmuno Laboratory of the Second Affiliated Hospital of the Kunming Medical College. The cortisol extraction and RIA analysis were performed under a double-blind design, with each hair sample tested twice and the mean of the two hair cortisol values used to reduce measurement error.

    Data analysis

    Data analysis was conducted using SPSS (SPSS inc,Chicago, IL, USA). Spearman correlations were used to assess the effects of age on social rank and hair cortisol levels and evaluate the correlation between social rank and hair cortisol levels. In all analyses, P-values were determined from two-tailed tests, with the significance level set at P<0.05.

    RESULTS

    Hierarchy linearity and social rank

    The linearity of the social hierarchy of five female rhesus macaque breeding colonies was measured using the H values, which were 0.86, 0.49, 0.86, 0.99 and 1.00 among the different groups. Two of the five groups were found to have more stringent linear hierarchies, with H values above 0.90. The remaining three groups with H values less than 0.90 were considered to have a weak linear order in their hierarchy.

    Effects of age on social rank and hair cortisol levels

    In all the female monkeys (n=20), age did not affect the social rank as measured by DS values (r=-0.14,P=0.57) and DSSvalues (r=-0.13, P=0.58). Similarly,there were no significant relationships between age and hair cortisol levels within the female monkeys (r=-0.06,P=0.82). In despotic female monkeys (n=8), age did not affect the social rank, including DS values (r=-0.18,P=0.67) and DSSvalues (r=-0.18, P=0.67), and there were no significant relationships between age and hair cortisol levels within any female monkeys (r=0.25,P=0.55). Likewise, in less stringent hierarchies (n=12),age did not affect the social rank, including DS values(r=-0.13, P=0.68) and DSSvalues (r=-0.09, P=0.78).Overall, there were no significant relationships between age and hair cortisol levels within all the female monkeys included in our study (r=-0.25, P=0.44).

    Correlation between social rank and hair cortisol levels

    No significant correlation was discovered in the relationship between social rank and hair cortisol levels within all the female monkeys (n=20) (Figure 1A;r=-0.01, P=0.98; Figure 1B; r=-0.06, P=0.79). However,a unique correlation between social rank and cortisol levels emerged after the female groups were separated into two groups according to their calculated H values.These two groups included monkeys in despotic female groups (n=8) and those in less stringent hierarchies(n=12). No significant correlation between social rank and cortisol levels was found in the despotic female groups (Figure 2A; r=0.35, P=0.40; Figure 2B; r=0.35,P=0.40) but we did find a significant correlation between social rank and cortisol levels within the less stringent hierarchies (Figure 3A; r=-0.62, P=0.03; Figure 3B;r=-0.61, P=0.03).

    DISCUSSION

    Figure 1 Correlation between social rank and hair cortisol levels in all female monkeys (n=20)X-axes refer to DS value (A) and DSS value (B). Y-axis refers to the mean hair cortisol (μg/dL).

    Figure 2 Correlation between social rank and hair cortisol levels in despotic female groups (n=8)X-axes refer to DS value (A) and DSS value (B). Y-axis refers to the mean hair cortisol (μg/dL).

    Figure 3 Correlation between social rank and hair cortisol levels in less stringent female groups (n=12)X-axes refer to DS value (A) and DSS value (B). Y-axis refers to the mean hair cortisol (μg/dL).

    In the present study, we first examined female rank physiology (as quantified by cortisol levels in hair) in adult rhesus monkeys, then, after finding no relationship between rank and cortisol levels, the groups were separated into two classes according to the linear degree of their hierarchies: despotic groups with a linear hierarchy (H values>.90) and groups with less stringent hierarchies (H values<.90) (Li et al, 2004). After separation, two distinct trends emerged from the analysis.In less stringent groups, there was a negative correlation between social rank and cortisol levels, suggesting that low ranking monkeys were more socially stressed than higher ranking monkeys. Conversely, groups with despotic hierarchies did not display this trend.

    Overall, despotic groups were much stricter than the less stringent group hierarchies, which meant that monkeys in the latter groups were relatively more assertive with dominance rank than monkeys in despotic groups. This finding suggests that the despotic patterns of female rhesus monkey culture have a differentiated effect on their rank physiology. Likely, less despotism of female rhesus monkey culture leads to an increased physiological stress for low ranking monkeys, which, in turn, increased release of cortisol by an excessive activation of the HPA axis.

    Age may be an important factor that affects social rank and cortisol of animals, but in our study, age had no effect on the social rank and cortisol levels of female monkeys. On the whole, these findings are in line with hypothesis that variations in the strictness of dominance style are the determining factors in rank physiology(Sapolsky, 2005).

