• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Interchange between grooming and infant handling in female Tibetan macaques(Macaca thibetana)

    2019-04-29 07:10:34QiJiangDongPoXiaXiWangDaoZhangBingHuaSunJinHuaLi
    Zoological Research 2019年2期

    Qi Jiang,Dong-Po Xia,Xi Wang,Dao Zhang,Bing-Hua Sun,Jin-Hua Li,3,*

    1School of Resource and Environmental Engineering,Anhui University,Hefei Anhui 230601,China

    2School of Life Science,Anhui University,Hefei Anhui 230601,China

    3School of Life Science,Hefei Normal University,Hefei Anhui 241000,China

    ABSTRACT In some non-human primates,infants function as a social tool that can bridge relationships among group members.Infants are a desired commodity for group members,and mothers control access to them.The biological market theory suggests that grooming is widespread and represents a commodity that can be exchanged for infant handling. As a limited resource,however,the extent to which infants are interchanged between mothers(females with an infant)and non-mothers(potential handlers,females without an infant)remains unclear.In this study,we collected behavioral data to investigate the relationship between grooming and infant handling in free-ranging Tibetan macaques(Macaca thibetana)at Mt.Huangshan,China.Our results showed that females with infants received more grooming than females without infants.After her infant was handled,mother females received more grooming than they did during daily grooming interactions.However,with the increasing number of infants within the social group,both the grooming that mothers received and the grooming that non-mothers invested for handling infants decreased.We also found that non-mothers invested more time in grooming to gain access to younger infants than older infants.Our results provide evidence that infants are social commodities for both mother and non-mother females.Mothers use infants for obtain grooming and non-mothers use grooming to gain access to infants.The current study implies a bidirectional and complex interchange pattern between grooming and infant handling to compensate for the dyadic grooming disparity in non-human primates.

    Keywords:Tibetan macaques(Macaca thibetana);Interchange;Infant handling;Grooming;Biological market theory

    INTRODUCTION

    While female attraction to infants represents a common feature in non-human primate species,maternal responses to infant handling show a certain degree of variability(Maestripieri,1994;Nicolson,1987). In some species,such as Asian colobines,females allow their newborn infants to be held frequently and carried for long durations by other group members(infant caretaking)(Nicolson,1987;Stanford,1992).In other species,such as baboons and macaques,mothers are much more restrictive in allowing access their young offspring(Altmann,2002;Nicolson,1987),despite persistent attempts by group members to interact with their infant(Frank&Silk,2009;Gumert,2007a;Henzi&Barrett,2002).It is well documented that infants can be used as social tools to buffer agonistic interactions(Dunbar,1984;Taub,1980)and to facilitate social bonds(Dunayer&Berman,2017;Hsu et al.,2016;Manson,1999).Therefore,the infant handling concept may be indicative of the behavioral pattern that exists between an infant and a non-mother group member(Hsu et al.,2016;Jin et al.,2015;Maestripieri,1994).

    In recent decades,studies on the social function of infant handling within the framework of the biological market theory have gained increasing interest(Ueno,2017;Wei et al.,2013;Yu et al.,2013).Biological market theory proposes that the infant can be considered as a social commodity,and thus interchange relationships between infant handling and behavioral commodities may exist. For example,in chacma baboons(Papio cynocephalus ursinus),Henzi&Barrett(2002)proposed that female A will groom female B more often if female B has a newborn infant in order to gain access.Yu et al.(2013)described similar findings in golden snub-nosed monkeys(Rhinopithecus roxellana),indicating that infant handling might be interchanged for grooming.Infant handling is also reported as a social commodity to interchange for Affiliation,e.g.,embracing in spider monkeys(Ateles geoffroyi yucatanensis)(Slater et al.,2007).

    Grooming is a measure of Affiliative social relationships among non-human primates(Dunbar,2010).In some species,grooming accounts for as much as 10%–20%of an individual’s total daily activity budget(Lehmann et al.,2007).Previous studies have demonstrated that grooming can be interchanged for support in agonistic encounters(Seyfarth&Cheney,1984),mating opportunities(Barrett&Henzi,2001;Gumert,2007b),tolerance(Gumert&Ho,2008),food(Watts,2002),and infant handling(Gumert,2007a;Henzi&Barrett,2002;Tiddi et al.,2010).In tufted capuchin monkeys(Cebus apella nigritus),Tiddi et al.(2010)proposed that grooming is interchanged for infant handling among females,with grooming given by potential handlers increasing the likelihood of subsequent infant handling opportunities.Similar results have also been found in wild baboons(P.anubis)(Frank&Silk,2009),long-tail macaques(Macaca fascicularis)(Gumert,2007a),and golden snub-nosed monkeys(Wei et al.,2013).

    These previous studies suggest that both grooming and infant handling opportunities are valuable commodities in primates.Infant handling consists of an infant and adult female other than its mother.The groomer(female A)may interchange grooming to gain access to the groomee’s infant(female B,the infant’s mother). Accordingly,as a limited-competition resource,infants could be interchanged between the groomee(supplier,infant’s mother)and groomer(demander,potential handler,non-mother).However,the extent to which individuals perform grooming for infant handling or provide infant handling for grooming among females remains unclear.

