• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Complementiser and Complement Clause Preference for Verb-Heads in the Written English of Nigerian Undergraduates

    2018-01-25 08:48:34JulietUdoudomOgbonnaAnyanwu
    Language and Semiotic Studies 2017年4期

    Juliet Udoudom & Ogbonna Anyanwu

    University of Uyo, Nigeria

    1. Introduction

    Linguistic behaviour, whether in native or non-native linguistic environments, is determined by the ability of the language-user to make appropriate linguistic choices from a plethora of alternatives available in the relevant language system. Such choices may be made from the sound system, the vocabulary, the syntactic or the semantic system of the language in use, with the result that appropriate pronunciation is chosen for intelligible speech production to be achieved. Also, suitable lexical items and appropriate collocational patterns are selected for the construction of phrases, clauses, and sentences;and lexical items are utilized for the expression of intended meaning. The linguistic choices made by language users are expectedly informed by the existing linguistic principles governing usages in particular language systems (Lyons, 1981, 2008; Chomsky,1966, 1972; Radford, 1988), even though innovations and creativity are established as inherent properties of natural languages (Banjo, 1995; Yule, 2000; Chomsky, ibid.). For instance, Chomsky (1972) observes in relation to language users’ sentence construction practices:

    The normal use of language is innovative in the sense that much of what we say in the course of normal language use is entirely new, not a repetition of anything that we have heard before,and not even similar in pattern…to sentences or discourse that we have heard in the past.(Chomsky, 1972, p. 12)

    However, linguistic innovations and creativity are expected to be practised in conformity with the norms of the language in use, given that adherence to such norms make for uniformity in usage and cohesiveness within a particular speech community. Some syntactic studies have shown, however, that linguistic principles are not always adhered to; hence, appropriate linguistic choices are not always made. In English non-native linguistic contexts such as in Nigeria, the grammatical constructions of speakers of English as a second language have been observed to be fraught with deviant usages,resulting from inappropriate linguistic choices (Banjo, 1969, 1979; Adesanoye, 1973;Eka, 1979; Jibril, 1979; Jowit, 1991; Alo & Mesthrie, 2008, etc.).

    The present paper investigates an aspect of the syntactic construction of Nigerian users of English as a second language. It specifically examines the preference of complementiser and complement clause type for certain verb-heads by some Nigerian undergraduate users of English. The investigation seeks to determine and highlight the complementiser and complement clause types that are most preferred by the respondents:this is with a view to evaluate the appropriateness of such choices especially when viewed in line with subcategorization features of the verb-heads which select the complementiser and the complement clauses.

    The rest of the paper is organised as follows: in section 2, we provide an overview of clauses in English, while in sections 3 and 4, we present the methodology and data/discussion of the data respectively. Section 5 is the conclusion.

    2. Clauses in English

    A clause is generally considered to be a group of words with its own verb (finite or non-finite) and its own subject, and is capable of functioning as a single unit within the sentence context in which it occurs. Consider the sentences in (1):

    (1) (a) He claimed [that he was hungry]

    (b) The items [which were listed to be bought] have been given to him.

    (c) The men rented the place [when they arrived for the event].

    In (1a-c), the bracketed constituents are clauses functioning as noun object (1a),adjective, describing the noun ‘items’ (1b), and adverbial clause of time (1c). In the same way that clauses can perform object, adjectival or adjunct functions in their containing structures they can also realize complement functions in relevant/appropriate syntactic contexts, since the term ‘complement’ is not a categorial term, but a functional term just as subject and object (cf. Aarts, 2001; Adger, 2003). Traditionally, clauses in English may be distinguished into two types: those which are capable of independent existence and those which are not. The first type of clause is often described variously as a root, independent, super-ordinate, main, matrix or principal clause (Quirk &Greenbaum, 1974). The second type is referred to as a dependent, subordinate or minor clause. This type is so described because it is generally incapable of occurring on its own, instead, it is licensed by some other constituent within the structure in which it occurs for its meaning (ibid., p. 54). It is in this sense that subordinate (minor) clauses are also known as embedded clauses. In (1a-c) above, the bracketed constituents (even though each contains identifiable subjects and verbs) are not independent as shown in(2).

    (2) (a) …that he was hungry

    (b) …which was listed to be bought

    (c) …where the event occurred.

    Each of these needs a syntactic host to function as subject, object or complement (cf.Quirk & Greenbaum, 1974; Quirk et al., 1985; Borsely, 1991; Aarts, 2001; Adger, 2003,etc.). The focus of the present paper, however, is on clauses which function to serve as complements to V-heads in English and the kinds of complementisers which introduce them. We will therefore provide an overview on the nature and structure of the types of subordinate clause which regularly serve as complements of verbs in English.

    2.1 Overview of complement clauses in English

    As stated in the preceding sub-section, clauses which function as complements are typically subordinate clauses, hence they are referred to as complement clauses (Radford,1988, 1997; Borsely, 1991; Aarts, 2001; Adger, 2003; Moravcsik, 2006). Complement clauses are typically introduced by clause-introducers referred to as complementisers.In English,that,whether,forandifare examples of forms that can function as complementisers. The sentences in (3) exemplify complement clauses in English.

    (3) (a) We know for certain [thatthe government will approve the project]

    (b) The forecast could not really say [whetherit would rain tomorrow]

    (c) Both parties would obviously prefer [forthe matter to be resolved amicably]

    (d) They wanted to know [ifthey should come]

    In each of (3a-d), the bracketed constituent is the complement clause. As can be observed,each group of bracketed constituents has a word at the beginning of the group:thatin (a),whetherin (b),forin (c) andifin (d).