    A hierarchy is meaningful to the animals within it because it can affect rank-related physiology. The formation of a dominance hierarchy is not only predetermined by differences in the attributes of animals,but is also produced by the dynamics of the social interactions (Chase et al, 2002). Hierarchical structures vary among species. For example, dominance hierarchies in nonhuman primates can be linear (i.e., A>B>C>D) or circular (i.e., A>B>C>A) (Li et al, 2004). In this study, H values were used to calculate the linearity of hierarchies(Singh et al, 1992; Singh et al, 2003). Generally, H values ranging from 0.90 to 1.00 indicate a perfect linear order (Li et al, 2004). The low ranking animals in groups with H values less than 0.90 were found to experience more physiological stressors (as assessed by hair cortisol levels) than higher ranking animals. This phenomena probably occurs because low ranking females in the less stringent groups potentially lack predictive information and/or a sense of control in agonistic encounters with their counterparts, thereby contributing to the physiological stressors generated from such encounters.Moreover, subordinate females have relatively few coping outlets (e.g., being able to displace aggressive frustration onto other females with lower ranking) to alleviate the stress, and this stress may be attributable to the relatively unstable hierarchies (Sapolsky, 2004a).

    Although extensive research in nonhuman primates has shown that subordinates are more likely to exhibit higher levels of cortisol than dominants, including researches in female cynomolgus monkeys (Adams et al,1985), male squirrel monkeys (Manogue et al, 1975),male olive baboons (Sapolsky, 1990), female talapoin monkeys (Keverne et al, 1982), and male mouse lemurs(Schilling & Perret, 1987), a number of other studies have not found differences in cortisol levels between subordinates and dominants, including studies in female squirrel monkeys (Mendoza & Mason, 1991), male rhesus monkeys (Bercovitch & Clarke, 1995) and female talapoin monkeys (Yodyingyuad et al, 1985). In fact,other investigations have even reported higher cortisol levels in dominants than in subordinate common female marmosets (Saltzman et al, 1998), and in subordinate male and female cotton top tamarins (Snowdon et al,1985).

    These nonhuman primate studies mentioned above highlight a great inconsistency between cortisol and rank in a number of species. The inconsistent results are partly due to species variation, but also due to different methodologies. Previous studies utilized plasma or urine samples to measure cortisol levels. Blood sampling requires stressful procedures such as the capture,restraint or venipuncture of the animals, can elevate circulating cortisol levels (Vining et al, 1983), somewhat altering the situation they were intending to measure.Similarly, a shortcoming of urine samples is that they are easily contaminated because of uncontrolled urine excretion. Furthermore, measured concentrations of cortisol in both these media are subject to circadian effects. More importantly, however, these cortisol levels only reflect short-term stress that occur over hours to days, and without the repeated sampling of animals, they cannot assess chronic stress levels that occur over weeks to months long periods (Keay et al, 2006; Owen et al,2005). Cortisol measured in hair, meanwhile, has been considered a biomarker of chronic stress and has proved useful in two studies of rhesus macaques in which hair samples were obtained to assess long-term cortisol and stress levels (Davenport et al, 2006; Feng et al, 2011). To our knowledge, this is the first study using cortisol measurements from hair sampling to evaluate the relationship between social rank and long-term stress in female rhesus monkeys, offering novel insight into longterm stress that arises from social.

    Though the realities of nonhuman primate social systems are rather simpler than those of human social systems, humans are similarly considered vulnerable to psychosocial stress stemming from their rank in the socioeconomic status (SES) hierarchy (Brunner, 1997;Sapolsky, 2005). Low SES individuals generally experience worse health outcomes than individuals ranked above them. Potential health related outcomes include an increased risks of cardiovascular disease, rheumatoid arthritis, respiratory, respiratory, reproductive, immune system and psychiatric diseases, as well as an overall increased mortality risk (Adler et al, 2000; Kawachi &Kennedy, 2006; Siegrist & Marmot, 2004; Wilkinson,2001). Similarly, long-term relationships of dominance and subordination stressors generate stressors associated with negative health sequelae in nonhuman primates,such as an increased cardiovascular disease risk (Manuck et al, 1995; Willard & Shively, 2011), depressive behaviors (Shively et al, 2005), HPA axis perturbations(Shively, 1998; Shively et al, 1997), ovarian dysfunction(Shively et al, 1997), reduced hippocampal volume(Willard & Shively, 2011), decreased dopamine-2 receptor function (Shively, 1998), and altered serotonergic function (Willard & Shively, 2011).

    Taken together, researches on both human and nonhuman primates both support a role for physiological stress in a SES-related health gradient. As such, there is considerable interest in the development of ranking related stress models to aid the understanding of human SES-related diseases as well as guide the development of new therapeutics (Shively et al, 2005; Willard & Shively,2011).

    Abbott D, Keverne E, Bercovitch F, Shively CA, Mendoza SP,Saltzman W, Snowdon CT, Ziegler TE, Banjevic M, Garland T,Sapolsky RM. 2003. Are subordinates always stressed? A comparative analysis of rank differences in cortisol levels among primates.Hormones and Behavior, 43(1): 67-82.

    Adams MR, Kaplan JR, Koritnik DR. 1985. Psychosocial influences on ovarian endocrine and ovulatory function in Macaca fascicularis.Physiology & Behavior, 35(6): 935-940.

    Ader R, Cohen N. 2001. Conditioning and immunity. // Ader R, Felten D, Vohen N, eds. Psychoneuroimmunology. New York: Scademic Press,2: 3-34.

    Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR. 2000. Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy, White women.Health Psychology, 19(6): 586-532.

    Altmann J. 1974. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods.Behaviour, 49(3-4): 227-267.