    We used the biological market perspective to examine the interchange relationships between grooming and infant handling in Tibetan macaques(M.thibetana). Tibetan macaques live in multi-male-multi-female social groups(mean group size=33.8±6.79),with female philopatry and male dispersion(Li,1999).Tibetan macaques devote approximately 20%of their daily activity budget to grooming and thus it can be considered as a behavioral commodity in their social groups(Xia et al.,2012,2013).Similar to other macaque species,group members persistently attempt to interact with infants in multiple ways(Li,1999).

    We hypothesized the existence of bidirectional and complex interchange patterns between grooming and infant handling in Tibetan macaques.We tested the following predictions:if an infant is a limited and valuable commodity,(1)females with infants will be more attractive than females without infants,and females will receive more but provide less grooming after giving birth than before parturition;(2)with an increase in the number of infants,females with infants will receive less grooming from females without infants;(3)females with infants will receive more grooming if they first allow their infant to be handled;if grooming is an effective way to gain access to an infant,(4)females will obtain more infant handling opportunities after grooming the infant’s mother than they have under baseline social conditions.

    MATERIALS AND METHODS

    Study site and subjects

    This study was conducted at Mt.Huangshan National Reserve in Anhui Province,China.The reserve is a World Culture and Nature Heritage site and well-known tourist destination,as well as research site for the study of Tibetan macaques.

    The study site can be found within the reserve in an area known as the “Valley of the Monkeys”.There are two groups at the site:Yulingkeng A1(YA1,target group in this study)and Yulingkeng A2(YA2).Matrilineal kinship is known from historic and demographic data collected daily since 1986(Li,1999). The target group has been thoroughly habituated to close observation(i.e.,from<1 m)and all individuals can be recognized using physical features(i.e.,scars,hair color patterns,or facial/body contours)without disturbance or capture.During the study period,the group consisted of eleven adult males,thirteen adult females, five subadults/juveniles,thirteen yearlings,and eleven infants≤6-months-old(Table 1).We selected thirteen adult females and two subadult females for this study.

    Table 1 Birth records of infants in the YA1 group during the study period

    Data collection

    This study was conducted from July 2016 to January 2017 and from March to May 2017. All behavioral data were collected during an intensive study period over 201 days(average=25.1day/month,range=24–27). The social group was followed from dawn to dusk,and behavioral observations began at approximately 0700–0800 h and ended at 1700–1800 h each day(depending on the time of year).The observer maintained an observation distance of 5–10 m from the monkeys.

    Focal animal sampling and continuous recording(using a digital voice recorder)were used to score the daily activity of the focal individual(Altmann,1974),with the recorded data used as the baseline.Focal sample duration was set at 15 min(Li et al.,2007).We followed the sampling rules of Xia et al. (2012,2013)to avoid the influence of humans and double-counting social interactions.

    To investigate whether non-mothers were more likely to handle infants after grooming their mothers,we collected post-grooming(PG)samples. The PG samples consisted of 15-min focal observations of a non-mother who had just groomed a mother.To investigate whether infant handling by non-mothers increased subsequent grooming investment given to mothers,we collected post-infant handling(PH)samples.The PH samples consisted of 15-min focal observations of a non-mother who had just handled an infant of a mother.On the next day,at the same time,we conducted 15-min matched control(MC)focal observations matched with a PG or PH sample,with no grooming or infant handling preceding the focal observation(De Waal&Yoshihara,1983).We collected MC samples only if the two participants were in proximity(within 5 m).If the two participants were not in proximity during the MC observations and/or were involved in handling or grooming interactions within the 5 min preceding a planned MC or in the first 5 min of an ongoing MC,we postponed the MC until all conditions were meet.If a MC observation could not be conducted within one week of the PG or PH,the PG or PH was discarded.

    We used ad libitum sampling to collect data on duration of all grooming bouts between mothers and non-mothers that involved infant handling.We categorized an interaction as a grooming-infant handling interchange if a non-mother groomed a mother and handled her infant.This could occur in any order and any number of times.The interaction finished when the two individuals moved>5 m away from each other.We measured the total duration of grooming until the two females departed each other. We recorded the dyadic social relationships immediately when an interaction ended.According to Xia et al.(2012;2013),we defined grooming as any act in which a macaque(groomer)used its hand or mouth to touch,clean,or manipulate the fur of another individual(groomee).Infant handling behaviors included inspect,teeth-chatter,hold,groom,and bridge(Table 2).

    We used ad libitum sampling to record aggressive and submissive behaviors to determine dominance relationships.Aggressive interactions were defined as an individual threatening,chasing,slapping,grabbing,or biting another individual(Berman et al.,2007).Submissive behaviors were scored when an individual showed fearful interactions,such as fear grin,cower,mock leave,avoid, fl ee,or scream(Berman et al.,2004). All records of agonism were tallied for each focal female and divided into aggression given and aggression received.

    Data analysis

    We reported baseline data as mean±SE grooming duration(min/h)and infant handling frequency(times/h).We used a one sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test to examine whether the data conformed to normal distribution(P>0.05).

