• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Multileveled Engagement: A Cognitive Empirical Approach to Crossover Picturebooks

    2021-01-19 10:48:36XiaofeiShi
    Language and Semiotic Studies 2020年4期

    Xiaofei Shi

    Soochow University, China

    Abstract Crossover picturebooks attract readers of different ages. The trend of refocusing on the body in children’s literature criticism, especially under the influence of literary cognitive studies as Maria Nikolajeva points out (2016), provides a context to reconfigure our understanding of crossover picturebooks. Informed by literary cognitive studies, particularly the theory of brain laterality, the project selects the picturebook that may have crossover potential to start with, then investigates the actual readers’ engagement with the potentially crossover picturebook, and finally feeds the insights gleaned from the empirical study back into the understanding of the selected text, and possibly crossover picturebooks generally. The project aims to learn about the crossover picturebook through exploring how it impacts readers. It was a small-scale case study, conducted in the east of England, UK. Colin Thompson’s How to Live Forever was used with two adults and two children. Observation and interviewing were used to collect data. Text-related creative activities served to support or clarify the oral response. The project reveals: the multileveled nature of crossover picturebooks that many scholars emphasize may correspond to the multileveled engagement that texts can elicit from readers, which is partially the outcome of cognitive-affective skills and mechanisms engaged differently across children and adults. This study also shows the crossover potential particular to picturebooks: picturebooks may use the visual to flesh out the embodied experience, which can springboard further thinking about abstract, complicated notions. This mode of representation corresponds to the way the human mind works, thus engaging children and adults alike.

    Keywords: crossover picturebook, cognitive literary studies, empirical research

    1. Introduction

    Crossover in children’s literature criticism refers to the phenomenon when a text appeals to readers of a wide age range (Beckett, 2009, 2012; Falconer, 2009). Since the “l(fā)andmark” Harry Potter series as Sandra Beckett labels it (2009, pp. 1-2), the recent years have seen a rise in the production of and research on crossover literature. The existing research has focused on crossover fiction (e.g. Beckett, 2009; Falconer, 2009) and crossover picturebooks (e.g. Beckett, 2012; Ommundsen, 2014). This article discusses crossover picturebooks because how picturebooks, traditionally connected with children, may attract both children and adults makes an especially interesting line of enquiry, though some issues wrestled with in this study may be relevant to crossover literature in a broader sense. The view of picturebooks as children’s prerogative has been put to question (Scott, 1999; Desmet, 2004; Beckett, 2012; Salisbury & Styles, 2012). Changes in picturebook tradition, especially in the Nordic countries, even result in picturebooks aimed at adults (Ommundsen, 2014). Therefore, the changes in picturebook production also call for the research on crossover picturebooks.

    The issue that concerns many researchers is what kind of picturebooks is crossover. Beckett (2012) defines crossover picturebooks as “multileveled works that are suitable for all ages” (p. 16). ?se Marie Ommundsen argues that crossover picturebooks are the picturebooks that evoke an implied child reader and an implied adult reader (2014, p. 72). Ommundsen bases her definition on Wolfgang Iser’s notion of the implied reader, which is the reader constructed by and inscribed in the text rather than the flesh-and-blood person that approaches the text (Iser, 1974). Mieke Desmet suggests that the na?ve dimension of the Taiwanese picturebook creator Jimmy Liao’s works may attract a child audience and their complex and “highly allusive” dimension may appeal more to an adult audience (2004, p. 71). She seems to treat adults’ way of reading as superior to children’s (p. 77). Without working with real readers, the abovementioned scholars focus on crossover more as the affordances of the text, that is, how the text may have the potential to attract readers of different ages. Moreover, embedded in their attempts to delineate crossover picturebooks is the issue of how the child is conceived in relation to the adult.

    Karín Lesnik-Oberstein emphasises the constructedness of the concept of the child (2011, pp. 1-17). Richard Mills reviews various perspectives on the constructed child, such as the child as innocent, vulnerable and so on, situating the perspectives within the relevant sociocultural contexts (2000). In response to the perception of the child as a social construction, children’s literature research has shown a material turn, refocusing on “the physicality of children’s bodies”, as Maria Nikolajeva observes (2016, p. 132). The trend of returning to physicality is reflected in a growing body of research that draws on cognitive literary studies, for instance, Nikolajeva’s Reading for Learning (2014) and the issue 6.2 of the journal International Research in Children’s Literature (2013) published by Edinburgh University Press. The concept of crossover literature is established based on mixed audiences it can attract, which are separated into different age groups. The age difference inevitably causes the difference in the body. Even when explaining the constructedness of the child, Mills recognises that children are distinct from adults in cognition (2000, p. 23), and human cognition relies on the material existence of the body. The trend of refocusing on the body in children’s literature criticism, especially under the influence of literary cognitive studies, provides a context to reconfigure our understanding of crossover literature.

    Literary cognitive studies, referring to experimental psychology, neuroscience and brain research, provide a cross-disciplinary approach to the issues of reading, literacy and literature (Stockwell, 2002, pp. 1-11). The brain is the cognitive-affective structure that, developing with age, grounds the way readers engage with fictional narratives and in general, the world. The brain encodes its history as a dynamic system, with the evolutionarily newer layer atop the older one (Crago, 2014, pp. 7-8). The evolutionarily most recent layer is the cerebral cortex, which is divided into the right hemisphere and the left hemisphere. The bihemispheric structure gives rise to two fundamentally different ways of seeing and experiencing the world. Human beings’ interaction with the world is synthesised from the workings of the two hemispheres that cooperate but more often conflict with each other (McGilchrist, 2012; Panksepp & Biven, 2012; Crago, 2014). Hugh Crago (2014) suggests that the development of the capacity for understanding fiction from infancy to old age relates to the continual changes in the brain. The theory of brain laterality connects the readers’ engagement with the text to the cognitive-affective structure of the brain. Such a connection is plausible because what human beings encounter in the world is delivered to and mediated through the brain, which ultimately sends the encounter back to the world. Though it is arguable whether readers at a certain age may prefer a particular type of story, as Crago claims, his study offers a perspective on exploring the child-adult reader differences as spread on a broad spectrum rather than in two binary categories. This perspective helps to take into account a wide range of readers of crossover literature that cannot be distinctly categorised as the adult or the child.

    Connecting the age difference to the difference in cognitive-affective skills and mechanisms does not mean returning to essentialism. As Nikolajeva argues, “This trend [of refocusing on physicality] does not signal a return to essentialism but reflects the complexity, plurality, and ambiguity of our understanding of childhood” (2016, p. 132). Moreover, the investigation of the cognitive-affective skills and mechanisms that presuppose fiction reading allows for the consideration of sociocultural factors. For instance, Nikolajeva suggests that readers draw on their real-life experience to understand fiction, including the experience of interpersonal relationships (2014, p. 16), which is subject to the influence of the sociocultural environment one is situated in.