    Beerda B, Schilder MBH, Janssen NS, Mol JS. 1996. The use of saliva cortisol, urinary cortisol, and catecholamine measurements for a noninvasive assessment of stress responses in dogs. Hormones and Behavior, 30(3): 272-279.

    Bercovitch FB, Clarke AS. 1995. Dominance rank, cortisol concentrations, and reproductive maturation in male rhesus macaques.Physiology & Behavior, 58(2): 215-221.

    Brunner E. 1997. Socioeconomic determinants of health: stress and the biology of inequality. BMJ, 314(7092): 1472.

    Burch WM. 1982. Urine free-cortisol determination-a useful tool in the management of chronic hypoadrenal states. JAMA: the Journal of the American Medical Association, 247(14): 2002-2004.

    Cattet M, Christison K, Caulkett NA, Stenhouse GB. 2003. Physiologic responses of grizzly bears to different methods of capture. Journal of Wildlife Diseases, 39(3): 649-654.

    Cavogelli S A. 1999. Behavioural patterns associated with faecal cortisol levels in free-ranging female ring-tailed lemurs, Lemur catta.Animal Behaviour, 57(4): 935-944.

    Chamove AS, Bowman RE. 1976. Rank, rhesus social behavior, and stress. Folia Primatologica, 26(1): 57-66.

    Chase ID, Tovey C, Spangler-Martin D, Manfredonia M. 2002.Individual differences versus social dynamics in the formation of animal dominance hierarchies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(8): 5744-5749.

    Coe CL, Mendoza SP, Levine S. 1979. Social status constrains the stress response in the squirrel monkey. Physiology & Behavior, 23(4):633-638.

    Cohen S, Line S, Manuck SB, Rabin BS, Heise ER, Kaplan JR. 1997.Chronic social stress, social status, and susceptibility to upper respiratory infections in nonhuman primates. Psychosomatic Medicine,59(3): 213-221.

    Constable S, Parslow A, Dutton G, Rogers T, Hogg C. 2006. Urinary cortisol sampling: a non‐ invasive technique for examining cortisol concentrations in the Weddell seal, Leptonychotes weddellii. Zoo Biology, 25(2): 137-144.

    Davenport MD, Tiefenbacher S, Lutz CK, Novak MA, Meyer JS. 2006.

    Analysis of endogenous cortisol concentrations in the hair of rhesus macaques. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 147(3): 255-261.Dhabhar FS, Mcewen BS. 1999. Enhancing versus suppressive effects of stress hormones on skin immune function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96(3): 1059-1064.

    Ekkel ED, Dieleman SJ, Schouten WGP, Portela A, Cornélissen G,Tielen MJ, Halberg F. 1996. The circadian rhythm of cortisol in the saliva of young pigs. Physiology & Behavior, 60(3): 985-989.

    Feng X, Wang L, Yang S, Qin D, Wang J, Li C, Lü L, Ma Y, Hu X.2011. Maternal separation produces lasting changes in cortisol and behavior in rhesus monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(34): 14312-14317.

    Gammell MP, De Vries H, Jennings DJ, Carlin CM, Hayden TJ. 2003.David's score: a more appropriate dominance ranking method than Clutton-Brock et al. 's index. Animal Behaviour, 66(3): 601-605.

    Gatti R, Antonelli G, Prearo M, Spinella P, Cappellin E, De Palo EF.2009. Cortisol assays and diagnostic laboratory procedures in human biological fluids. Clinical Biochemistry, 42(12): 1205-1217.

    Gow R, Thomson S, Rieder M, Van Unm S, Koren G. 2010. An assessment of cortisol analysis in hair and its clinical applications.Forensic Science International, 196(1-3): 32-37.

    Gust DA, Gordon TP, Hambright MK, Wilson ME. 1993. Relationship between social factors and pituitary-adrenocortical activity in female rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Hormones and Behavior, 27(3):318-331.

    Gust DA, Gordon TP, Wilson ME, Ahmed-Ansari A, Brodie AR,McClure HM. 1991. Formation of a new social group of unfamiliar female rhesus monkeys affects the immune and pituitary adrenocortical systems. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 5(3): 296-307.

    Kawachi I, Kennedy BP. 2006. The Health of Nations: Why Inequality is Harmful to Your Health. New York: New Press.

    Keay JM, Singh J, Gaunt MC, Kaur T. 2006. Fecal glucocorticoids and their metabolites as indicators of stress in various mammalian species:a literature review. Journal of Zoo and Wildlife Medicine, 37(3): 234-244.

    Keverne EB, Meller RE, Eberhart A. 1982. Dominance and subordination: concepts or physiological states. // Chiarelli AB,Corruccini RS. Advanced Views in Primate Biology. Berlin: Springer-Verlag: 81-94.

    Li HQ, Zhang YH, Li BG. 2004. Review on dominance hierarchy of Non-human Primates. Acta Theriologica Sinica, 24(1): 53-60 (in Chinese).

    Manogue KR, Leshner AI, Candland DK. 1975. Dominance status and adrenocortical reactivity to stress in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Primates, 16(4): 457-463.

    Manuck SB, Marsland AL, Kaplan JR, Williams JK. 1995. The pathogenicity of behavior and its neuroendocrine mediation: An example from coronary artery disease. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57(3):275-283.