    Clauses which function as complements may be classified syntactically into three major sub-types, namely ordinary clauses (OCs), exceptional clauses (ECs), and small clauses (SCs) (Radford, 1988, p. 353). Ordinary clauses like those bracketed in (3) form an S-bar constituent with their immediate constituents: complementiser and sentence (ibid.,p. 294). Complement clauses described as exceptional clauses are typically of the form [NP to VP] as those bracketed in (4) below:

    (4) (a) I know [the Chairman to be honest]

    (b) Some believe [the verdict to be fair]

    (c) I consider [the flight to have arrived early]

    (d) They reported [the matter to be before a judge]

    As can be observed in (4), exceptional clauses cannot be introduced by an overt complementiser such asfor,if,whether, andthat, and this accounts for the ungrammaticality in (5).

    (5) (a) *I know [for the chairman to be honest]

    (b) *Some believe [if the verdict to be fair]

    (c) *I consider [whether the flight to have arrived early]

    (d) *They reported [that the matter to be before a judge]

    Thus, based on this property of exceptional clauses, they have the status of S and not S-bar since they lack the complementisers which are constituents of S-bar (ibid., p. 317). Small clauses on the other hand are those bracketed in (6).

    (6) (a) They want [Mr. Okpon out of the race]

    (b) Some house members believe [the Minister incapable of fraud]

    (c) Most people find [education quite exciting]

    (d) Why not let [everyone into one hall]

    As can be seen, the structure of the bracketed constituents in (6a-d) show that small clauses have the canonical [NP XP] structure, where XP may be instantiated by an adjective phrase, a noun phrase or a prepositional phrase. Also apparent from the structure of small clauses in (6) is that they have neither the C nor the inflection (I)constituents.

    The internal structure of each of the clause types shows that the ordinary clause (S-bar)contains both a C and an I constituent; the exceptional clause contains an I constituent but no C and the small clause lacks both the C and I constituents (ibid, p. 356). The small clause has also been referred to as a “verbless clause” (Radford, ibid.; Eka, 1994). Our focus in the present study is on the ordinary clause. Two reasons inform our focus on this syntactically determined clause-type. A cursory look at their constituent parts shows that an ordinary clause contains a complementiser, which, as will be clear later, determines a head’s selection of an appropriate complement clause. Also, verb-heads in English generally select complement clauses with the [C-S] structure. Thus, a complement clause usually contains a complementiser as an obligatory constituent and such a complementiser heads the ordinary clause (Radford, 1988, p. 295; Adger, 2003, p. 290). We shall briefly examine the structure of ordinary clauses in English.

    2.2 Internal structure of ordinary clauses

    Following the explanation of a clause offered in (2.1) as a group of words with its own subject and verb, the traditional phrase structure (PS) rule expanding clauses is (7), where NP is the maximal phrasal expansion of N, and VP is similarly the maximal phrasal expansion of V.

    (7) S→NP modal (M) VP

    However, as would be observed from the rule in (7), it does not seem to capture the constituent structure in which the subject NP is preceded by a C such asthat,for,whetherorif. Two possibilities regarding the constituent structure of clauses which contain C constituents have been put forward: first by Emonds (1976, p. 142) and Soames and Perlmutter (1976, p. 63) who note that C is generated within S as a sister to the Subject NP of the relevant clause by a rule such as (8), and second, by Bresnan (1970) who argues that a C and S merge to form a larger clausal unit referred to as S-bar (S’). Bresnan’s (1970)analysis incorporates two PS rules as in (9a) and (9b).

    (8) S→C NP M VP

    (9) (a) S’→C S

    (b) S→NP M VP

    The rules in (9) can be represented on a tree schema as in figure 1:

    Figure 1. Tree structure representation of an English S-bar constituent

    However, as Radford (1981) proposes, to accommodate both the finite indicative clauses as well as infinitival complement clauses within a phrase structure rule schema,and also capture the obvious structural parallelism between the N element in indicative clauses and the infinitival particle ‘to’, it is assumed that M and ‘to’ elements are members of the category inflection (I) (following Chomsky, 1981, p. 18). On this proposal therefore the basic internal structure of ordinary clauses is as specified in the two rules in (10):

    (10) (a) S’→C S

    (b) S→NP I VP

    I indicates whether the relevant complement clause is finite or non-finite. Ordinary clauses are therefore of the schematic form in figure 2.

    Figure 2. Tree structure representation of an English ordinary clause

    The present study partly follows both Bresnan’s (1970) and Chomsky’s (1981) analyses of the constituent structure of complement clauses in English. It further assumes that a subordinate clause which functions as a complement role must have a complementiser as one of its immediate and obligatory constituents (Radford, 1988, p. 295; Adger,2003). Due to the centrality of the C constituent in clause complementation, we shall provide an overview on the complementiser highlighting its status as a distinct linguistic category.

    2.3 Complementisers in English: An overview

    Complementisers denote a specific category of words and evidence for the classification of words likethat,whether,forandifas complementisers has been offered in Adger (2003,pp. 290-291) as follows:

    (11) (a) they occur at the start of (hence introduce) embedded clauses;

    (b) they form constituents with the clauses which follow them and not with the embedding verb of the main clause; and

    (c) they would move with their following clauses and not be stranded in the event of pseudo-clefting.

    Following Radford (1988, p. 302), it is assumed here that the C can be expanded into a bundlle of features such as (12).

    (12) C = [±WH, ±FINITE]

    The feature rule of the C constituent in (12) will generate the feature complexes in (13a-d):

    (13) (a) [+WH, +FINITE] can be filled by ‘whether/if’

    (b) [+WH, -FINITE] can be filled by ‘whether’

    (c) [-WH, +FINITE] can be filled by ‘that’

    (d) [-WH, -FINITE] can be filled by ‘for’

    Thus, the features of complementisers in English as specified (13a-d) can be summarized as in (14).