    Despite the ongoing changes and possible individual differences, some cognitive functions remain qualitatively alike throughout various life stages and across human beings. For instance, the right hemisphere of the brain plays a dominant role in broad and flexible attention, while the world of the left hemisphere is more local and narrowly focused (McGilchrist, 2012, pp. 39-40). Anything new must first be directed to the right hemisphere before coming into focus for the left. Therefore, the right hemisphere is more engaged than the left in apprehending anything new, both verbal and visual (p. 40). The primacy of the right hemisphere in attending to the new forms part of the fundamental human existence. This accounts for scholars’ observation (e.g. Wolf, 2012, pp. 3-6; Nikolajeva, 2014, p. 27) that human beings, both child and adult, are by nature capable of engaging with an inventive fictional world very different from what they are used to in real life. The relative stability of some cognitive functions also provides evidence for the claim of crossover scholars, such as Maija-Liisa Harju (2009, pp. 365-366), Carole Scott (1999, p. 102) and Deborah Thacker (2000, p. 1), that a continuum of understanding and experience exists throughout various life stages.

    Literary cognitive studies explore not only textual affordances, but also how readers engage with texts. Aside from Beckett’s, Ommundsen’s, and Desmet’s accounts of the textual affordances of crossover, the other aspect is how actual readers engage with the text that may attract a wide range of readers. Adults are often seen as parents in shared picturebook readings rather than as readers on their own. The presumptions of adult picturebook readers not supported by empirical evidence may wrongly inform the way crossover picturebooks are conceived. Thus, an empirical study of actual child and adult readers is especially beneficial for exploring crossover picturebooks. Evelyn Arizpe and Morag Styles include literary cognitive studies in the newly emerging analytical tools and theoretical frameworks that have a huge potential for picturebook research (2016/2003, p. 143). Though empirical research of picturebooks has not yet largely applied cognitive literary studies, this theoretical perspective has informed research with actual young readers. For instance, Marie-Laure Ryan (2003), using the novella Chronicle of a Death Foretold with high school students, explores how the readers’ construction of narrative space is subject to the workings of cognitive mechanism, such as short-term memory. Ryan’s project shows the potential of literary cognitive studies for guiding and structuring empirical research.

    However, as Peter Stockwell suggests, literary cognitive studies fundamentally are a new way of thinking about literature (2002, pp. 1-11). Karen Coats (2013) provides an example of how to learn about texts through exploring the way they affect readers. She proposes to understand children’s poetry in terms of how it creates “a holding environment in language to help children manage their sensory environments, map and regulate their neurological functions, and contain their existential anxieties” (p. 140). The theoretical perspective of literary cognitive studies therefore offers a way to integrate the textual affordances of crossover with the actual readers’ responses to the textual affordances: examining the actual readers’ engagement is to shed new light on the texts. Rachel Falconer even contends that we can only arrive at a more comprehensive understanding of crossover literature through exploring what it does for readers (2009, p. 27).

    Informed by the perspective of literary cognitive studies, the project selects the picturebook that may have crossover potential to start with, then investigates the actual readers’ engagement with the potentially crossover picturebook, and finally feeds the insights gleaned from the empirical study back into the understanding of the selected text, and possibly crossover picturebooks in general. The selection and analysis of the primary text is guided by the existing studies of crossover picturebooks and the theory of brain laterality. The empirical part seeks to address the two questions: (1) How do the actual child and adult readers engage with the potentially crossover picturebook? (2) What can we learn about the selected picturebook as a crossover text and possibly crossover picturebooks generally in the light of the actual readers’ engagement?

    2. Selecting and Analyzing the Picturebook

    Colin Thompson is often mentioned in crossover studies (e.g. Scott, 1999). Nikolajeva and Scott point out that the abundance of pictorial details evoking rich connections is Thompson’s trademark (2006, p. 239). The representation of the fictional world epitomises Thompson’s unique style. Scott illustrates with the fictional world in Thompson’s Looking for Atlantis to explain how the dual narrative of picturebooks enhances crossover potential (1999, pp. 99-110). The construction of the fictional world is also a research focus in literary cognitive studies (Nikolajeva, 2014, pp. 21-47). This study uses Thompson’s less explored picturebook—How to Live Forever (1995), henceforth Forever. The focus of the text analysis is on the fictional world, and correspondingly, how the actual readers engage with the fictional world represented in Forever will be the focus in the empirical part.

    In Forever, Peter finds a record card of the book called “How to Live Forever”, but the book is missing from the shelf where it should have been. Determining to find it so that he will not grow old, Peter starts to look for the book. He meets four old men in an attic, and the men lead him to the ancient child, who has read the book “How to Live Forever,” and is forever frozen in time. After talking to the ancient child, Peter relinquishes the idea of reading the book and living forever.

    Forever represents a fictional world inventive in the fundamental ontological aspect. In the picturebook, a series of worlds is interlocked. These worlds can be divided into two main types. The first type is the mundane world. The second type is the miniature world of magic, such as the interior of the library populated by miniature creatures living in the books and under the floorboards. By embedding lighted doors and windows in the grand mundane world, the visual hints at the magical miniature worlds underneath. The multitudinous worlds are tightly packed with enigmatic visual details. Eyes peek out from behind the shelves, limbs seem to be alive on their own, and dilapidated odds and ends are lying around at every corner. The fictional world challenges the law of time readers are used to in the real world, as time forever freezes in the ancient child. As aforementioned, since the right hemisphere of the brain is naturally tuned to the new and unfamiliar, child and adult readers should be able to engage with the inventive fictional world represented in Forever.

    The multitudinous worlds show a string of changes. The interior of the book on the tenth spread is compartmentalised into lacquered shelves. The tight compartmentalisation creates an effect of fragmentation and restriction, resulting in the inability to see a larger picture. On the twelfth spread, the Chinese garden is depicted in regular blue and white patterns. Peter is the only colourful spot against the huge cold background. The picture counterpoints the verbal “bright butterflies” (my emphasis). The regular geometric shapes and cold colours give an unpleasant sense of stagnation. The fifteenth spread depicts a garden of lighted windows, flowing water, green trees, and a bright butterfly, implying a restoration of life. The pattern of the changes in the multitudinous worlds parallels the theme of the picturebook—going through immortality, Peter discovers that immortality equals death as immortality means a stagnant state of existence that does not lead anywhere, and decides to move forward with life, in another sense, to inevitable mortality. The picturebook subtly incorporates and compares two ways of seeing and experiencing the world—the right hemisphere encourages us to connect to the world around us with its “changeability”, “impermanence”, and “interconnectedness”, while the left hemisphere enables us to make sense of the world through fragmenting and stabilising it (McGilchrist, 2012, p. 93). The way the fictional world is represented therefore has crossover potential because it is grounded in human beings’ existential conditions, that is, the conflict and cooperation of the right hemisphere and the left.