    Mcewen BS, Lasley EN. 2004. The End of Stress as We Know It.Washington: Joseph Henry Press.

    Mendoza SP, Mason WA. 1991. Breeding readiness in squirrel monkeys: female-primed females are triggered by males. Physiology &Behavior, 49(3): 471-479.

    Mendoza SP, Coe CL, Lowe EL, Levine S. 1978. The physiological response to group formation in adult male squirrel monkeys.Psychoneuroendocrinology, 3(3-4): 221-229.

    Millspaugh JJ, Washburn BE, Milanick MA, Beringer J, Hansen LP,MeyerTM. 2002. Non-invasive techniques for stress assessment in white-tailed deer. Wildlife Society Bulletin, 30(3): 899-907.

    Mormède P, Andanson S, Aupérin B, Beerdad B, Guémenée D,Malmkvistf J, Mantecag X, Manteuffelh G, Prunetc P, van Reenend CG,Richarde S, Veissierb I. 2007. Exploration of the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal function as a tool to evaluate animal welfare.Physiology & Behavior, 92(3): 317-339.

    Owen MA, Czekala NM, Swaisgood RR, Steinman K, Lindburg DG.2005. Seasonal and diurnal dynamics of glucocorticoids and behavior in giant pandas. Ursus, 16(2): 208-221.

    Russell E, Koren G, Rieder M, Van Uum S. 2011. Hair cortisol as a biological marker of chronic stress: Current status, future directions and unanswered questions. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(5): 589-601.

    Saltzman W, Schultz-Darken NJ, Scheffler G, Wegner FH, Abbott DH.1994. Social and reproductive influences on plasma cortisol in female marmoset monkeys. Physiology & Behavior, 56(4): 801-810.

    Saltzman W, Schultz-Darken NJ, Wegner FH, Wittwer DJ, Abbott DH.1998. Suppression of cortisol levels in subordinate female marmosets:reproductive and social contributions. Hormones and Behavior, 33(1):58-74.

    Sapolsky RM. 1990. Adrenocortical function, social rank, and personality among wild baboons. Biological Psychiatry, 28(10): 862-878.

    Sapolsky RM. 2004a. Social status and health in humans and other animals. Annual Review of Anthropology, 33: 393-418.

    Sapolsky RM. 2004b. Why Zebras Don't Get Ulcers: The Acclaimed Guide to Stress, Stress-Related Diseases, and Coping-Now Revised and Updated. New York: Holt Paperbacks.

    Sapolsky RM. 2005. The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science, 308(5722): 648-652.

    Sapolsky RM, Share L J. 1994. Rank‐ related differences in cardiovascular function among wild baboons: Role of sensitivity to glucocorticoids. American Journal of Primatology, 32(4): 261-275.

    Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck AU. 2000. How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive,stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine Reviews, 21(1): 55-89.Schilling A, Perret M. 1987. Chemical signals and reproductive capacity in a male prosimian primate (Microcebus murinus). Chemical Senses, 12(1): 143-157.

    Shively CA. 1998. Social subordination stress, behavior, and central monoaminergic function in female cynomolgus monkeys. Biological Psychiatry, 44(9): 882-891.

    Shively CA, Clarkson TB. 1994. Social status and coronary artery atherosclerosis in female monkeys. Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis, and Vascular Biology, 14(5): 721-726.

    Shively CA, Laber-Laird K, Anton RF. 1997. Behavior and physiology of social stress and depression in female cynomolgus monkeys.Biological Psychiatry, 41(8): 871-882.

    Shively CA, Register TC, Friedman DP, Morgan TM, Thompson J,Lanier T. 2005. Social stress-associated depression in adult female cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). Biological Psychology,69(1): 67-84.

    Siegrist J, Marmot M. 2004. Health inequalities and the psychosocial environment--two scientific challenges. Social Science & Medicine,58(8): 1463-1473.

    Singh M, D'souza L, Singh M. 1992. Hierarchy, kinship and social interaction among Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Journal of Biosciences, 17(1): 15-27.

    Singh M, Sharma A, Krishna B. 2003. Methodological considerations in measurement of dominance in primates. Current Science, 84(5): 709-713.

    Snowdon CT, Savage A, Mcconnell PB. 1985. A breeding colony of cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Laboratory Animal Science,35(5): 477-480.

    Steklis HD, Raleigh MJ, Kling AS, Tachiki K. 1986. Biochemical and hormonal correlates of dominance and social behavior in all male ‐groups of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). American Journal of Primatology, 11(2): 133-145.

    Van Schaik C, Van Noordwijk M, Van Bragt T, Blankenstein MA. 1991.A pilot study of the social correlates of levels of urinary cortisol,prolactin, and testosterone in wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Primates, 32(3): 345-356.

    Vining RF, Mcginley RA, Maksvytis JJ, Ho KY. 1983. Salivary cortisol:a better measure of adrenal cortical function than serum cortisol.Annals of Clinical Biochemistry, 20(P6): 329-335.

    Von Der Ohe CG, Servheen C. 2002. Measuring stress in mammals using fecal glucocorticoids: opportunities and challenges. Wildlife Society Bulletin, 30(4): 1215-1225.