    Table 2 Definitions of infant handling behaviors

    We used the baseline data and calculated the duration of grooming received by mothers from non-mothers and given to non-mothers by mothers before parturition and six months after giving birth.Data were compared using paired t-tests to analyze whether mothers were groomed for longer periods after giving birth than before parturition,and whether mothers groomed non-mothers less after giving birth than before parturition.

    We collected data on 11 of the 15 females investigated for PG and PH.No data were collected for three adult females(TT,YZ,YM),who were older and less socially active in regard to infant handling and did not give birth or become pregnant during the study.One subadult female(THX)was not observed to engage in a grooming-infant handling interchange.We used paired t-test to compare the duration of grooming between focal samples and PH samples,and between MC samples and PH samples to determine if non-mothers handling infants promoted the non-mothers to groom mothers.We used paired t-test to compare the frequency of infant handling between focal samples and PG samples,and between MC samples and PG samples to determine if non-mothers grooming mothers promoted non-mother infant handling.

    We used the ad libitum data collected on grooming-infant handling interchange to investigate whether the number of infants per female was negatively correlated with duration of non-mothers grooming mothers during the interaction. We used linear regression to determine relationships between grooming and the number of infants per female.

    We analyzed grooming-infant handling interchange data to elucidate the effect of infant age on the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers during the interaction. We used linear regression to determine the relationship between infant age and duration of non-mothers grooming mothers.We assessed individual dominance rank by calculating David’s Score(DS).We also calculated linearity for the obtained dominance hierarchy(De Vries,1995;Gammell et al.,2003)Rank distance is the number of individuals ranking between the focal animal and a given partner,plus 1(Castles et al.,1999). In our study,the rank distance was the number of individuals ranking between the mother and non-mother,using the mother’s rank as the standard.We determined the sequence of social ranks based on the DS values,according to Gammell et al.(2003).YH was the highest ranked,followed by YXX,YCY,YM,TXH,TH,TXX,YCLA,HH,TR,TRY,THX,TT,YZ,and THY.

    We analyzed grooming-infant handling interchange data and used linear regression analysis to test the relationship between rank distance and duration of non-mothers grooming mothers.To investigate whether kinship between non-mother and mother affected the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers when infant handling,we used paired t-tests to compare the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers between kin and non-kin.

    To account for potential bias caused by pseudoreplication,we used factorial ANOVA to test for variation across individuals and number of infants,infant age,and rank distance on the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers. We found a significant effect of number of infants,infant age,and rank distance on grooming duration,but no significant effect on individuals or interaction effects. We found no evidence that individual variation existed and therefore no support that individual idiosyncrasies solely accounted for the results.Thus,pseudoreplication did not seriously affect the results.

    RESULTS

    During the study period,we collected 90 valid PG-MC samples,86 valid PH-MC samples,and 148 grooming-infant handling interchanges.

    Grooming variation among females

    The mean duration ofmothersgrooming non-mothers before birth(1.04±0.05 min/h)(mean±SE)was significantly longerthan afterbirth (0.62±0.07 min/h)(paired t-test:t=6.233,df=5,P=0.002),whereas the mean duration of non-mothers grooming mothers after birth(1.27±0.17 min/h)was significantly longer than that before birth(0.66±0.09 min/h)(paired t-test:t=–4.218,df=5,P=0.008)(Figure 1).

    Grooming variation between PH and baseline

    Forfocalsample observations,the mean duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was 1.52±0.13 min/h(mean±SE).After non-mothers handled infants of mothers,the mean duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was 3.02±0.22 min/h(mean±SE).The duration of grooming was significantly higher in PH samples than in focal samples(paired t-test:t=7.679,df=10,P<0.001).In MC sample observations,the mean duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was 1.74±0.08 min/h(mean±SE).The duration of grooming was significantly higher in PH samples than in MC samples(paired t-test:t=6.475,df=10,P<0.001)(Figure 2).

    Figure 1 Duration of grooming females gave and received before birth and after birth(mean±SE)

    Figure 2 Duration of non-mothers grooming mothers(mean±SE)

    Grooming variation with number of infants

    The number of infants per female was negatively correlated with the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers during grooming-infanthandling interchange (linear regression:R=0.624,F=93.271,P<0.001,R2=0.390,df=147)(Figure 3).

    Variation of infant handling

    In focalsample observations,the mean frequency of non-mother infant handling was 2.07±0.48 times/h(mean±SE).After non-mothers groomed mothers,the mean frequency of non-mother infant handling was 6.20±0.65 times/h(mean±SE).The mean frequency of infant handling was significantly higher in PG samples than in focal samples(paired t-test:t=7.642,df=10,P<0.001).In MC sample observations,the mean frequency of infant handling was 2.60±0.36 times/h(mean±SE).The mean frequency of infant handling was significantly higher in PG samples than in MC samples(paired t-test:t=5.858,df=10,P<0.001)(Figure 4).

    Figure 3 Duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was negatively correlated with the number of infants per female(mean±SE)

    Figure 4 Frequency of non-mother infant handling(mean±SE)

    Effect of infant age

    Infantage was negativelycorrelated with the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers during grooming-infant handling interchanges(linear regression:R=0.634,F=98.128,P<0.001,R2=0.402,df=147)(Figure 5). The duration of non-mothers grooming mothers decreased significantly with infant age.