    The fictional world represented in Forever may engage readers’ attention and imagination and stimulate their cognitive activity. Readers of a wide age range can engage with the inventive fictional world because the right hemisphere of the brain is tuned to the new and unfamiliar. The way of representing the multitudinous worlds is grounded in the fundamental human existential conditions.

    3. Method

    3.1 Sampling

    The project was a small-scale case study, conducted in the east of England, UK. The brief information of the participants is as follows:

    Lucy (female, 9 years old)

    Lucy is from Ireland, and was studying in a local primary school at the site of research. She was well read.

    Oliver (male, 8 years old)

    Oliver, from Ireland, was studying at the same primary school. He said that he was not a big fan of reading, and when he read, he preferred comic books.

    Rihanna (female, 24 years old)

    Rihanna is American, and was studying at the site of research for one year. She was doing a Master’s degree, which had nothing to do with children’s literature.

    Sophie (female, 30 years old)

    Sophie is Chinese, and was studying at the doctoral level at the site of research. Her research was not relevant to children’s literature.

    The particularities of the participants were largely the result of convenience sampling. Convenience sampling does not aim for the sample’s representativeness of a larger population, and enables the researcher to pool together limited resources within reach (Cohen et al., 2011, pp. 155-156). Since this project was an exploratory study of a new way of thinking about crossover picturebooks through investigating the actual readers’ engagement, the generalisability of specific conclusions regarding the set of sampled readers was not of primary concern. The particularities of the participants, on the one hand, posed a challenge to the researcher in singling out age and cognitive function as specific factors for the analysis later. On the other hand, if the patterns of difference between the adults’ reading and the children’s could be identified despite their particularities, this rather lent support to an interpretation isolating the factors of age and cognitive function.

    3.2 Data collection

    For each participant, there were in total four sessions of interview. An overview of the sessions is as follows:

    Table 1. Overview of the sessions

    The semi-structured interview with its “flexibility” allows the participant to “develop ideas and speak more widely on the issues raised by the researcher” (Denscombe, 2010, p. 175), and the researcher to probe into the given response (Cohen et al., 2011, p. 420). The semi-structured interview therefore was adopted in all the interviewing sessions. The first session with the participants was not to discuss the selected text, but to know more about their reading interests and habits. Group interviewing was used with the children because children may feel less uncomfortable in the presence of a stranger when they are talking in a group (Greig & Taylor, 1999, p. 132). Individual interviewing was used with the adults. The second session of individual interview centred on the readers’ encounter with the selected picturebook. The participants were not told beforehand which picturebook they would read because their spontaneous reaction was of more interest. The third session of follow-up individual interview was designed to address the issues that emerged from the second session. The fourth session was to further probe the readers’ responses to the text in the form of textrelated creative activities, for instance, drawing or creative writing of their own choice. The main purpose of the creative activities was to support or clarify the oral responses.

    Several interview prompts were prepared for the first and the second sessions (see Appendix A and B for the interview prompts of the first two sessions respectively). During the interviews, the sequence and exact wording of the prompts did not have to be strictly followed. The interview prompts had been tested in a pilot study prior to the project. The sessions two to four were videotaped, while the first one was audiotaped for the concern that the participants might get intimidated to be videotaped the first time we met. The sessions were conducted in a relatively informal setting, as the project was about the readers’ spontaneous engagement rather than their instructed responses.

    3.3 Data analysis

    The data included the audiotapes and videotapes, a full transcription of the videotapes, and the products of the text-related creative activities. The data was first examined in terms of what textual aspects the readers engaged with. The pieces of data relating to the readers’ engagement with the fictional world were then selected out for further analysis. The data analysis consists in the individual accounts of the readers’ engagement that explore the salient features and patterns of their readings. The discussion is placed at the end of the article to explore what patterns of differences and similarities emerge across the four readers, and see how the findings may inform the understanding of the selected text and possibly crossover picturebooks in general.

    4. Findings

    4.1 Lucy

    In the first reading, when I asked Lucy to choose one page and tell me about what she thought of it, she opted to stay at the third spread that presents a panoramic view of the bookshelf world. She spent half of the first reading describing how the bookshelf world looked like a home to her and later produced a piece of writing about it. In the second reading, she acted out the idea that the bookshelf was a home:

    L: And maybe they can live all up inside the whole shelf, oh ~ [clapping]. Ba, ba, ba, ba, ba [laughing, and emulating how a person goes up]. It could be like a block of

    apartments, like there’s a room there, a room there, a room there, and a room there [using her finger to point at the books all the way up the shelf]. And somebody may live in the ceiling, between the ceiling and the floor, there’s a book, pull it out [pretending to pull something down from the ceiling], that would be so cool [laughing and waving her hand in a big circle]! It would be awesome to live in a shelf, like “nice book over here, yeah, um ~ sounds good” [laughing and pretending to snatch a book and read]. Snatch, Snatch [laughing and pretending to make great efforts in order to snatch something out of her reach]. It’s great! And then you’ve got a front door and a back door. In the front door, out the back door, go there, so like bye bye Amanda, hello Samantha [laughing]!

    Researcher: Oh, that’s wonderful!

    L: Oh ~ Yeah! And then we can have something like “-anda,” like something, so like, like, I have to think about this, like, bye bye Amanda, hello Samantha, hello Lyanda [laughing], and then hello somebody else, up, hello, hello, up, hello neighbors, up, hello neighbors [using her finger to point out on the page the route upstairs]. Like, hello best friend, like go inside, whoo~ [using her finger to point all the way up the shelf], hi, best friend. Living in the ceiling, they have a great view over the city. She showed a bustling bookshelf world, where miniature people live all the way up, winding their ways around, greeting friends and neighbours, and snatching books as they climb up. Her performance incorporated pointing, gesturing, shifting body postures, and laughing.

    Her performance was also rhythmic, resembling poetry in its primitive form. The rhythmic bits in her performance can be grouped as follows:

    ba/ ba/ ba/ ba/ ba

    a room there/ a room there/ a room there/ and a room there

    snatch/ snatch

    in the front door/ out the back door

    bye bye Amanda/ hello Samantha/ hello Lyanda

    Iain McGilchrist sees music as preceding language rather than as an incidental spinoff from language, because the musical aspects of language (such as intonation, pitch, rhythm, and so on) come first in children before syntax and vocabulary (2012, pp. 103-104). This resonates with Crago’s finding that children, as young as under two years old, experiment with baby talk of a musical nature. The meaning of baby talk depends not so much on the words that are used as on the embedded sound patterns. When experimenting with sound patterns, young children are conveying an underlying emotional concern (Crago, 2014, pp. 15-17). McGilchrist goes further to argue that music is the “communication of emotion”: “the prosody and rhythmic motion … emerge intuitively from entrainment of the body in emotional expression” (2012, p. 103). The musical aspects of Lucy’s performance were anchored in her embodied experience, as she climbed upwards, propped the body with clawing hands, occasionally spared one hand to snatch a book, and waved to friends when she scrambled up. The embodied experience was replete with emotions—joy at seeing and greeting friends, relief and pleasure at taking in a whole view of the city when she finally arrived at the top, and most importantly, the ecstasy of living through a fictional world. Her performance epitomised what McGilchrist views as the fundamental interconnectedness of embodiedness, musicality, and emotion.