    (14) (a) that = [-WH, +FINITE]

    (b) for = [-WH, -FINITE]

    (c) whether = [+WH, +FINITE]

    (d) if = [+WH, +FINITE]

    The information in (13) and (14) can be expressed in syntactic and morphological terms on the basis of which Radford (1988, p. 302) classifies complmentisers in English. On the syntactic criterion, complementisers can occur in interrogative or noninterrogative clauses and are therefore specified as [+WH]; on the morphological criterion, complementisers may serve to introduce finite or non-finite clauses and thus have the feature specification [+FINITE]. Whereas [+WH] denotes the syntactic feature of complementiser, [+FINITE] specifies their morphological feature. The classificatory and distributional information about complementisers in English shown in (13) and (14)is summarized in Table 1.

    Table 1. Complementisers in English and their pattern of occurrence within complement clauses

    3. Methodology

    The data for this study were obtained from written responses (based on a free composition task designed to elicit grammaticality judgment intuition) of 420 Nigerian undergraduates(respondents) through a stratified random sampling method. The free composition task was designed to test the respondents’ most preferred choices of English complementisers and complement clause types for verb-heads in English within the range of complement clauses headed bythat,whether,orandif. The respondents were drawn from six federal universities: the University of Uyo, Uyo, University of Nigeria, Nsukka, University of Ibadan, Ibadan, Ahmadu Bello University, Zaria, University of Maiduguri, Maiduguiri,and University of Abuja, Abuja. The justification for the choice of six federal universities is based on the fact that the undergraduate population in the federal universities is representative of the ethnic nationalities in Nigeria, as well as the speakers of the various Nigerian languages. This is because the federal universities in Nigeria operate a state-bystate quota admission system which allows for admission of students in both the Sciences and Arts courses from the different ethnic nationalites (Hausa, Igbo, Yoruba, Ibibio,Edo, Izon, Tiv, etc), especially in states around where a particular federal university is located. Thus, in every federal university in Nigeria, at least six ethnic nationalities are represented.

    The respondents were given a written test which required them to fill out their complementiser preferences to complement clauses of certain verb-heads in English.Some of the complementiser/complement clause-types preferred by the respondents recurred both in the same respondents’ outputs as well as in the choices of other respondents. All the different complentiser choices were sorted out, analyzed, and summarized into a comprehensive list (Tables 2 and 3) .

    4. Presentation and Discussion of Data

    As stated earlier, data for this study were collected through a grammaticality test which was designed to determine respondents’ ability in selecting complementiser/clausal complements which are syntactically and semantically compatible with their associated V-heads. The data elicited from the respondents were analyzed and observed to feature small clauses, exceptional clauses, and ordinary clauses.The results of the study show evidence for the preference of complement clauses introduced by the complementisersthatandwhether. Thus, the complement clauses produced by the respondents featured morethatandwhetherclauses than complement clauses introduced by complementisers likeifandfor. By counting the tokens of occurrence of complemetisers and complemet clause types, and also calculating their simple percentages, it was specifically noted that the total number ofthatclauses was 128, representing 54.46% of the total number of complement clauses produced by the respondents, while the complement clauses introduced by the complementiserwhetherwas 73, representing 31.07% of the total number of complement clauses produced by the respondents. The complement clause type with the higher preference choice is described here as the “preferred choices”, while those with preference choice below 40% are, in the context of the present investigation, referred to as “l(fā)ow preferred choices”. The percentages of preferred and low preferred complement clause choices are shown in Table 2. Table 3 contains the actual instances of the complement clauses produced by the respondents.

    Table 2. Preferred and low preferred complement clause choice in %

    Table 3. Sample of complement clauses in the respondents’ outputs

    * The asterisk is used to indicate respodents’ structures whose grammaticality statuses are in doubt.

    4.1. Preference of complement clauses headed by the that-complementiser

    Respondents’ verb-clause complementation responses presented in Table 3 show that different V-heads select clausal complements introduced by different complementisers(Borsely, 1991; Haegeman, 1994), since the choice of a complement by a V-head is determined largely by semantic considerations (Radford, 1997).

    With respect to the choice of complement clauses, it is clear from Table 3 that the respondents showed preference for complement clauses introduced by the complementiserthat. The 54% recorded forthatclauses among the respondents may be indicative of respondents’ mind set, regardingthatas the appropriate complementiser in the particular contexts given the semantic properties of the embedding verbs as well as the morphological and syntactic properties of the complement clauses with whichthatenters into constituency.

    The first five entries in Table 3 show V-heads which subcategorise for clausal complements introduced bythat. The first two entries in Table 3 feature the V-heads, ‘told’ and‘suggested’. The respondents’ use of the V-heads, ‘told’ and ‘suggested’ shows that each of them takes a nominal and a PP complement in addition to subcategorized clausal complements. It is on this criterion that the two have been analyzed as taking two complements; the nominal/PP complement and the clausal complement headed bythat.The preference ofthatclauses as complements of the V-heads ‘told’ and ‘suggested’ is consistent with the feature rules in (13) and (14). The features of the complementiserthatare [-WH] and [+FINITE], indicating that syntactically,thatusually introduces non-interrogative clausal constituents, and morphologically it occurs in complement clauses whose verbs show morphological contrasts of past and non-past tense. On the semantic dimension, the V-heads ‘told’ and ‘suggested’ are classified as ASSERTIVE predicates (Bresnan, 1970, 1979) on the basis of which each of them selects athat-clause complement (Radford, 1997) which is [+DECLARATIVE] and introduces a statementmaking clause, and not an interrogative one.