    Effect of rank and kin

    The rank distance between mothers and non-mothers was negatively correlated with the duration ofnon-mothers grooming mothers(linear regression:R=0.277,F=12.088,P=0.001,R2=0.076,df=147)(Figure 6).

    There was no significant difference in the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers when infant handling between kin and non-kin(paired t-test:t=0.758,df=4,P=0.491).

    Figure 5 Duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was negatively correlated with infant age(mean±SE)

    Figure 6 Duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was negatively correlated with rank distance between mothers and non-mothers(mean±SE)

    DISCUSSION

    Infants are considered as commodities between a groomee(supplier,infant’s mother)and groomer(demander,potential handler,non-mother).However,the extent to which individuals groom in exchange for infant handling or offer infant handling in exchange for grooming among females remains unclear.In this study,we found that mothers received more grooming from non-mothers after birth than before birth,and that mothers groomed non-mothers less after birth than before birth.These results supported the prediction that females with infants are more attractive than females without infants and that females will receive more but provide less grooming after birth than before parturition.These findings are similar to that reported for golden snub-nosed monkeys(R.roxellana)(Wei et al.,2013).Furthermore,after non-mothers handled infants,the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers was significantly higher than that observed under baseline conditions,supporting the prediction that females with infants will receive more grooming if they first allow their infant to be handled. These results are similar to that reported for wild baboons(P.anubis)(Frank&Silk,2009). We also demonstrated positive evidence for the prediction that females will gain higher infant handling opportunities after grooming an infant’s mother compared with baseline interactions. Similar results have also been found in studies on golden snub-nosed monkeys(R.roxellana)and long-tailed macaques(M.fascicularis)(Gumert,2007a;Yu et al.,2013).Thus,we concluded that infants are a limited and valuable commodity and can be used by mothers in exchange for grooming.Furthermore,grooming is an effective way for non-mothers to access infants.Our results showed bidirectional and complex interchange patterns between grooming and infant handling in Tibetan macaques,and that the adaptive investment of females will change based on their social characteristics and group dynamics.

    A single behavioral exchange may not explain all behavioral relationships among individuals.In the case of reciprocity,pairs of actors are expected to form long-term and predictable social relationships so that,on average,the overall costs and benefits to each are relatively equal over time(Seyfarth,1977;Xia et al.,2012).In contrast,under conditions in which individual actors have the opportunity to interact with a diverse set of social partners who vary in the quality of goods or services each can provide at any given moment in time,individuals are expected to seek out partners who provide the greatest benefit at the lowest cost(No? et al.,1991;Wilkinson,1984).These interactions can involve an exchange of the same goods or services(i.e.,grooming in exchange for grooming)or the interchange of different goods or services.In Tibetan macaques,grooming can be exchanged for grooming(Xia et al.,2012,2013)and interchanged for tolerance(Xia et al.,2012). In our study,grooming was interchanged for infant handling.However,we suggest that a single exchange may not fully explain the behavioral relationship between individuals.The behavioral exchange model is complex,reflecting the evolution of individual adaptability and promoting relationship maintenance and group stability in primate societies.

    Market forces affect the interchange between grooming and infant handling. Infant age is a market force that can influence grooming payment. In our study,Tibetan macaque females groom other females for a longer period when theyhave a youngerinfant,with similarresults also reported in studies on long-tailed macaques (M.fascicularis),tufted capuchin monkeys(Cebus apella nigritus),sooty mangabeys(Cercocebus atys),and vervet monkeys(Chlorocebus aethiops)(Fruteau et al.,2011;Gumert,2007a;Tiddi et al.,2010).These results reflect that newborn infants are generally more attractive than older infants(Nicolson,1987),with infants get older,their attractiveness declined.Market theory predicts that supply and demand will influence behavior exchange among group members by altering the value of the commodities traded(No? et al.,2001).Under a biological market perspective,potential handlers attracted to infants represent the demand and infants represent the supply(Gumert,2007a;Henzi&Barrett,2002).In Tibetan macaques,with the increase in the number of infants,the duration of non-mothers grooming mothers for infant handling decreased significantly. Similar results have been reported in studies on chacma baboons(P.cynocephalus ursinus)and long-tailed macaques(M.fascicularis)(Gumert,2007a;Henzi&Barrett,2002).

    Dominance hierarchy is a factor that has considerable influence on socialinterchange in non-human primates.Dominance relationshipsmaymake socialcommodities cheaper for higher-ranked individuals and more expensive for lower-ranked individuals(Gumert,2007a;Henzi&Barrett,2002;Tiddi et al.,2010;Wei et al.,2012).In Tibetan macaques,rank can affect the patterns of grooming reciprocity among females;within the context of a biological market,higher ranking females have a wider set of social options than middle and lower ranking females(Xia et al.,2012). Our study indicated that dominance also altered the interchange between grooming and infant handling in Tibetan macaques,by reducing the grooming investment that higher ranking non-mothers gave to mothers when infant handling and increasing the grooming investment that lower ranking non-mothers gave to mothers.These results indicate that,in an infant market,lower-ranked individuals will pay more for interacting with higher-ranked partners,and rank might corrupt a social market and skew social exchange to benefit higher-ranked individuals.