    4.2 Oliver

    When reading Forever for the first time, Oliver sensed the existence of another space beyond the bookshelf world from the pictorial detail of the spaceship taking off:

    O: that’s kind of like stopping people who’re living in the space from going there, they’re stopping at this atmosphere, and they’re seeking another atmosphere.

    Researcher: So you mean there’s something different out there?

    O: Yeah. Because this background, this space, these astronaut things, like the astronaut living.

    The dialogue revealed Oliver’s view of the bookshelf world: first, the bookshelf constituted a living space; second, the bookshelf was confining as its inhabitants were stopped from “going there.”

    Compared to Lucy, who seemed to view the bookshelf world in a positive light as manifest in her response “my dream bookshelves,” Oliver tended to take a negative stance. Oliver’s negative stance towards the bookshelf world paralleled his earlier idea that it was confining. When I invited him to picture what it would be like to live in a world of books, Oliver acted out the danger the inhabitants of the bookshelf would face if they were discovered:

    No, [“no” meaning “I don’t want to live in the bookshelf world”]. Because if somebody comes to borrow it [a book], they open it up, and there will just be rooms in it. That would actually be pretty weird [laughing]. You can just pick people up by their hair [putting his hand on top of the head, and lifting his own hair up], and they’re like “don’t lift me up” [squinting up and laughing]! But they still lift them up, and they’re so frightened, and they try to run away.

    Oliver tried out the perspective of those who live in books, squinting around as if to see who was lifting him up, and exclaiming “don’t lift me up!” It can be inferred from his peals of laughter that Oliver very likely had fun with acting out the scene of a person being pulled up by his hair and uprooted out of his home, which however would be frightening in real life. As Susan Keen suggests, the trying out of different perspectives and feelings in literary reading occurs in a safe zone without putting a resultant demand on readers’ action in real life (2007, p. 4). Oliver’s performance was circumscribed within the safe zone—he could imagine and act out the danger in the fictional world without jeopardising his own security.

    4.3 Rihanna

    It fascinated Rihanna to see the pictorial representation of books as a physical space. She was amazed at how “each book is a house”. She traced the changes in the position and layout of the books, and then moved from the materiality of books foregrounded in the visual to a more abstract notion of books as carriers of life stories and information: “It makes sense, because if you read a book, it’s a story about somebody else’s life. It’s almost like where they live. So it’s almost metaphorical.” For her, the multilayered visual details crowding the bookshelf could generate the same effect on senses as the “massive”, “overwhelming” amount of information that a book contains.Another salient feature of Rihanna’s engagement with the fictional world was that she made efforts to trace and explain the consistent appearances of the small visual details that do not contribute to the development of the main narrative. She noticed the author/illustrator’s name “Colin Thompson” on the front cover, and the copyright year “1995” on the first spread from the very start of her first encounter with the text. Paying close attention to these paratextual features enabled her to infer that the pictorial detail of “1994” was the author/illustrator’s “signature”: “I’m curious why he put 1994, specifically that year. And I saw here [pointing at spread 1] his copyright is 1995. So this is maybe his signature of when this was written” (my emphasis). She noticed the reappearance of the number “1994” on the second spread (“ah ~ the author is putting up 1994 again”), and predicted whether the popping up of “1994” would be a trend throughout. Her prediction was later confirmed. She then conjectured at the author/illustrator’s intention of inserting his “signature” into the text: “Oh, lots of play … how funny that he carries that through! I wonder if it’s more for his own satisfaction … maybe it doesn’t have anything to do with the reader.” Rihanna’s attempt to trace and account for the appearances of “1994”, “1995”, and “CT” reflected her intention to sort out consistency in the fictional world crowded with multilayered visual details.

    What also stood out in Rihanna’s reading was that she treated some pictorial details as temporal markers, for instance, “the sunglasses that look like they can be from the sixties”, “a modern day pencil”, and the “floppy disk” that was popular more than ten years ago. Drawing on the paratextual knowledge of the text being produced in 1995, she placed the detail “record card” within the context: “So this was written in the nineties when that transition [from the use of record cards to that of computer catalogues in libraries] was happening. So there were still record cards. But computers were becoming a thing.” Therefore, Rihanna’s reading showed three salient features: first, proceeding from the notion of books as a physical space to that of books as an abstract carrier of life stories; second, striving to sort out consistency in the multilayered visual details, especially paying attention to the paratext; third, seeking temporal markers, and situating the picturebook in its context of production. Rihanna’s emphasis on the paratextual features revealed her awareness of fiction as a deliberate construction. The acquisition of the awareness that fiction is a constructed set of settings, events and characters has to do with the skill of abstract thinking (Nikolajeva, 2014, pp. 22-27).

    4.4 Sophie

    The visual image of two sloping books that form a tent on the title page caught Sophie’s attention from the beginning of her first encounter with the text. She associated the two sloping books with a “tent” and a “shelter”. Both “tent” and “shelter” denote space: the former is a concrete physical space, while the latter can be any refuge in a more abstract sense. The first impression of the book as a space was interweaved into her following reading. This echoes Ryan’s finding that when readers construct narrative space, the first impression is the strongest and not easily remedied, even if it is wrong (2003, pp. 214-242).

    Fascinated with the visual representation of space and time in Forever, Sophie invented a new story based on the fictional world in the second reading. She inferred about the temporal dimension from pictorial details, and integrated the spatial construction with the sense of temporality:they [the person in a red top and the four old men on spread 9] might not be from the same time ‘cause they’re [pointing at the old men] dressed like in ancient costume, clothes. But then he’s [the person in a red top] obviously from the modern time. So it’s a journey into, like, beyond time. Time travel? Probably, or maybe there’s no sense of time within the bookshelf. ‘Cause maybe the past and the present are confused, mixed into that space. And how different spaces lay out, maybe space can represent time itself.

    Sophie used the clothes as a temporal marker to infer that the five characters came from different times. She combined a mixture of time dimensions with a series of different spaces, and reached the conclusion that “space can represent time itself”. Her interpretation touched on the fundamental parameters of the fictional world.