    The respondents’ preference choice of thethatclause complements as shown in entries 3, 4, and 5 in Table 3, further demonstrates appropriate intuitive knowledge on the part of the respondents. The embedding verbs ‘thought’, ‘knew’ and ‘realized’ are classified semantically as COGNITIVE verbs (Bresnan, 1979), and on the basis of this semantic property, select clausal complements introduced by the complementiserthat.Each of the clauses in the entries 3, 4, and 5 in Table 3 possesses both the syntactic and the morphological features which clausal complements of the respective V-heads should select as complements. That is, the clausal constituents in entries 3, 4, and 5 are finite,non-interrogative clauses and that is why they are introduced bythat, a complementiser with the features [-WH], [+FINITE].

    Similarly, entries 6, 9, 10, and 11 exemplify felicitous choices by the respondents’showing that the verbal heads ‘preferred’, ‘doubted’ and ‘hoped’ are the verbs of the respective embedded clauses as shown in entiries 6, 9, 10, and 11. The grammaticality pattern in 6-11 is explicable in terms of the fact that, generally, verbs in English impose restrictions on the complementisers which introduce the complement clauses selected to complement them. Such restrictions are in turn determined not only by syntactic and morphological considerations (see figs. 6 and 7), but also by the semantic properties which relevant heads possess, such as MANDATIVE, ASSERTIVE, COGNITIVE, etc.(Bresnan, 1979).

    The embedding verb of Table 3 for entries 6 and 9 is ‘preferred’, and it is characterized semantically as a DESIDERATIVE predicate (Bresnan, 1979, p. 82). Given this semantic property, the verb ‘preferred’ can require a non-infinitival complement clause introduced by a complementiser with the features [-WH], [+FINITE], as occurred in the respondents’ output. The choice of athatclause therefore does not violate the C-selection restrictions of the verb ‘preferred’. Also due to its semantic classification as a DUBITATIVE predicate, the embedding verb in entry 10, ‘doubted’ (Bresnan,1979, p. 67) can require a complement clause introduced by a finite non-interrogative complementiser such as ‘that’ with the features [-WH], [+FINITE]. As is apparent from the data collected, an felicitous choice of the complementiserthatwas made, a choice which does not violate the C-selection principles of complement-taking predicate such as‘doubted’ (Radford, 1997).

    4.2 Preference of complement clauses headed by the whether-complementiser

    A 31.07% choice preference was indicated for complement clauses introduced by the complementiserwhetherin the respondents’ output. Considered against the preference forthatclauses discussed earlier (3.1), respondents’ choice preference forwhetherheaded clauses shows a 23.39% difference. This is significant since it suggests that the respondents were not aware of the linguistic fact that some complementisers possess morphological and syntactic features, which determine the range of complement clauses that they should introduce. With respect to its features, the complementiserwhetheris marked by [+WH], [+FINITE], specifying that it introduces finite interrogative complement causes in morpho-syntactic contexts. On semantic grounds(cf. Bresnan, 1979) thewhether-clause, since it is an interrogative clause itself, occurs after INTERROGATIVE and DUBITATIVE predicates. The choice of interrogative complement clauses introduced by the complementiserwhetherin Table 3, entry 12 is, therefore, consistent with the C-selection principles of the verb-head. However, an analysis of the constituent structure of the embedded clause in entry 13 shows that it is an infinitival sentence. This is signaled by the presence of the infinitival particle ‘to’,which precedes the verb ‘write’. The complementizerwhetherhas the morphological feature [+FINITE] and should introduce embedded clauses with a finite verb. This is not the case with entry 13 in Table 3. To create an appropriate morphological context for the complementiserwhether, the VP of the complement clause has to be finite so that the clause would read ‘whether the union should/could write to the president’.

    Entries 14, 15, 23, and 24 also feature complement clauses introduced by the complementiserwhether. As with the embedding verbs in entries 12 and 13, the embedding verbs of the complement clauses in entries 14, 15, 23 and 24 should require complementisers with the features [+WH], [+FINITE]. It is observed from the respondents’ output that the complementiserwhetheris chosen to introduce the complement clause in entry 14. This is semantically appropriate given the classification of the embedding verb ‘wondered’ as a DUBITATIVE predicate (Bresnan, 1979, p. 82).However, the choice of awhetherclause as the clausal complement of the verb ‘said’ in entry 15 violates the C-selection principle which restricts the choice of a head’s complement to one which is semantically compatible with the head in question—in this case the embedding verb ‘said’. In the entry 15, the verb ‘said’ is characterized as an ASSERTIVE predicate, and therefore, should take athatcomplement such that the entry would be:‘said [that government will increase prices of petroleum products]sincethatis a finite non-interrogative complementiser which normally introduces statement/declarative subordinate clauses.

    Respondents’ choice of the complementiserwhetheras the clause-introducer of the complement clause entry 23 of Table 3 conforms to the C-selection requirements of complement choice on the syntactic, morphological, and semantic criteria. On the syntactic criterion,whetherhas the feature [+WH] since it functions to introduce interrogative complement clauses. On the morphological criterion,whetheris marked by [+FINITE], and can therefore head finite or infinitival clauses in appropriate contexts. The embedding verb ‘decided’ in entry 24 of Table 3 is an ASSERTIVE predicate and should normally be complemented by a statement-making/declarative complement clause, and not an interrogative one as entry 24 indicates. Thus, even though the complementisersthatandwhetherhave a similar morphological feature[+FINITE], they have different syntactic features whilethatis marked for [-WH],whereaswhetheris [+WH]. The difference in syntactic marking makes respondents’preference for ‘whether’ inappropriate in the context of entry 24. The choice of awhetherclause in this instance is explicable in terms of the fact that in Englishwhether/ifare in complementary distribution tothat(Adger, 2003, p. 292): hence the difference in syntactic marking on the two complementiserswhetherandthatseems to have been blurred.