    Wallner B, M?stl E, Dittami J, Prossinger H. 1999. Fecal glucocorticoids document stress in female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). General and Comparative Endocrinology, 113(1): 80-86.

    Weingrill T, Gray DA, Barrett L, Henzi SP. 2004. Fecal cortisol levels in free-ranging female chacma baboons: relationship to dominance,reproductive state and environmental factors. Hormones and Behavior,45(4): 259-269.

    Wennig R. 2000. Potential problems with the interpretation of hair analysis results. Forensic Science International, 107(1-3): 5-12.

    Whitten PL, Stavisky R, Aureli F, Rusell E. 1999. Response of fecal cortisol to stress in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology, 44(1): 57-69.

    Wilkinson RG. 2001. Mind the Gap: Hierarchies, Health and Human Evolution. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Willard SL, Shively CA. 2011. Modeling depression in adult female cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology, 74(6): 528-542.

    Yodyingyuad U, De La Riva C, Abbott D, Herbert J, Keverne EB. 1985.Relationship between dominance hierarchy, cerebrospinal fluid levels of amine transmitter metabolites (5-hydroxyindole acetic acid and homovanillic acid) and plasma cortisol in monkeys. Neuroscience,16(4): 851-858.

    Zhao HT, Zhang J, Zhu ZR, Li BG, Wang XW. 2011. The methods of research on the dominance of female in non-human primates. Acta Anthropologica Sinica, 30(4): 415-424 (in Chinese).