    To include the character of the ancient child in her story, Sophie suspended her knowledge of the original story conveyed in the verbal, and followed her train of thought:

    I remember this is the ancient child, but then if we forget about that story, he [the character in the red top] came out of this wonderful space [the Chinese garden on spread 12], and then met this guy, who’s in charge of the whole city [laughing].

    Early in her first reading, Sophie associated books as an actual living space with books as a carrier of abstract knowledge—“I wonder if those people live inside these books, do they read the books themselves? Or do they just automatically absorb all the information into their heads?” This interpretation fed into her understanding of the image of the ancient child. She associated the spookiness and gloominess of the image with the nature of knowledge:

    And the person [the ancient child] who’s in charge of all the books is such a weird person. He’s both young and old because all the knowledge is old. But he’s young because all the knowledge apparently is trying to produce something new, and then make changes to the future … So it’s such a contradictory world.

    She moreover incorporated into her story the visual detail of the yellow light that caught her attention from early on—the yellow light suggested a world of hope beyond.

    Assembling the interconnectedness of space and time, books as a carrier of knowledge, and the yellow light, Sophie came up with a story about the limitation of knowledge and the need to look beyond it:

    So maybe, the reason why he [the character in the red top] came into this library is because he’s very interested in learning … So either it’s a dream, or it’s what happens to him because he has this wish to live among all those books. So he came into this place, and then had a real experience of what it would be like to live among all those books. But actually living among the books, it turned out to be not that enjoyable [laughing] … So he realises that eventually we have to live in reality… So the ultimate spiritual home for me, well, from the point of view of this person, is not within the bookshelves, but back in reality [pointing at the last page]. Yeah, … that’s the story I would make.

    In the last session, Sophie, based on her story, drew a picture of one person emerging out of darkness to look into the sunny world outside. Sophie’s story revolved around the issues that had attracted her from the very beginning. Her response adds to Ryan’s finding: the first impression of the space represented in literature can be elaborated in more detail, especially when the literary work provides sufficient fabrics for readers to work with, allowing their imagination and creativity to unfold.

    5. Discussion

    In this empirical study, the adult readers showed the tendency to use some pictorial details as temporal markers of the fictional world, while the child readers did not seem to. The theory of brain laterality may provide an explanation for this difference. The sense of time passing is associated with sustained attention (McGilchrist, 2012, pp. 75-76), that is, the type of attention closely relating to a coherent perception of the world (pp. 38-39). Since the right hemisphere governs sustained attention, the sense of the temporal flow, especially time as “something lived through”, arises in the right hemisphere (p. 76; my emphasis). The left hemisphere tends to perceive time as “an infinite series of static moments” (p. 76). The left hemisphere thereby has a particular advantage where there is an emphasis on “a point in time” (p. 77). The pictorial details used as temporal markers correspond to various points in time. The right hemisphere, compared to the left hemisphere, matures earlier, and gets more involved in early childhood mental activities (p. 88; Crago, 2014, p. 12). Presumably, this suggests that children likely have a stronger sense of time as a flow, something to be “l(fā)ived through”, while adults may tend to use a few static points as temporal markers. This is precisely the case with the four readers in my study.

    The readers externalised the idea of books as a living space for miniature inhabitants in different ways. Lucy and Oliver acted the idea out, orchestrating verbal language, pointing, gesturing, body movement, and laughing. Lucy’s performance was especially sensuous and musical. Lucy’s and Oliver’s performance showed their different attitudes towards the idea of books as a living space: Lucy seemed to be fascinated with living in books, while Oliver performed how unpleasant and dangerous it could be. This difference may relate to the fact that Lucy was fond of reading while Oliver was not a big fan of reading, as they claimed in the first session on reading interests and habits. Both Lucy and Oliver tried out the perspective of those who live in books. The fictional world can provide a safe zone to engage with various new things one has not encountered or cannot possibly encounter in real life, which may be especially beneficial for child readers as their repertoire of experience is in the process of forming and restructuring.

    Rihanna and Sophie, when externalising the idea of books as a physical space, did not seem to engage the body as actively and deeply as Lucy and Oliver did. The difference, again, may be explained in terms of brain laterality. While the right hemisphere sees the self as embodied and within the world, the left hemisphere sees the self as abstract and isolated (McGilchrist, 2012, pp. 46-51, pp. 54-56). Through a more intimate connection to the more ancient layers of the brain, the right hemisphere is more closely involved in emotional life. The left is more responsible for making sense of the world through dividing it into manageable bits and pieces (pp. 51-53, pp. 59-64; Crago, 2014, pp. 8-10). To restate: compared to the left hemisphere, the right hemisphere matures earlier. Lucy and Oliver may have moved forward from early childhood, but Rihanna and Sophie had ten to twenty more years of training in and practice of language, abstract and critical thinking. Moreover, given their educational backgrounds, the adult readers must have a more developed capacity for a lefthemisphere dominant way of attending to the world. Both Lucy and Sophie showed a strong story impulse that, according to Crago, arises from the right hemisphere because of its close connection to emotion and experience (2014, p. 13). Lucy’s way of bringing the story to life relied on entrainment of the body in emotional expression, typical of a right-hemisphere dominant way of experiencing the world. Sophie leaned towards the left hemisphere’s manner of culling details as she read along, organising and reorganising them, giving patterns to them, constantly modifying the patterns, and weaving the details into a coherent whole.

    When a researcher uses Thompson’s picturebooks with readers, the tightly packed visual details may serve as “points of fascination”, which means the aspects of a text that have a particular potential for triggering readers’ imaginative responses (Baird et al., 2016, p. 12). All the readers actively engaged with various points of fascination. Rihanna and Sophie displayed an especially strong tendency to sort out consistency in the world of overwhelming details. They traced, connected, and attempted to explain the repeated appearances of some pictorial details. This is typical of a lefthemisphere dominant need for making sense and taking control of a seemingly chaotic world. Despite the four readers’ different ways of engaging with the fictional world, they all seemed to enjoy the multilayered visual details, as manifest in their laughing and exclamations. Lucy’s and Oliver’s relationships with the fictional world were embodied while Rihanna’s and Sophie’s were more abstract yet no less creative. This gives rise to the reflection: what matters is not only what aspects of the text the readers come to have a relationship with, but also, and more importantly, how the relationship is, and how they come to have that relationship. The reflection is of central importance to the discussion of crossover picturebooks, wherein the key issue involved is how a text can appeal to different audiences.