    4.3 Preference of complement clauses headed by the if-complementiser

    Table 3 indicates that a preferred choice of 8.94% was recorded in favour of the complementiserif, a clause-introducer, which, in contrast towhether, ‘can only introduce finite complement clauses’ (Radford, 1988, p. 302). Respondents’ choice of the complementiserifas the clause-introducer of the complement clauses ‘to accept the government’s proposals on the subsidy issue’ (entry 25) and ‘to send the union’s position to the Minister of Labour and Productivity’ (entry 26) violates C-selection restrictions on complements of V-heads on morphological grounds. As is apparent in (13a) and (14d),ifintroduces only finite subordinate clauses, hence it bears the morphological feature [+FINITE].Thus, even though the embedding verb is semantically an interrogative verb, the morphological motivation for its choice is not fulfilled in the complementation contexts under examination. The more appropriate morphological environment for the said complementiser in the two entries are shown in entries 25 and 26.

    Entry 25 ... wondered [if the union can/should accept the government’s proposal on the subsidy issue]

    Entry 26 … did not ask [if the union can/should send her position to the Minister of Labour and Productivity]

    Entries 27, 28, and 29 demonstrate respondents’ intuitive knowledge of C-selection restrictions on the complement clause. As is evident from the data, the choice of the complementiserifsatisfies both the syntactic and the morphological requirements on complementiser choice by the V-heads. Since the complementiserifis marked by [+WH,+FINITE], it is appropriate on these two grounds to introduce the respective complement clauses in the entries. Furthermore, the embedding verbs ‘wondered’, ‘knew’ and ‘a(chǎn)sked’are DUBITATIVE, COGNITIVE, and INTERROGATIVE predicates respectively, and require anifclause complement clause since it (if) is semantically compatible with the semantic properties of the verbs.

    4.4 Preference of complement clauses headed by the for-complementiser

    The complementiserforrecorded the lowest preference choice among the respondents.In terms of its inherent feature,foris specified as [-WH, -FINITE], indicating that syntactically it introduces non-interrogative complement clauses and morphologically occurs in infinitival clauses. The preference score recorded for this complementiser is 5.53%, as Table 3 indicates. This low preference choice may be attributed to the fact that the respondents in this study may have associatedformore with its prepositional function than with its role as a complementiser.

    In Table 3, entries 30, 32, and 33 demonstrate respondents’ familiarity with the semantic properties which V-heads ‘preferred’, ‘dying’, and ‘desired’ possess on the basis of which appropriate C-selection restrictions on complements were enforced. In entry 30, the embedding verb is ‘preferred’. Semantically, it is classified as a MANDATIVE predicate (Quirk, Leech, & Svartvik, 1985, pp. 155-157). Following this the complement clause which should complement the verb ‘preferred’ is one introduced by a noninterrogative infinitival complementiser such asfor. These requirements are met, henceforis an appropriate complementiser choice to introduce the complement clauses subcategorized for by the V-head ‘prefer’.

    The C-selection conditions for the complement clause choice for entries 32 and 33 V-heads ‘dying’ and ‘desired’ are satisfied since ‘dying’ is an EMOTIVE predicate and‘desired’ a DESIDERATIVE predicate (Bresnan, 1979). The complementiserforbears syntactic and morphological features which make it semantically compatible with the V-heads. However, respondents’ preferred choice ofthatas the complement clauseintroducer in entries 31 and 34 is inconsistent with the C-selection restrictions which the V-heads in the entries under study impose on the complementiser introducing their complement clauses. The embedding V-head in entry 31 is ‘was aiming’, classified semantically as a DESIDERATIVE predicate (Bresnan, 1979). It typically takes infinitival complement clauses introduced byfor, which is inherently specified by the features: [-WH,-FINITE], and notthat,which, as has been shown earlier (3.1) introduces finite noninterrogative complement clauses.

    Similarly, the verb ‘a(chǎn)stounded’ in entry 34, owing to its semantic properties as an EMOTIVE predicate (Bresnan, 1979), restricts the complementiser which should introduce its complement clause tofor, since this complementiser bears features semantically compatible with its own. We might say that the more appropriate rendering of entries 31 and 34 are as indicated below:

    Entry 31 … was aiming [for negotiations to commence soon]

    Entry 34 … was astounded the union [for the government to act in such a manner]

    Besides the inappropriate choice of the complementiser for entries 31 and 34 V-heads,the morphological criterion is not met. The clauses in the two entries are finite clauses signaled by the presence of the modals ‘will/can’, whereasforbears the morphological feature [-FINITE].

    5. Conclusion

    This paper has examined complementiser and complement clause preference choice in the written English of some Nigerian undergraduates. The analyses of the data obtained from the respondents showed that both inappropriate and appropriate complement clauses choices were made. The respondents’ outputs showed a general tendency for a high preference ofthatcomplement clauses in comparison to other types of clause. It is also observed from the respondents’ choices that complementisers constitute a distinct category of items, possessing idiosyncratic morphological, syntactic and semantic features which are sensitive to the choice of the type of complement clauses they introduce. Thus, the morphological, syntactic, and semantic features of a complementiser must be compatible with the morphological, syntactic and semantic features of the complement clause with which the complementiser enters into constituency. This is also in line with the fact that selecting predicates (V-heads) may reject certain complement clauses on account of the complementiser which introduces the complement clause. The consequence of the failure in satisfying this requirement results in some of the infelicitous complement sentences found in respondents’ outputs.