    又粗又硬又长又爽又黄的视频| 国产一区有黄有色的免费视频| 婷婷色av中文字幕| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区国产| 性色avwww在线观看| 18禁在线无遮挡免费观看视频| 国精品久久久久久国模美| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 视频中文字幕在线观看| 久久97久久精品| 高清不卡的av网站| 国产精品久久久久久av不卡| 好男人视频免费观看在线| 午夜老司机福利剧场| 大片免费播放器 马上看| 美女福利国产在线| 在线观看免费高清a一片| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 亚洲精品久久午夜乱码| 一级毛片我不卡| 国产精品一区www在线观看| 国产69精品久久久久777片| 建设人人有责人人尽责人人享有的| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 亚洲精品456在线播放app| 香蕉精品网在线| 国产欧美日韩一区二区三区在线 | 久久午夜福利片| 99久国产av精品国产电影| 免费黄色在线免费观看| 久久精品久久久久久久性| 亚洲精品国产av蜜桃| 久久久国产欧美日韩av| 80岁老熟妇乱子伦牲交| 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 九九在线视频观看精品| 成人毛片a级毛片在线播放| 亚洲真实伦在线观看| 久久精品久久久久久噜噜老黄| 免费不卡的大黄色大毛片视频在线观看| 精品久久久久久久久av| 嫩草影院新地址| 插阴视频在线观看视频| 97精品久久久久久久久久精品| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| 亚洲三级黄色毛片| 免费看不卡的av| kizo精华| 国产成人freesex在线| 欧美老熟妇乱子伦牲交| 精品一品国产午夜福利视频| 久久国产精品男人的天堂亚洲 | 天美传媒精品一区二区| 高清不卡的av网站| 毛片一级片免费看久久久久| 男男h啪啪无遮挡| 好男人视频免费观看在线| 老司机亚洲免费影院| 亚洲欧美清纯卡通| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 韩国高清视频一区二区三区| 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区 | 91午夜精品亚洲一区二区三区| 亚洲欧洲日产国产| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| 精品少妇黑人巨大在线播放| 免费播放大片免费观看视频在线观看| 啦啦啦视频在线资源免费观看| 中文字幕制服av| 成年人免费黄色播放视频 | 少妇人妻精品综合一区二区| 99国产精品免费福利视频| 91aial.com中文字幕在线观看| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 国产精品国产三级国产专区5o| 欧美老熟妇乱子伦牲交| 久久久久精品久久久久真实原创| 春色校园在线视频观看| 成人午夜精彩视频在线观看| 男女边摸边吃奶| 伦理电影免费视频| 如何舔出高潮| 亚洲丝袜综合中文字幕| 久久久国产欧美日韩av| 国产深夜福利视频在线观看| 国产在线男女| 黄色配什么色好看| 欧美精品人与动牲交sv欧美| 哪个播放器可以免费观看大片| 各种免费的搞黄视频| 天堂8中文在线网| av不卡在线播放| 在线亚洲精品国产二区图片欧美 | kizo精华| 欧美日韩一区二区视频在线观看视频在线| 亚洲欧美精品专区久久| 亚州av有码| 久久精品久久精品一区二区三区| 在线 av 中文字幕| 九九爱精品视频在线观看| 另类亚洲欧美激情| 高清午夜精品一区二区三区| 国产精品成人在线| 国产精品人妻久久久影院| 最近中文字幕高清免费大全6| 国产片特级美女逼逼视频| 午夜精品国产一区二区电影| 免费人成在线观看视频色| av福利片在线观看| av在线观看视频网站免费| 十分钟在线观看高清视频www | 久久久久久久国产电影| 久久毛片免费看一区二区三区| 国产精品一区二区在线观看99| 日韩av不卡免费在线播放| 国产精品秋霞免费鲁丝片| 一级毛片电影观看| 麻豆成人午夜福利视频| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 免费看不卡的av| 日本-黄色视频高清免费观看| av免费观看日本| 黑人猛操日本美女一级片| 亚洲va在线va天堂va国产| 国产精品久久久久久精品古装| 久久狼人影院| 人人妻人人澡人人爽人人夜夜| 免费高清在线观看视频在线观看| 黑人高潮一二区| 精品卡一卡二卡四卡免费| 超碰97精品在线观看| 日韩精品免费视频一区二区三区 | 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 色视频www国产| 精品一区二区三卡| av福利片在线观看| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 国产男人的电影天堂91| 久久 成人 亚洲| 国产乱来视频区| 国产成人精品一,二区| 成人亚洲精品一区在线观看| av播播在线观看一区| 国产精品免费大片| 国语对白做爰xxxⅹ性视频网站| 嘟嘟电影网在线观看| a级一级毛片免费在线观看| 九九在线视频观看精品| 婷婷色综合大香蕉| 91久久精品国产一区二区三区| 中文字幕精品免费在线观看视频 | 久久久精品94久久精品| 美女内射精品一级片tv| 美女视频免费永久观看网站| 国产在线男女| 永久免费av网站大全| 桃花免费在线播放| 热re99久久国产66热| 欧美3d第一页| 久久毛片免费看一区二区三区| 寂寞人妻少妇视频99o| 美女国产视频在线观看| 国精品久久久久久国模美| 性色av一级| 久久久精品免费免费高清| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 日韩伦理黄色片| 日本av手机在线免费观看| 狂野欧美激情性bbbbbb| 日本91视频免费播放| 成人免费观看视频高清| 精华霜和精华液先用哪个| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 亚洲精品成人av观看孕妇| 一级爰片在线观看| 精品国产露脸久久av麻豆| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 青春草亚洲视频在线观看| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 欧美成人精品欧美一级黄| 国产毛片在线视频| 国产黄频视频在线观看| 国产亚洲午夜精品一区二区久久| 日本欧美国产在线视频| 免费黄频网站在线观看国产| 岛国毛片在线播放| 街头女战士在线观看网站| 欧美少妇被猛烈插入视频| 男人爽女人下面视频在线观看| 一个人免费看片子| 美女脱内裤让男人舔精品视频| 五月伊人婷婷丁香| 人人妻人人添人人爽欧美一区卜| 免费黄色在线免费观看| 国产av一区二区精品久久| 国产成人精品久久久久久| 一级毛片 在线播放| 国模一区二区三区四区视频| 久久久久久人妻| www.av在线官网国产| 国产精品福利在线免费观看| 亚洲欧洲精品一区二区精品久久久 | 国产精品熟女久久久久浪| 久久精品国产a三级三级三级| 国产成人精品福利久久| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 精品少妇久久久久久888优播| 人妻系列 视频| 97超碰精品成人国产| 亚洲精品自拍成人| 国产精品欧美亚洲77777| 久久99热这里只频精品6学生| 男女国产视频网站| 春色校园在线视频观看| 亚洲精品一区蜜桃| 黄色配什么色好看| 草草在线视频免费看| 久久99热这里只频精品6学生| 亚洲精品中文字幕在线视频 | 国产黄频视频在线观看| 成年人午夜在线观看视频| 看免费成人av毛片| 久久午夜综合久久蜜桃| 国产淫语在线视频| www.