    Definitions of crossover picturebooks often emphasise that a text addresses adult readers and child readers on different levels yet in equal terms. The multileveled nature of crossover picturebooks may correspond to the multileveled engagement that texts can elicit from readers. The multileveled engagement is to a great extent the outcome of cognitive-affective skills and mechanisms engaged differently across children and adults. Therefore, the term “multileveled” in Beckett’s definition of crossover picturebooks (2012, p. 16) may be interpreted as on multiple cognitiveaffective levels. The actual readers’ responses also reveal an unexpected aspect of Forever’s crossover potential. Through picturing the books that, in different layouts and positions, shelter the miniature inhabitants, the visual foregrounds the materiality of books as objects that can evoke a tactile and sensuous experience. Such an experience parallels the readers’ ongoing process of closing and opening the book, fingering and turning the pages, pointing and trailing, going back and forth, and playing it around. All the readers mentioned books as a physical space. The foregrounded materiality of books then opens up avenues for thinking about books as an abstract space that stores massive information, knowledge and life stories. The move from materiality to abstraction is inextricably bound up with the way the human mind works—the understanding of more abstract concepts is grounded in our embodied experience (Lakoff & Johnson, 1999). Scholars often associate the picturebook’s capacity for appealing to diverse audiences with dual narrative, that is, the combination of the visual narrative and the verbal narrative. For instance, Beckett argues that dual narrative gives rise to experimentation and innovation, which is at the heart of picturebooks’ crossover potential (2012, p. 2). Scott argues that the dual narrative of picturebooks affords a unique opportunity for “the collaborative relationship” between children and adults (1999, p. 101). This study shows another aspect of the crossover potential particular to the medium of picturebooks: picturebooks can use the visual to flesh out the embodied experience, which can springboard further thinking about more abstract and complicated notions. This mode of representation corresponds to the way the human mind works, thus engaging children and adults alike.

    Acknowledgements

    The writing up of this study is supported by the research project “Contemporary Western Picturebook Theory” (18WWC005) funded by Jiangsu Planning Office of Philosophy and Social Science and the project “Chinese Picturebooks and Their English Translations” (2017SJB1318) funded by Jiangsu Education Department.