    Aarts, B. (2001).English syntax and argumentation(2nd ed.). New York: Palgrave Publishers.

    Adesanoye, F. (1973).Varieties of written English in Nigeria(Unpublished doctoral dissertation).University of Ibadan.

    Adger, D. (2003).Core syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Alo, M. A., & Mesthrie, R. (2008). Nigerian English: Morphology and syntax. In R. Mesthrie (Ed.),Varieties of English 4: Africa, South and Southeast Asia(pp. 323-339). Berlin & New York:Mouton de Gruyter.

    Banjo, A. (1979). Beyond intelligibility. In E. Ubahakwe (Ed.),Varieties and functions of English in Nigeria(pp. 7-13). Ibadan: African University Press.

    Banjo, A. (1995). On codifying Nigerian English: Research so far. In A. Bamgbose et al. (Eds.),New Englishes: A West African perspective(pp. 203-231). Ibadan: Mosuro Publishers.

    Borsely, R. D. (1991).Syntactic theory: A unified approach. London: Edward Arnold.

    Bresnan, J. W. (1970). On complementisers: Towards a syntactic theory of complement types.Foundations of Language,6, 297-327.

    Bresnan, J. W. (1979).Theory of complementation in English syntax. New York: Garland.

    Chomsky, N. (1966).Topics in the theory of Generative Grammar. The Hague: Mouton.

    Chomsky, N. (1972).Language and mind. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Chomsky, N. (1981).Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Culicover, P. W. (1976).Syntax. New York: Academic Press.

    Eka, D. (1979).A comparative study of Efik and English phonology(Unpublished master’s thesis).Ahmadu Bello University.

    Emonds, J. E. (1976).A transformational approach to English syntax. New York: Academic Press.

    Haegeman, L. (1994).Introduction to government and binding theory. Cambridge: Blackwell Publishers.

    Jowit, D. (1991).Nigerian English usage: An introduction. Ibadan: Heineman.

    Jubril, M. (1979). Regional variation in Nigerian spoken English. In E. Ubahakwe (Ed.),Varieties and functions of English in Nigeria(pp. 43-53). Ibadan: African University Press.

    Lyons, J. (1981).Language and linguistics: An introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Moravcsik, E. (2006).An introduction to syntax: Fundamentals to syntactic analysis. London:Continuum.

    Quirk, R., & Greenbaun, S. (1974).A grammar of contemporary English. London: Longman.

    Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., & Svartvik, J. (1985).A comprehensive grammar of the English language. London: Longman.