    In Tibetan macaques,we found the interchange between grooming and infant handling to be bidirectional and to change with the age and number of infants.The interchange relationship between grooming and infant handling is an important part of social relationship maintenance and group stability in primate societies.It not only helps us to understand the characteristics of behavioral adaptation in adult individuals,but also highlights the social functions of infants.

    COMPETING INTERESTS

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    AUTHORS’CONTRIBUTIONS

    Q.J.and D.P.X.designed the study. Q.J.and K.H.P.conducted data collection.Q.J.and K.H.P.performed statistical analysis,Q.J.and D.P.X.wrote the manuscript,and J.H.L.revised the manuscript.All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    This study was conducted at Mt.Huangshan China.We give special thanks to Mr.HB Cheng and family for their outstanding logistical support during our study.We are also very grateful to Mr.SF Xie,YG Xie and the staff of Huangshan Monkey Center for their complete support and assistance.

    国产精品av视频在线免费观看| 男女之事视频高清在线观看| 免费看a级黄色片| 熟妇人妻久久中文字幕3abv| 看免费av毛片| 国内久久婷婷六月综合欲色啪| 亚洲欧美激情综合另类| 99riav亚洲国产免费| 欧美日韩中文字幕国产精品一区二区三区| 91字幕亚洲| 欧美绝顶高潮抽搐喷水| 国产激情偷乱视频一区二区| 国产精品久久久久久久久免 | h日本视频在线播放| 淫妇啪啪啪对白视频| 特大巨黑吊av在线直播| 禁无遮挡网站| 色播亚洲综合网| 九九热线精品视视频播放| 国产视频一区二区在线看| 日本一本二区三区精品| 免费看日本二区| 欧美在线一区亚洲| 精品国产美女av久久久久小说| 女同久久另类99精品国产91| 亚洲av五月六月丁香网| 色综合婷婷激情| 亚洲第一欧美日韩一区二区三区| 久久性视频一级片| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 亚洲av第一区精品v没综合| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 99久久精品热视频| 91久久精品电影网| 看片在线看免费视频| 午夜久久久久精精品| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产 | 中文字幕高清在线视频| 亚洲天堂国产精品一区在线| 国产精品久久电影中文字幕| 国产成人欧美在线观看| 性欧美人与动物交配| 国产97色在线日韩免费| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 成人无遮挡网站| 日本一二三区视频观看| 欧美成人一区二区免费高清观看| 色播亚洲综合网| 亚洲av第一区精品v没综合| 丰满乱子伦码专区| 深夜精品福利| 一级a爱片免费观看的视频| 91麻豆精品激情在线观看国产| 欧美午夜高清在线| 国产av在哪里看| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av在线| av天堂在线播放| 日本五十路高清| 12—13女人毛片做爰片一| 国产午夜福利久久久久久| 日本黄色视频三级网站网址| 欧美三级亚洲精品| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 老熟妇乱子伦视频在线观看| 久久久久国产精品人妻aⅴ院| 日韩精品中文字幕看吧| 亚洲专区国产一区二区| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 高清在线国产一区| 真人做人爱边吃奶动态| 国产精品亚洲美女久久久| 精品久久久久久久久久免费视频| 久久久久国产精品人妻aⅴ院| av中文乱码字幕在线| 亚洲av成人av| 国产亚洲欧美在线一区二区| av中文乱码字幕在线| 成人一区二区视频在线观看| 国产精品一区二区免费欧美| 国产蜜桃级精品一区二区三区| 国产午夜精品久久久久久一区二区三区 | 制服人妻中文乱码| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 九色国产91popny在线| 亚洲在线观看片| 亚洲中文日韩欧美视频| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看 | 夜夜看夜夜爽夜夜摸| 欧美日韩精品网址| 精品国产超薄肉色丝袜足j| 成人国产一区最新在线观看| 亚洲精品亚洲一区二区| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 欧美日韩综合久久久久久 | avwww免费| 