色视频.com| 久久人人爽人人片av| 免费av不卡在线播放| 精品一区在线观看国产| av黄色大香蕉| 少妇丰满av| 国产美女午夜福利| 全区人妻精品视频| 少妇猛男粗大的猛烈进出视频| 人妻 亚洲 视频| 精品国产一区二区久久| 成人二区视频| 肉色欧美久久久久久久蜜桃| xxx大片免费视频| a级毛色黄片| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 国产综合精华液| 久久国产精品男人的天堂亚洲 | 亚洲真实伦在线观看| 亚洲欧美清纯卡通| .国产精品久久| 丰满迷人的少妇在线观看| 久久久a久久爽久久v久久| 成人黄色视频免费在线看| 男人爽女人下面视频在线观看| 伦理电影大哥的女人| √禁漫天堂资源中文www| 欧美成人午夜免费资源| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 国产成人aa在线观看| 欧美区成人在线视频| 国产综合精华液| 日韩精品免费视频一区二区三区 | 国产高清不卡午夜福利| 一二三四中文在线观看免费高清| 欧美人与善性xxx| 久久久国产一区二区| 欧美成人午夜免费资源| 看非洲黑人一级黄片| 欧美日韩av久久| 国产黄片美女视频| 亚洲色图综合在线观看| 麻豆成人av视频| 2018国产大陆天天弄谢| 丝袜在线中文字幕| 国产精品99久久久久久久久| 在线看a的网站| 久久久久久久久大av| 精品少妇内射三级| 国产精品蜜桃在线观看| 久久国产精品大桥未久av | 国产视频内射| 免费看光身美女| 国产伦精品一区二区三区视频9| 99九九在线精品视频 | 色婷婷av一区二区三区视频| 亚洲综合精品二区| 男人添女人高潮全过程视频| 国产精品国产三级专区第一集| 免费观看性生交大片5| 丁香六月天网| 久久99一区二区三区| 亚洲精品国产色婷婷电影| 亚洲精品中文字幕在线视频 | 精品一区二区免费观看| 婷婷色综合www| 亚洲人成网站在线观看播放| 日本色播在线视频| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| h视频一区二区三区| 这个男人来自地球电影免费观看 | 97在线视频观看| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| 99热网站在线观看| 麻豆成人午夜福利视频| 国产毛片在线视频| 亚洲欧美成人综合另类久久久| 亚洲va在线va天堂va国产| 午夜免费观看性视频| 日本av免费视频播放| 午夜福利视频精品| 精品久久久噜噜| 最新的欧美精品一区二区| 久久精品久久精品一区二区三区| 国产日韩欧美亚洲二区| 成人毛片a级毛片在线播放| 搡老乐熟女国产| 亚洲国产精品国产精品| 欧美精品一区二区大全| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图| 久久久久久久国产电影| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区黑人 | 超碰97精品在线观看| 国产极品粉嫩免费观看在线 | 亚洲精品国产成人久久av| 亚洲国产欧美日韩在线播放 | 日韩免费高清中文字幕av| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| 国产黄频视频在线观看| 一区二区三区免费毛片| 久久婷婷青草| av福利片在线| 国产成人a∨麻豆精品| 日本欧美视频一区| 中文欧美无线码| 久久午夜福利片| 国产av一区二区精品久久| 3wmmmm亚洲av在线观看| 日本欧美国产在线视频| 亚洲国产精品999| 少妇人妻久久综合中文| 亚洲欧美中文字幕日韩二区| 内射极品少妇av片p| av免费在线看不卡| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 成人免费观看视频高清| 国产欧美另类精品又又久久亚洲欧美| 国产日韩欧美视频二区| 国产在线免费精品| 久久人人爽人人爽人人片va| 日韩 亚洲 欧美在线| 日本黄色日本黄色录像| 亚洲av福利一区| 亚洲成人一二三区av| 中文字幕人妻丝袜制服| av天堂久久9| 新久久久久国产一级毛片| 日韩人妻高清精品专区| 日日爽夜夜爽网站| 一级毛片aaaaaa免费看小| 午夜av观看不卡| 色婷婷久久久亚洲欧美| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| xxx大片免费视频| 成人二区视频| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区黑人 | 99九九在线精品视频 | 亚洲精品国产成人久久av| 黑人巨大精品欧美一区二区蜜桃 | 纯流量卡能插随身wifi吗| 制服丝袜香蕉在线| 日本av手机在线免费观看| 欧美97在线视频| 婷婷色综合大香蕉| 亚洲精品日本国产第一区| 在线精品无人区一区二区三| 国产一区亚洲一区在线观看| 麻豆精品久久久久久蜜桃| 欧美最新免费一区二区三区| av一本久久久久| 高清在线视频一区二区三区| 哪个播放器可以免费观看大片| 日韩免费高清中文字幕av| 亚洲精品一二三| 久久久久久久久大av| 亚洲人与动物交配视频| 黄色一级大片看看| 精品久久国产蜜桃| av在线app专区| 日本与韩国留学比较| 99久久精品国产国产毛片| 国产探花极品一区二区| xxx大片免费视频| 18禁动态无遮挡网站| 寂寞人妻少妇视频99o| 亚洲欧美日韩卡通动漫| 99久久精品一区二区三区| 搡女人真爽免费视频火全软件| 亚洲欧美日韩另类电影网站| 欧美日韩一区二区视频在线观看视频在线| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 久久国产精品男人的天堂亚洲 | 91精品一卡2卡3卡4卡| 桃花免费在线播放| 久热久热在线精品观看| 91久久精品电影网| 精品少妇内射三级| 国产伦理片在线播放av一区| 2018国产大陆天天弄谢| 久久精品国产a三级三级三级| 国产精品福利在线免费观看| 久久久久网色| 波野结衣二区三区在线| 丰满迷人的少妇在线观看| 久久国产精品男人的天堂亚洲 | 99re6热这里在线精品视频| 大码成人一级视频| 亚洲美女搞黄在线观看| 亚洲真实伦在线观看| 国产 精品1| 