    Radford, A. (1981).Transformational syntax: A students’ guide to Chomsky’s extended standard theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Radford, A. (1988).Transformational grammar: A first course. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Radford, A. (1997).Syntactic theory and the structure of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Soames, S., & Perlmutter, D. M. (1979).Syntactic argumentation and the structure of English.Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Yule, G. (2000).The study of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    国产精品一区二区免费欧美| 精品国产三级普通话版| 国产乱人伦免费视频| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 村上凉子中文字幕在线| 国产午夜精品久久久久久一区二区三区 | 日本一本二区三区精品| 99视频精品全部免费 在线| 日本一二三区视频观看| 国产伦人伦偷精品视频| 此物有八面人人有两片| 亚洲真实伦在线观看| 男人舔奶头视频| 嫩草影视91久久| 成人三级黄色视频| 3wmmmm亚洲av在线观看| 一级a爱片免费观看的视频| 搡女人真爽免费视频火全软件 | 国产一区二区在线观看日韩| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 国产精品日韩av在线免费观看| 青草久久国产| 亚洲精品乱码久久久v下载方式| 99久久99久久久精品蜜桃| 成人高潮视频无遮挡免费网站| 午夜精品一区二区三区免费看| 十八禁人妻一区二区| 日本精品一区二区三区蜜桃| 成人精品一区二区免费| 天美传媒精品一区二区| av在线观看视频网站免费| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| 国产真实乱freesex| 我要看日韩黄色一级片| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 亚洲欧美日韩高清专用| 很黄的视频免费| 国产久久久一区二区三区| 757午夜福利合集在线观看| 两个人的视频大全免费| 色哟哟·www| 亚洲人成电影免费在线| 搡老熟女国产l中国老女人| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 听说在线观看完整版免费高清| 51国产日韩欧美| a级一级毛片免费在线观看| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 亚洲18禁久久av| 久久性视频一级片| 日韩欧美在线二视频| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 国产精品女同一区二区软件 | 麻豆一二三区av精品| 俺也久久电影网| 香蕉av资源在线| 久久久久久久久大av| 国产精品综合久久久久久久免费| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 成人欧美大片| 免费av观看视频| 99久久九九国产精品国产免费| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 亚洲美女黄片视频| www日本黄色视频网| 国产精品98久久久久久宅男小说| 成年女人看的毛片在线观看| 成人三级黄色视频| 九色成人免费人妻av| 中国美女看黄片| 99久久精品国产亚洲精品| 精品久久久久久成人av| 国产v大片淫在线免费观看| 久久久久精品国产欧美久久久| 最近最新中文字幕大全电影3| 久久国产精品人妻蜜桃| 男女那种视频在线观看| 国产白丝娇喘喷水9色精品| 国产一区二区在线观看日韩| 免费在线观看亚洲国产| 9191精品国产免费久久| 男女下面进入的视频免费午夜| 亚洲黑人精品在线| 久久精品国产亚洲av天美| av福利片在线观看| 琪琪午夜伦伦电影理论片6080| 日韩 亚洲 欧美在线| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av在线| 久久久精品欧美日韩精品| 国产日本99.免费观看| 搡老熟女国产l中国老女人| 国产真实伦视频高清在线观看 | 成年免费大片在线观看| 网址你懂的国产日韩在线| 国产伦精品一区二区三区视频9| 日韩欧美精品免费久久 | 听说在线观看完整版免费高清| 国产69精品久久久久777片| 欧美绝顶高潮抽搐喷水| 热99在线观看视频| 麻豆成人午夜福利视频| 国产探花极品一区二区| 国产私拍福利视频在线观看| 午夜激情欧美在线| 国产乱人伦免费视频| 91麻豆精品激情在线观看国产| 午夜精品在线福利| 免费在线观看日本一区| av在线观看视频网站免费| 午夜亚洲福利在线播放| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| 九九热线精品视视频播放| 成年免费大片在线观看| 欧美区成人在线视频| 天堂√8在线中文| 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 中文资源天堂在线| 他把我摸到了高潮在线观看| 精品无人区乱码1区二区| 国产精品亚洲美女久久久| 久久6这里有精品| 亚洲第一电影网av| 999久久久精品免费观看国产| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| 91麻豆av在线| 99久久久亚洲精品蜜臀av| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 97超级碰碰碰精品色视频在线观看| 国产免费男女视频| 久久99热这里只有精品18| 成人无遮挡网站| 久久人妻av系列| 99久久久亚洲精品蜜臀av| 一个人免费在线观看的高清视频| 日本三级黄在线观看| 日本黄大片高清| 90打野战视频偷拍视频| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 亚洲最大成人中文| 97超视频在线观看视频| 国产69精品久久久久777片| 一个人看的www免费观看视频| 久久久久久久久久黄片| 美女高潮的动态| 国产精品98久久久久久宅男小说| 久久久久久大精品| 日韩欧美在线二视频| 欧美日韩黄片免| 国产精品爽爽va在线观看网站| 老司机午夜福利在线观看视频| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av| 一进一出好大好爽视频| 欧美性感艳星| 国产精品电影一区二区三区| 久久精品人妻少妇| 欧美黑人巨大hd| 国产在线精品亚洲第一网站| 日韩人妻高清精品专区| 色播亚洲综合网| 亚洲av成人精品一区久久| 成人国产一区最新在线观看| 国产精品久久视频播放| 9191精品国产免费久久| avwww免费| 欧美色视频一区免费| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区 | 夜夜躁狠狠躁天天躁| 亚洲精品在线观看二区| 桃红色精品国产亚洲av| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 九色成人免费人妻av| 成人鲁丝片一二三区免费| 欧美最新免费一区二区三区 | 日韩精品中文字幕看吧| 国产国拍精品亚洲av在线观看| 国产视频内射| 亚洲精品一卡2卡三卡4卡5卡| 国产精品,欧美在线| 久久久国产成人免费| 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 成年女人毛片免费观看观看9| 99视频精品全部免费 在线| 亚洲无线在线观看| 亚洲美女搞黄在线观看 | 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| 亚洲在线观看片| 村上凉子中文字幕在线| 亚洲成av人片免费观看| 亚洲无线观看免费| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av| 在线看三级毛片| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av| 国产亚洲欧美在线一区二区| 国产在视频线在精品| 国产精品久久久久久亚洲av鲁大| 毛片一级片免费看久久久久 | 757午夜福利合集在线观看| 欧美黑人欧美精品刺激| 亚洲avbb在线观看| 一区二区三区高清视频在线| 国产精品久久视频播放| 亚洲va日本ⅴa欧美va伊人久久| 亚州av有码| 首页视频小说图片口味搜索| 12—13女人毛片做爰片一| av视频在线观看入口| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 亚洲精品日韩av片在线观看| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 99视频精品全部免费 在线| 