黄片小视频在线播放| 国产亚洲欧美在线一区二区| 最近视频中文字幕2019在线8| 日本a在线网址| 一区二区三区免费毛片| 亚洲五月婷婷丁香| 嫩草影院入口| 久久久国产成人免费| 热99re8久久精品国产| 国产精华一区二区三区| 99热6这里只有精品| 国产成人啪精品午夜网站| 亚洲人与动物交配视频| 欧美成狂野欧美在线观看| 午夜精品久久久久久毛片777| 最近最新中文字幕大全免费视频| 国产精品爽爽va在线观看网站| 在线观看av片永久免费下载| 大型黄色视频在线免费观看| 99国产极品粉嫩在线观看| 亚洲美女黄片视频| 午夜两性在线视频| 欧美日韩国产亚洲二区| 欧美区成人在线视频| 神马国产精品三级电影在线观看| 免费高清视频大片| 俺也久久电影网| 欧美成人免费av一区二区三区| 久久天躁狠狠躁夜夜2o2o| 性色avwww在线观看| 午夜精品久久久久久毛片777| 久久草成人影院| 午夜精品一区二区三区免费看| 亚洲成av人片在线播放无| 在线观看66精品国产| 精华霜和精华液先用哪个| 久久久国产精品麻豆| 日本 欧美在线| 日韩有码中文字幕| 欧美成人免费av一区二区三区| 亚洲在线观看片| 国产亚洲精品综合一区在线观看| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| 亚洲最大成人中文| 日韩中文字幕欧美一区二区| 国产一级毛片七仙女欲春2| 亚洲真实伦在线观看| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av | 嫩草影院精品99| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 国产成人影院久久av| 在线看三级毛片| 天堂网av新在线| 欧美日韩国产亚洲二区| 小说图片视频综合网站| 国产三级中文精品| 久久精品国产亚洲av香蕉五月| 久久精品国产清高在天天线| 少妇的逼好多水| 亚洲av熟女| 亚洲av免费高清在线观看| 国产亚洲欧美98| 欧美国产日韩亚洲一区| 日韩欧美在线乱码| 免费看光身美女| 不卡一级毛片| 国产成人系列免费观看| 99在线人妻在线中文字幕| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品| 黄色视频,在线免费观看| 欧美zozozo另类| 精品欧美国产一区二区三| 人妻久久中文字幕网| 欧美3d第一页| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av | 国产三级黄色录像| 日本黄大片高清| 性欧美人与动物交配| 最近在线观看免费完整版| 精品一区二区三区视频在线 | 熟女少妇亚洲综合色aaa.| 国产 一区 欧美 日韩| 久久久精品欧美日韩精品| 国产精品,欧美在线| 欧美在线一区亚洲| 丰满的人妻完整版| 真人一进一出gif抽搐免费| 精品一区二区三区视频在线 | 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 在线观看美女被高潮喷水网站 | 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 亚洲无线在线观看| 激情在线观看视频在线高清| 一区二区三区免费毛片| 91字幕亚洲| 国产视频内射| 久久人人精品亚洲av| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 18禁国产床啪视频网站| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 国产成人aa在线观看| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线| 脱女人内裤的视频| 18禁美女被吸乳视频| 亚洲在线自拍视频| 欧美激情在线99| 国产高清视频在线播放一区| 欧美色视频一区免费| 亚洲 国产 在线| 99久久精品国产亚洲精品| 久久久久亚洲av毛片大全| 国产成人欧美在线观看| 亚洲真实伦在线观看| 久久久久精品国产欧美久久久| 国产成人系列免费观看| 日日夜夜操网爽| 两个人视频免费观看高清| 国产乱人伦免费视频| 性色avwww在线观看| 国内精品美女久久久久久| 亚洲成a人片在线一区二区| 国产不卡一卡二| 天堂网av新在线| 国产av不卡久久| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 夜夜爽天天搞| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 一夜夜www| 在线观看66精品国产| 一夜夜www| 成年版毛片免费区| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 国产精品一区二区三区四区免费观看 | 国产成人啪精品午夜网站| 欧美午夜高清在线| 国产精品,欧美在线| 高清日韩中文字幕在线| 婷婷亚洲欧美| 亚洲自拍偷在线| 国产亚洲欧美在线一区二区| 午夜福利视频1000在线观看| 亚洲天堂国产精品一区在线| 激情在线观看视频在线高清| 午夜福利高清视频| 亚洲国产精品999在线| 久久久久久久久大av| 亚洲精品乱码久久久v下载方式 | 悠悠久久av| 夜夜爽天天搞| 搡老妇女老女人老熟妇| 久久精品91无色码中文字幕| 女人被狂操c到高潮| 久久精品亚洲精品国产色婷小说| 久久久久亚洲av毛片大全| 午夜福利免费观看在线| 亚洲欧美激情综合另类| 亚洲无线观看免费| 999久久久精品免费观看国产| 午夜a级毛片| 18禁美女被吸乳视频| 无人区码免费观看不卡| netflix在线观看网站| 国产三级中文精品| 国产一区二区三区在线臀色熟女| 国内精品久久久久久久电影| 91九色精品人成在线观看| 国产精品影院久久| 国产精品自产拍在线观看55亚洲| 麻豆成人午夜福利视频| 成人欧美大片| 色在线成人网| 啪啪无遮挡十八禁网站| 亚洲人成网站高清观看| av在线蜜桃| 国产精品久久视频播放| 日韩欧美在线二视频| 人妻丰满熟妇av一区二区三区| 欧美日韩综合久久久久久 | 国产91精品成人一区二区三区| 精品久久久久久久末码| 亚洲精品乱码久久久v下载方式 | 亚洲av免费在线观看| 午夜精品一区二区三区免费看| 在线十欧美十亚洲十日本专区| 国产亚洲精品久久久久久毛片| 嫩草影视91久久| 宅男免费午夜| 欧美日韩乱码在线| 人妻久久中文字幕网| 国产精品99久久99久久久不卡| 免费电影在线观看免费观看| 日本一二三区视频观看| 国产成人av激情在线播放| 亚洲精品一区av在线观看| 精品久久久久久久毛片微露脸| 老司机福利观看| 一区二区三区高清视频在线| 99在线人妻在线中文字幕| 真人一进一出gif抽搐免费| 免费一级毛片在线播放高清视频| 午夜福利在线在线| 少妇的逼好多水| 亚洲人成伊人成综合网2020| 国产探花极品一区二区| www.