    美女脱内裤让男人舔精品视频| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 亚洲精品成人av观看孕妇| videos熟女内射| 久久久久国产网址| av免费观看日本| 国产视频首页在线观看| 亚洲av免费高清在线观看| 晚上一个人看的免费电影| 一级毛片aaaaaa免费看小| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 国产黄频视频在线观看| 午夜久久久在线观看| av卡一久久| 少妇熟女欧美另类| 久热这里只有精品99| 成人亚洲欧美一区二区av| 久久久亚洲精品成人影院| 国产欧美日韩综合在线一区二区 | 天堂俺去俺来也www色官网| 99热这里只有精品一区| 日韩熟女老妇一区二区性免费视频| av在线观看视频网站免费| 又粗又硬又长又爽又黄的视频| 国产成人免费无遮挡视频| 国产无遮挡羞羞视频在线观看| 国产免费又黄又爽又色| 日本-黄色视频高清免费观看| 日韩制服骚丝袜av| 午夜福利视频精品| 亚洲不卡免费看| 99re6热这里在线精品视频| 久久热精品热| 亚洲内射少妇av| 一区二区三区乱码不卡18| 亚洲av综合色区一区| 人妻 亚洲 视频| 天天操日日干夜夜撸| 各种免费的搞黄视频| 日韩精品有码人妻一区| 国产男女内射视频| 青春草国产在线视频| 国产淫语在线视频| 国产极品天堂在线| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 免费少妇av软件| 插逼视频在线观看| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 国产日韩欧美视频二区| 少妇被粗大的猛进出69影院 | 黄色欧美视频在线观看| 日韩av在线免费看完整版不卡| 夫妻午夜视频| 妹子高潮喷水视频| 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看 | 日本黄大片高清| 国产精品国产三级国产av玫瑰| 男男h啪啪无遮挡| 欧美成人午夜免费资源| 亚洲欧美精品自产自拍| 国产亚洲最大av| 韩国av在线不卡| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| 日韩av在线免费看完整版不卡| 国产亚洲91精品色在线| 最近2019中文字幕mv第一页| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看| 在线观看免费日韩欧美大片 | 国产一级毛片在线| 成人亚洲精品一区在线观看| 97超视频在线观看视频| 水蜜桃什么品种好| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 亚洲美女黄色视频免费看| 亚洲情色 制服丝袜| 人妻系列 视频| 91精品一卡2卡3卡4卡| 一区二区三区精品91| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清| 亚洲精品456在线播放app| 国产精品99久久久久久久久| 日本vs欧美在线观看视频 | 伊人亚洲综合成人网| 日本猛色少妇xxxxx猛交久久| 亚洲国产色片| 欧美人与善性xxx| 国产黄频视频在线观看| 精品卡一卡二卡四卡免费| 国产91av在线免费观看| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看| 中国三级夫妇交换| 99热6这里只有精品| 亚洲性久久影院| 婷婷色综合大香蕉| 中文字幕精品免费在线观看视频 | 欧美日韩亚洲高清精品| av.在线天堂| 大码成人一级视频| 热re99久久精品国产66热6| 不卡视频在线观看欧美| 97超视频在线观看视频| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| 国产在线免费精品| 国产亚洲5aaaaa淫片| 3wmmmm亚洲av在线观看| 日韩三级伦理在线观看| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 久久韩国三级中文字幕| 美女内射精品一级片tv| 国产乱来视频区| 少妇精品久久久久久久| 亚洲无线观看免费| 日韩欧美 国产精品| av网站免费在线观看视频| 国产精品偷伦视频观看了| 五月伊人婷婷丁香| 日本猛色少妇xxxxx猛交久久| 91精品伊人久久大香线蕉| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 在线观看一区二区三区激情| 亚洲国产日韩一区二区| 美女脱内裤让男人舔精品视频| 日韩av不卡免费在线播放| 视频区图区小说| √禁漫天堂资源中文www| av在线播放精品| 麻豆乱淫一区二区| av在线老鸭窝| 久久久久久久久久久免费av| 蜜臀久久99精品久久宅男| 男女边吃奶边做爰视频| 插阴视频在线观看视频| 美女视频免费永久观看网站| 在线播放无遮挡| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 国产精品国产三级国产av玫瑰| 黄色毛片三级朝国网站 | 亚洲真实伦在线观看| 啦啦啦在线观看免费高清www| 18禁在线无遮挡免费观看视频| a级毛色黄片| 亚洲丝袜综合中文字幕| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 国模一区二区三区四区视频| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添av毛片| 亚洲美女视频黄频| 一区二区三区免费毛片| 中国美白少妇内射xxxbb| 久久精品久久久久久噜噜老黄| 久久午夜福利片| 男女无遮挡免费网站观看| 午夜日本视频在线| 日日爽夜夜爽网站| 国产亚洲av片在线观看秒播厂| 高清黄色对白视频在线免费看 | 伦理电影大哥的女人| 国产成人免费观看mmmm| 国产精品国产三级国产专区5o| av有码第一页| 各种免费的搞黄视频| 99热6这里只有精品| 国产又色又爽无遮挡免| 亚洲精品日韩av片在线观看| 久久6这里有精品| 国产在线一区二区三区精| 精品国产露脸久久av麻豆| 免费av中文字幕在线| 欧美3d第一页| 亚洲在久久综合| 成人国产av品久久久| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 九草在线视频观看| 亚洲av福利一区| 少妇 在线观看| 国产在线免费精品| 日韩av免费高清视频| 成人二区视频| 欧美日韩一区二区视频在线观看视频在线| 黄色怎么调成土黄色| 国产亚洲av片在线观看秒播厂| 纵有疾风起免费观看全集完整版| 久久 成人 亚洲| 男人添女人高潮全过程视频| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 性色avwww在线观看| 久久毛片免费看一区二区三区| 69精品国产乱码久久久| 老司机影院成人| 国产精品99久久久久久久久| 国产男人的电影天堂91| 久久精品国产亚洲av天美| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 嫩草影院新地址| 欧美精品高潮呻吟av久久| 男的添女的下面高潮视频| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图| 欧美成人午夜免费资源| 国产精品国产三级专区第一集| 大片免费播放器 马上看| 99久久精品一区二区三区| 精华霜和精华液先用哪个| 在现免费观看毛片| .国产精品久久| 国产亚洲午夜精品一区二区久久| 超碰97精品在线观看| 大陆偷拍与自拍| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 欧美日韩亚洲高清精品| 亚洲精品乱码久久久久久按摩| av卡一久久| av专区在线播放| 少妇人妻一区二区三区视频| 久久国内精品自在自线图片| 国产成人午夜福利电影在线观看| 18禁裸乳无遮挡动漫免费视频| 国产亚洲一区二区精品| 欧美精品人与动牲交sv欧美| 国产免费一区二区三区四区乱码| 精品人妻熟女毛片av久久网站| 自拍欧美九色日韩亚洲蝌蚪91 | 各种免费的搞黄视频| 妹子高潮喷水视频| 在线看a的网站| 免费观看性生交大片5| 精品一区二区三卡| 日本wwww免费看| 少妇 在线观看| 欧美区成人在线视频| 9色porny在线观看| 久久久精品免费免费高清| 国产精品秋霞免费鲁丝片| 免费黄色在线免费观看| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 亚洲va在线va天堂va国产| 麻豆成人av视频| 自拍欧美九色日韩亚洲蝌蚪91 | 日韩三级伦理在线观看| 亚洲图色成人| 只有这里有精品99| 最近2019中文字幕mv第一页| 大话2 男鬼变身卡| 午夜福利影视在线免费观看| 国产黄片美女视频| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 成人午夜精彩视频在线观看| 国产亚洲91精品色在线| 日本91视频免费播放| 一区在线观看完整版| 欧美精品亚洲一区二区| 欧美日韩综合久久久久久| 久久人人爽av亚洲精品天堂| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看| 少妇的逼好多水| 亚洲图色成人| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 大香蕉97超碰在线| 精品国产露脸久久av麻豆| 黄色怎么调成土黄色| 久久婷婷青草| 色婷婷av一区二区三区视频| 久久久久久人妻| 汤姆久久久久久久影院中文字幕| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 国产精品一区二区性色av| 91精品伊人久久大香线蕉| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区| 一级毛片电影观看| 精品久久久噜噜| 人人妻人人澡人人看| 国产视频内射| 成人毛片60女人毛片免费| 国产免费视频播放在线视频| 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看 | 3wmmmm亚洲av在线观看| 国产免费一区二区三区四区乱码| 我要看黄色一级片免费的| 一级a做视频免费观看| 女性被躁到高潮视频| 91精品一卡2卡3卡4卡| 国产精品无大码| 啦啦啦中文免费视频观看日本| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆| 国产精品人妻久久久久久| 一本—道久久a久久精品蜜桃钙片| 国产 精品1| 欧美最新免费一区二区三区| 久久影院123| 国产亚洲av片在线观看秒播厂| 久久久午夜欧美精品| 亚洲高清免费不卡视频| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| 国产午夜精品久久久久久一区二区三区| 在线观看免费高清a一片| 一级爰片在线观看| 久久99精品国语久久久| 国产免费视频播放在线视频| 亚洲丝袜综合中文字幕| 午夜av观看不卡| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| 国产探花极品一区二区| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线| 国内精品宾馆在线| 18+在线观看网站| 久久精品久久久久久噜噜老黄| 99久国产av精品国产电影| 