久久久久久久久久人人人人人人| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 久久婷婷青草| 色吧在线观看| 美女视频免费永久观看网站| 天天躁夜夜躁狠狠久久av| 精品久久久精品久久久| 国产欧美亚洲国产| 激情五月婷婷亚洲| 在线观看一区二区三区激情| 99热国产这里只有精品6| 三级国产精品片| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 欧美日韩国产mv在线观看视频| 不卡视频在线观看欧美| 亚洲内射少妇av| videos熟女内射| 国产极品粉嫩免费观看在线 | 日本欧美国产在线视频| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 综合色丁香网| 赤兔流量卡办理| 大码成人一级视频| 97在线人人人人妻| 99热国产这里只有精品6| 色视频www国产| a级毛片在线看网站| 最近2019中文字幕mv第一页| 亚洲无线观看免费| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 好男人视频免费观看在线| 亚洲在久久综合| av天堂久久9| 国产成人一区二区在线| 热re99久久国产66热| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 日韩 亚洲 欧美在线| 欧美精品一区二区免费开放| 自线自在国产av| 日本vs欧美在线观看视频 | 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 人妻少妇偷人精品九色| 熟女电影av网| 国产精品久久久久久av不卡| 久久av网站| 免费av中文字幕在线| 大又大粗又爽又黄少妇毛片口| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 日本黄色日本黄色录像| 99久久精品国产国产毛片| 日韩av在线免费看完整版不卡| 免费看日本二区| 久久久久久伊人网av| 精品一品国产午夜福利视频| 精品少妇久久久久久888优播| 九草在线视频观看| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 久久av网站| 成人影院久久| 精品一品国产午夜福利视频| av又黄又爽大尺度在线免费看| 精品亚洲成a人片在线观看| 人人妻人人澡人人看| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| 亚洲精品日韩av片在线观看| 另类精品久久| freevideosex欧美| 免费黄色在线免费观看| 天天躁夜夜躁狠狠久久av| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 99精国产麻豆久久婷婷| 热re99久久国产66热| 99精国产麻豆久久婷婷| 精品少妇内射三级| 性高湖久久久久久久久免费观看| 久久精品久久精品一区二区三区| 久久久久久久久久成人| 99九九线精品视频在线观看视频| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看| √禁漫天堂资源中文www| 秋霞伦理黄片| 97在线视频观看| 精品一品国产午夜福利视频| 一级片'在线观看视频| 夜夜爽夜夜爽视频| 中文精品一卡2卡3卡4更新| 免费黄频网站在线观看国产| 美女国产视频在线观看| 伊人久久国产一区二区| 亚洲国产精品专区欧美| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 国产伦精品一区二区三区视频9| 自线自在国产av| 欧美日韩视频精品一区| 少妇人妻精品综合一区二区| 一级,二级,三级黄色视频| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 91午夜精品亚洲一区二区三区| 久久精品夜色国产| 这个男人来自地球电影免费观看 | 多毛熟女@视频| 精品视频人人做人人爽| 日韩av免费高清视频| 大又大粗又爽又黄少妇毛片口| 国产男人的电影天堂91| 秋霞伦理黄片| 久久久久人妻精品一区果冻| 少妇的逼水好多| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 午夜福利,免费看| 人人妻人人添人人爽欧美一区卜| 亚洲性久久影院| 天美传媒精品一区二区| 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看 | 日韩一本色道免费dvd| 国产精品一区www在线观看| 男女啪啪激烈高潮av片| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产| 日韩av在线免费看完整版不卡| 秋霞在线观看毛片| 亚洲欧美精品专区久久| 美女视频免费永久观看网站| 免费播放大片免费观看视频在线观看| 亚洲精品一区蜜桃| 成人国产av品久久久| 国产精品秋霞免费鲁丝片| 新久久久久国产一级毛片| 国产精品人妻久久久影院| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 国产精品欧美亚洲77777| a级片在线免费高清观看视频| 久久午夜综合久久蜜桃| 免费看光身美女| 99久久综合免费| 天堂中文最新版在线下载| 男人爽女人下面视频在线观看| 日韩人妻高清精品专区| 国产视频内射| 日韩伦理黄色片| 乱码一卡2卡4卡精品| 一级片'在线观看视频| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 黄色配什么色好看| 久久精品久久精品一区二区三区| 三级经典国产精品| 我要看黄色一级片免费的| 美女大奶头黄色视频| 韩国高清视频一区二区三区| 又大又黄又爽视频免费| 涩涩av久久男人的天堂| 亚洲国产欧美在线一区| 午夜视频国产福利| 人人澡人人妻人| 女性生殖器流出的白浆| 午夜福利影视在线免费观看| 永久免费av网站大全| 99热6这里只有精品| 午夜av观看不卡| 国产深夜福利视频在线观看| 日韩三级伦理在线观看| 国产伦精品一区二区三区四那| 国产日韩一区二区三区精品不卡 | 久久久精品免费免费高清| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 女性生殖器流出的白浆| 丰满少妇做爰视频| 亚洲av日韩在线播放| 亚洲精品成人av观看孕妇| av免费观看日本| av黄色大香蕉| 一级a做视频免费观看| 亚洲国产精品999| 在线观看免费高清a一片| 精品国产露脸久久av麻豆| 丝袜脚勾引网站| 亚洲美女黄色视频免费看| 亚洲精品国产av蜜桃| 18禁裸乳无遮挡动漫免费视频| 久久久精品免费免费高清| 啦啦啦中文免费视频观看日本| 欧美日韩综合久久久久久| 亚洲国产色片| 免费人妻精品一区二区三区视频| 中文字幕亚洲精品专区| 日日撸夜夜添| 人妻 亚洲 视频| 一区二区三区四区激情视频| 在线天堂最新版资源| 久久久久精品性色| 少妇人妻一区二区三区视频| 亚洲精品一区蜜桃| 免费久久久久久久精品成人欧美视频 | 夜夜看夜夜爽夜夜摸| 99热6这里只有精品| 免费播放大片免费观看视频在线观看| 中文字幕久久专区| 视频中文字幕在线观看| 免费av不卡在线播放| 亚洲国产色片|