色视频.com| 国产成人av教育| 亚洲最大成人中文| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| 首页视频小说图片口味搜索| 国产视频一区二区在线看| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| 成年女人毛片免费观看观看9| 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品| 内地一区二区视频在线| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 国产成人欧美在线观看| 日本黄色视频三级网站网址| 搡老妇女老女人老熟妇| 啦啦啦韩国在线观看视频| 久久亚洲精品不卡| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产 | 亚洲,欧美精品.| 真人一进一出gif抽搐免费| 成人精品一区二区免费| 久久精品人妻少妇| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清| 久99久视频精品免费| 中亚洲国语对白在线视频| 国产精品 欧美亚洲| 亚洲成人免费电影在线观看| 少妇的逼好多水| 亚洲成a人片在线一区二区| 在线十欧美十亚洲十日本专区| 男女做爰动态图高潮gif福利片| avwww免费| 禁无遮挡网站| 欧美高清成人免费视频www| 少妇丰满av| 在线观看日韩欧美| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 一卡2卡三卡四卡精品乱码亚洲| 欧美乱色亚洲激情| 国产精品久久久久久精品电影| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频| 看免费av毛片| 无遮挡黄片免费观看| 国产精品女同一区二区软件 | 国产亚洲精品一区二区www| 中文字幕av成人在线电影| 国产精品一区二区三区四区久久| 有码 亚洲区| 很黄的视频免费| 九色国产91popny在线| 在线a可以看的网站| 日韩成人在线观看一区二区三区| aaaaa片日本免费| 在线国产一区二区在线| 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 内射极品少妇av片p| 岛国在线观看网站| 动漫黄色视频在线观看| 精品人妻1区二区| 男人舔女人下体高潮全视频| 99精品欧美一区二区三区四区| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 夜夜爽天天搞| 91在线精品国自产拍蜜月 | 欧美xxxx黑人xx丫x性爽| 最近最新免费中文字幕在线| 麻豆成人午夜福利视频| 国产精品 欧美亚洲| 国产熟女xx| 老汉色∧v一级毛片| 又黄又爽又免费观看的视频| 午夜亚洲福利在线播放| 久久久久九九精品影院| 国产麻豆成人av免费视频| 女人被狂操c到高潮| 国产三级黄色录像| 两个人视频免费观看高清| 成年人黄色毛片网站| 精品熟女少妇八av免费久了| 99热这里只有精品一区| 日本三级黄在线观看| 国产 一区 欧美 日韩| 人妻丰满熟妇av一区二区三区| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看 | 国模一区二区三区四区视频| 黄片大片在线免费观看| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 国产成人av激情在线播放| 国产免费男女视频| 麻豆久久精品国产亚洲av| 欧美又色又爽又黄视频| 国产一区二区三区视频了| eeuss影院久久| 午夜老司机福利剧场| 成年女人毛片免费观看观看9| 亚洲av五月六月丁香网| 国产精品一区二区三区四区免费观看 | 韩国av一区二区三区四区| 国产亚洲精品av在线| 日韩精品中文字幕看吧| 麻豆久久精品国产亚洲av| 午夜福利高清视频| 男女做爰动态图高潮gif福利片| 久久久成人免费电影| 免费高清视频大片| 国产精品日韩av在线免费观看| 最近最新中文字幕大全免费视频| 在线观看免费午夜福利视频| 国产v大片淫在线免费观看| 国产精品电影一区二区三区| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 久久久久久人人人人人| 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 国产三级黄色录像| 亚洲熟妇熟女久久| 久久久久久久精品吃奶| 久久久久久久亚洲中文字幕 | xxx96com| 午夜福利视频1000在线观看| 中国美女看黄片| 51国产日韩欧美| 欧美xxxx黑人xx丫x性爽| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 午夜福利高清视频| 日本黄大片高清| 成人特级黄色片久久久久久久| 好男人电影高清在线观看| 国产毛片a区久久久久| 免费看日本二区| 中亚洲国语对白在线视频| 免费看十八禁软件| 亚洲最大成人手机在线| 91在线精品国自产拍蜜月 | 3wmmmm亚洲av在线观看| 国产精品综合久久久久久久免费| 成人特级黄色片久久久久久久| 神马国产精品三级电影在线观看| 日本免费一区二区三区高清不卡| 91久久精品电影网| 国产亚洲精品久久久久久毛片| 97超视频在线观看视频| 