91精品一卡2卡3卡4卡| 亚洲怡红院男人天堂| 人妻少妇偷人精品九色| 国产成人freesex在线| 成人国产麻豆网| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 免费看光身美女| 爱豆传媒免费全集在线观看| 亚洲av.av天堂| 狂野欧美激情性bbbbbb| 欧美日本中文国产一区发布| 一本—道久久a久久精品蜜桃钙片| 国产片特级美女逼逼视频| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 精品午夜福利在线看| 丁香六月天网| 精品一区二区免费观看| 又爽又黄a免费视频| 人人妻人人澡人人看| 久热久热在线精品观看| 国产熟女午夜一区二区三区 | 高清在线视频一区二区三区| 日日撸夜夜添| 国产在线一区二区三区精| 看非洲黑人一级黄片| 国产一级毛片在线| av福利片在线| 国产精品伦人一区二区| 美女主播在线视频| 精品99又大又爽又粗少妇毛片| 亚洲精品亚洲一区二区| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 中文天堂在线官网| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 欧美精品一区二区大全| 亚洲人与动物交配视频| freevideosex欧美| 性色av一级| 只有这里有精品99| 男人狂女人下面高潮的视频| 最黄视频免费看| 如何舔出高潮| 在线观看免费日韩欧美大片 | 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 日韩欧美一区视频在线观看 | 青春草国产在线视频| 寂寞人妻少妇视频99o| 卡戴珊不雅视频在线播放| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| 亚洲精品视频女| 王馨瑶露胸无遮挡在线观看| 欧美变态另类bdsm刘玥| 26uuu在线亚洲综合色| 久久久久精品性色| 五月开心婷婷网| 国国产精品蜜臀av免费| 丝瓜视频免费看黄片| 寂寞人妻少妇视频99o| 最近中文字幕高清免费大全6| 国产淫语在线视频| 又大又黄又爽视频免费| 免费黄色在线免费观看| 99精国产麻豆久久婷婷| 我要看日韩黄色一级片| 久久久久久久亚洲中文字幕| 亚洲精品日韩av片在线观看| 国产精品一区www在线观看| 久久女婷五月综合色啪小说| 精品一区在线观看国产| 丝瓜视频免费看黄片| 在线看a的网站| 啦啦啦视频在线资源免费观看| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 69精品国产乱码久久久| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 黄色一级大片看看| 欧美少妇被猛烈插入视频| 精品亚洲成a人片在线观看| 街头女战士在线观看网站| 日韩亚洲欧美综合| 五月开心婷婷网| 亚洲精品国产色婷婷电影| 日本91视频免费播放| av免费观看日本| 欧美精品亚洲一区二区| 久久国产精品大桥未久av | 在线播放无遮挡| 日韩中文字幕视频在线看片| 狠狠精品人妻久久久久久综合| 美女大奶头黄色视频| 丰满少妇做爰视频| 国产成人一区二区在线| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清| 不卡视频在线观看欧美| 18禁在线无遮挡免费观看视频| 大陆偷拍与自拍| 春色校园在线视频观看| 成人毛片60女人毛片免费| 黄色视频在线播放观看不卡| 日韩av在线免费看完整版不卡| 国产日韩欧美视频二区| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 蜜桃久久精品国产亚洲av| www.色视频.com| 少妇精品久久久久久久| 国国产精品蜜臀av免费| 伦精品一区二区三区| 久久97久久精品| 久久女婷五月综合色啪小说| 日本欧美国产在线视频| 亚洲精品日韩在线中文字幕| 一级av片app| 日韩精品有码人妻一区| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 久久久国产一区二区| 赤兔流量卡办理| 免费大片18禁| 五月开心婷婷网| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 伊人亚洲综合成人网| 青春草国产在线视频| 婷婷色综合大香蕉| 日本91视频免费播放| 高清av免费在线| 18+在线观看网站| 日本av手机在线免费观看| 日韩熟女老妇一区二区性免费视频| 午夜老司机福利剧场| 高清黄色对白视频在线免费看 | av福利片在线| 国产日韩欧美亚洲二区| 午夜激情久久久久久久| 国产伦理片在线播放av一区| 毛片一级片免费看久久久久| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 中文乱码字字幕精品一区二区三区| 久久久久久久久久久久大奶| 欧美3d第一页| 日本wwww免费看| 在线观看一区二区三区激情| 国产高清国产精品国产三级| av有码第一页| av不卡在线播放| 一级,二级,三级黄色视频| 另类精品久久| 亚洲伊人久久精品综合| 秋霞伦理黄片| 黄色怎么调成土黄色| 久久国产精品大桥未久av | 欧美日韩视频精品一区| 高清毛片免费看| 日韩中文字幕视频在线看片| 欧美老熟妇乱子伦牲交| 六月丁香七月| 成人毛片60女人毛片免费| 三级国产精品片| 久久久久精品久久久久真实原创| 又爽又黄a免费视频| 97超视频在线观看视频| 欧美高清成人免费视频www| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 全区人妻精品视频| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 精品亚洲成a人片在线观看| 国产精品免费大片| 不卡视频在线观看欧美| 免费观看的影片在线观看| 午夜日本视频在线| 狠狠精品人妻久久久久久综合| 亚洲一级一片aⅴ在线观看| 国产在线男女| 91精品国产国语对白视频| 永久网站在线| 热re99久久精品国产66热6| 99久久精品热视频| 男女无遮挡免费网站观看| 青青草视频在线视频观看| av天堂中文字幕网| 下体分泌物呈黄色| 日本色播在线视频| 国产一区有黄有色的免费视频| 成人影院久久| 欧美最新免费一区二区三区| 精品人妻熟女av久视频| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 国产日韩欧美在线精品| 国产日韩一区二区三区精品不卡 | 我的老师免费观看完整版| 久久精品久久久久久久性| 日本vs欧美在线观看视频 | av国产精品久久久久影院| 插逼视频在线观看| 国产乱来视频区| 99久久精品热视频| 赤兔流量卡办理| 啦啦啦视频在线资源免费观看| 日本黄大片高清| 热re99久久精品国产66热6| 久久久久久久久大av| 少妇的逼水好多| 国产免费视频播放在线视频| 天天操日日干夜夜撸| 极品教师在线视频| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 国产在线视频一区二区| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 国产一区二区三区av在线| 观看免费一级毛片| 在线观看美女被高潮喷水网站| 久久久久久久久久人人人人人人| 少妇人妻精品综合一区二区| 国产精品秋霞免费鲁丝片| 久久这里有精品视频免费| 精品少妇久久久久久888优播| 熟女电影av网| 色视频www国产| 亚洲国产精品专区欧美| 亚洲精品一二三| 亚洲成人手机| 亚洲精品日韩在线中文字幕| 亚洲国产精品国产精品| 搡老乐熟女国产| 最近中文字幕2019免费版| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| av免费在线看不卡| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区| 亚洲综合精品二区| av播播在线观看一区| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 免费大片18禁| 国产成人精品久久久久久| 在线观看www视频免费| 一本色道久久久久久精品综合| 亚洲国产精品999| 夜夜骑夜夜射夜夜干| 中文欧美无线码| 一级爰片在线观看| 一区二区三区四区激情视频| 成人毛片a级毛片在线播放| 精品99又大又爽又粗少妇毛片| 亚洲欧洲日产国产| av福利片在线观看| 建设人人有责人人尽责人人享有的| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 欧美 亚洲 国产 日韩一| 午夜av观看不卡| 精品一区二区三卡| 91精品国产国语对白视频| 久久免费观看电影| 97在线人人人人妻| 国产精品女同一区二区软件| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看| 超碰97精品在线观看| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 少妇高潮的动态图| 一级黄片播放器| 色吧在线观看| 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看 | videossex国产| 只有这里有精品99| 日本欧美视频一区| 国产成人免费无遮挡视频| 亚洲精品乱码久久久久久按摩| 又爽又黄a免费视频| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 国产精品一区二区性色av| 国产91av在线免费观看| 日韩强制内射视频| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 特大巨黑吊av在线直播| 黑人猛操日本美女一级片| 久久久久久久久久人人人人人人| 久久精品久久久久久久性| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 国产黄片美女视频| 国产亚洲一区二区精品| 777米奇影视久久| 日韩熟女老妇一区二区性免费视频| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产| 色吧在线观看| 看免费成人av毛片| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产| 中国三级夫妇交换| 看免费成人av毛片| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 人人妻人人澡人人看| 黄色欧美视频在线观看| 搡老乐熟女国产| 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区 | 亚洲欧美一区二区三区黑人 | 男女无遮挡免费网站观看| 久久影院123| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 中文乱码字字幕精品一区二区三区| 最新的欧美精品一区二区| 丝袜在线中文字幕| 成人国产av品久久久| 少妇人妻 视频| 国产精品麻豆人妻色哟哟久久| 91久久精品国产一区二区三区| 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区 | 99久久精品热视频| 成人午夜精彩视频在线观看| 久久青草综合色| av.在线天堂| 国产91av在线免费观看|