青草久久国产| 人妻久久中文字幕网| 国产一级毛片七仙女欲春2| 欧美午夜高清在线| 亚洲av熟女| 精品日产1卡2卡| 久久久久国产精品人妻aⅴ院| 日韩精品青青久久久久久| 久久精品夜夜夜夜夜久久蜜豆| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 老司机午夜十八禁免费视频| 99久久综合精品五月天人人| 9191精品国产免费久久| 色在线成人网| 叶爱在线成人免费视频播放| 国产中年淑女户外野战色| 日本熟妇午夜| 日韩精品青青久久久久久| 亚洲无线在线观看| 一区二区三区免费毛片| 国产成人啪精品午夜网站| 亚洲中文字幕日韩| avwww免费| 午夜福利视频1000在线观看| 亚洲天堂国产精品一区在线| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱 | 亚洲精品一卡2卡三卡4卡5卡| 亚洲成人精品中文字幕电影| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 亚洲人与动物交配视频| 黄色日韩在线| 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 1024手机看黄色片| 99精品在免费线老司机午夜| 一进一出抽搐gif免费好疼| 亚洲国产欧洲综合997久久,| 国产黄色小视频在线观看| 国产色婷婷99| 12—13女人毛片做爰片一| 麻豆国产97在线/欧美| 色视频www国产| 亚洲一区二区三区不卡视频| 村上凉子中文字幕在线| 嫩草影院入口| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区| 欧美中文综合在线视频| 国产成+人综合+亚洲专区| av中文乱码字幕在线| 午夜a级毛片| 国产一区二区激情短视频| 夜夜爽天天搞| 亚洲人成电影免费在线| 亚洲男人的天堂狠狠| 真人做人爱边吃奶动态| 白带黄色成豆腐渣| 国产乱人视频| 99精品欧美一区二区三区四区| 性色av乱码一区二区三区2| 欧美色视频一区免费| 欧美国产日韩亚洲一区| 欧美绝顶高潮抽搐喷水| 国产亚洲精品av在线| 9191精品国产免费久久| 亚洲精品一区av在线观看| 亚洲狠狠婷婷综合久久图片| 国内精品久久久久久久电影| 又黄又粗又硬又大视频| 啦啦啦免费观看视频1| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 美女高潮的动态| 看免费av毛片| 亚洲国产精品sss在线观看| 欧美成人性av电影在线观看| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 国产三级中文精品| 哪里可以看免费的av片| 老司机午夜福利在线观看视频| 亚洲中文日韩欧美视频| 久久精品人妻少妇| 午夜亚洲福利在线播放| 日本a在线网址| 欧美色欧美亚洲另类二区| 国产探花极品一区二区| 国产蜜桃级精品一区二区三区| 黄片大片在线免费观看| 精品久久久久久久久久免费视频| 亚洲专区中文字幕在线| 午夜福利视频1000在线观看| 日本熟妇午夜| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 欧美在线一区亚洲| 日本熟妇午夜| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 久久久久久九九精品二区国产| 在线观看日韩欧美| 亚洲色图av天堂| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| 一本综合久久免费| 免费高清视频大片| 亚洲欧美精品综合久久99| 999久久久精品免费观看国产| 毛片女人毛片| 国产精品亚洲一级av第二区| 长腿黑丝高跟| 国产综合懂色| 老司机在亚洲福利影院| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 亚洲成a人片在线一区二区| h日本视频在线播放| 两性午夜刺激爽爽歪歪视频在线观看| 成人国产一区最新在线观看| 99国产精品一区二区蜜桃av| 国产精品野战在线观看| 国产欧美日韩一区二区三| 操出白浆在线播放| 国产伦精品一区二区三区四那| 色噜噜av男人的天堂激情| 最近在线观看免费完整版| 内射极品少妇av片p| 国产91精品成人一区二区三区| 精品99又大又爽又粗少妇毛片 | 天堂av国产一区二区熟女人妻| 国产av在哪里看| 91在线精品国自产拍蜜月 | 天堂av国产一区二区熟女人妻| 亚洲自拍偷在线| 欧美在线一区亚洲| 亚洲午夜理论影院| 97人妻精品一区二区三区麻豆| 午夜精品在线福利| 亚洲无线观看免费| 国产欧美日韩精品亚洲av| 无人区码免费观看不卡| 又黄又粗又硬又大视频| 国产91精品成人一区二区三区| 一本久久中文字幕| 免费观看精品视频网站| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 九九热线精品视视频播放| 色视频www国产| 欧美日韩综合久久久久久 | 在线观看免费午夜福利视频| 亚洲午夜理论影院| 精品国产三级普通话版| 波多野结衣高清作品| 少妇人妻精品综合一区二区 | 色哟哟哟哟哟哟| 在线a可以看的网站| 男女视频在线观看网站免费| 又黄又粗又硬又大视频| www.999成人在线观看| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 男女做爰动态图高潮gif福利片| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看| 午夜免费成人在线视频| 在线观看日韩欧美| 精品人妻偷拍中文字幕| 精品一区二区三区视频在线 | av在线天堂中文字幕| 99久久99久久久精品蜜桃| 一个人免费在线观看的高清视频| 久久久久久久久中文| 国产精品日韩av在线免费观看| 国产精品野战在线观看| 19禁男女啪啪无遮挡网站| 窝窝影院91人妻| 国产av不卡久久| 美女被艹到高潮喷水动态| 19禁男女啪啪无遮挡网站| 天堂av国产一区二区熟女人妻| 少妇的逼好多水| 真人做人爱边吃奶动态| 噜噜噜噜噜久久久久久91|