亚洲av美国av| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 欧美国产日韩亚洲一区| 精品福利观看| 色综合亚洲欧美另类图片| 欧美黑人巨大hd| 日本在线视频免费播放| 亚洲av五月六月丁香网| 久久精品国产亚洲av香蕉五月| 午夜亚洲福利在线播放| 国产成人av教育| 夜夜躁狠狠躁天天躁| 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 深夜a级毛片| 精品免费久久久久久久清纯| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 久久久久久久亚洲中文字幕 | 一个人观看的视频www高清免费观看| 精品午夜福利视频在线观看一区| 欧美乱色亚洲激情| 亚洲自拍偷在线| 观看美女的网站| 极品教师在线视频| 国产乱人伦免费视频| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 亚洲国产日韩欧美精品在线观看| 成人av在线播放网站| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| 有码 亚洲区| 国内久久婷婷六月综合欲色啪| 久久久久亚洲av毛片大全| 国产成人影院久久av| 老司机深夜福利视频在线观看| 九九久久精品国产亚洲av麻豆| 久9热在线精品视频| 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 丰满乱子伦码专区| 久久精品国产亚洲av天美| 日韩中文字幕欧美一区二区| 免费高清视频大片| 18禁黄网站禁片午夜丰满| 中文字幕高清在线视频| 国产午夜精品久久久久久一区二区三区 | 免费人成在线观看视频色| 色哟哟哟哟哟哟| 亚洲av日韩精品久久久久久密| 夜夜夜夜夜久久久久| 大型黄色视频在线免费观看| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| av女优亚洲男人天堂| 欧美+日韩+精品| 亚洲 国产 在线| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品| 少妇人妻精品综合一区二区 | 国产亚洲精品综合一区在线观看| 国产av在哪里看| 听说在线观看完整版免费高清| 男人狂女人下面高潮的视频| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| www.色视频.com| 嫁个100分男人电影在线观看| 日韩欧美国产在线观看| 欧美三级亚洲精品| 变态另类丝袜制服| 日本撒尿小便嘘嘘汇集6| 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 怎么达到女性高潮| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| 中文字幕熟女人妻在线| 日韩人妻高清精品专区| 国产黄a三级三级三级人| 最近在线观看免费完整版| 在线国产一区二区在线| 日韩欧美精品v在线| 91在线精品国自产拍蜜月| 欧洲精品卡2卡3卡4卡5卡区| 国产精品亚洲一级av第二区| 长腿黑丝高跟| 很黄的视频免费| 国产黄片美女视频| 在线免费观看的www视频| www.www免费av| 99热这里只有是精品50| 欧美三级亚洲精品| 变态另类丝袜制服| 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 国产精品亚洲美女久久久| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 午夜免费激情av| 国产激情偷乱视频一区二区| 神马国产精品三级电影在线观看| 女人十人毛片免费观看3o分钟| 亚洲在线观看片| 久久国产乱子伦精品免费另类| 欧美zozozo另类| 老师上课跳d突然被开到最大视频 久久午夜综合久久蜜桃 | 日本与韩国留学比较| 99久久无色码亚洲精品果冻| av在线老鸭窝| 男人舔奶头视频| 一夜夜www| 别揉我奶头 嗯啊视频| 国产亚洲精品久久久久久毛片| 国产国拍精品亚洲av在线观看| 99久久精品国产亚洲精品| 老熟妇乱子伦视频在线观看| 波多野结衣高清作品| 亚洲人成伊人成综合网2020| 亚洲最大成人中文| 美女黄网站色视频| 国产成人av教育| 蜜桃久久精品国产亚洲av| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 国产野战对白在线观看| 久久亚洲真实| 国产av一区在线观看免费| 国产免费一级a男人的天堂| 男女之事视频高清在线观看| 日本三级黄在线观看| 三级国产精品欧美在线观看| 国产精品自产拍在线观看55亚洲| 亚洲久久久久久中文字幕| 夜夜看夜夜爽夜夜摸| 一个人免费在线观看的高清视频| 亚洲久久久久久中文字幕| 国产爱豆传媒在线观看| 成人午夜高清在线视频| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 好男人电影高清在线观看| 日本 欧美在线| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| 国产男靠女视频免费网站| 一个人观看的视频www高清免费观看| 国产色婷婷99| 国产乱人视频| 亚洲五月天丁香| 真人做人爱边吃奶动态| eeuss影院久久| 日韩亚洲欧美综合| 国产在线男女| 欧美bdsm另类| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 少妇熟女aⅴ在线视频| 国产精品一区二区三区四区久久| xxxwww97欧美| 亚洲 欧美 日韩 在线 免费| 国产黄色小视频在线观看| 十八禁国产超污无遮挡网站| 精品久久久久久久久亚洲 | bbb黄色大片| 又紧又爽又黄一区二区| 午夜亚洲福利在线播放| 啦啦啦观看免费观看视频高清| 精品人妻视频免费看| 日韩欧美精品免费久久 | 久久精品综合一区二区三区| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看| 一区二区三区激情视频| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 国产精品永久免费网站| 老熟妇仑乱视频hdxx| 亚洲欧美日韩卡通动漫| 久久久精品大字幕| 国产真实伦视频高清在线观看 | 青草久久国产| 伊人久久精品亚洲午夜| 亚洲av成人av| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| ponron亚洲| 亚洲国产精品成人综合色| 久久久精品大字幕| 搡老岳熟女国产| 99精品久久久久人妻精品| 激情在线观看视频在线高清| 精品久久国产蜜桃| 99热这里只有精品一区| 神马国产精品三级电影在线观看| 色精品久久人妻99蜜桃| 成人美女网站在线观看视频| 亚洲美女黄片视频| 亚洲成av人片免费观看| 久久久久九九精品影院| 国产精品野战在线观看| 搡女人真爽免费视频火全软件 | 国产老妇女一区| 国产精品自产拍在线观看55亚洲| 国产大屁股一区二区在线视频| av天堂在线播放| 国产亚洲精品久久久com| 日韩精品中文字幕看吧| 精品久久国产蜜桃| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 精华霜和精华液先用哪个| 别揉我奶头~嗯~啊~动态视频| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看| 亚洲av一区综合| 国产av麻豆久久久久久久| 黄片小视频在线播放| 精品久久久久久成人av| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 欧美日韩福利视频一区二区| 又紧又爽又黄一区二区| 极品教师在线免费播放| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添小说| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 国内精品久久久久久久电影| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 欧美黑人巨大hd| 国产亚洲精品av在线| 国产一区二区激情短视频| 老女人水多毛片| 亚洲自拍偷在线| 久久久久性生活片| 成人av一区二区三区在线看| 久久午夜福利片| 免费看光身美女| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 9191精品国产免费久久| 国产成人欧美在线观看| 搞女人的毛片| 90打野战视频偷拍视频| 热99re8久久精品国产| 又紧又爽又黄一区二区| 性色av乱码一区二区三区2| 午夜视频国产福利| 亚洲激情在线av| 极品教师在线视频| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 看片在线看免费视频| 午夜a级毛片| 国产精品久久久久久久久免 | 嫩草影院新地址| 中文资源天堂在线| 少妇的逼好多水| 欧美极品一区二区三区四区| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av在线| 简卡轻食公司| 国产午夜福利久久久久久| 午夜老司机福利剧场| 人妻制服诱惑在线中文字幕| 国产精品自产拍在线观看55亚洲| 夜夜躁狠狠躁天天躁| 一进一出好大好爽视频| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区| 亚洲真实伦在线观看| 18禁黄网站禁片午夜丰满| 亚洲精品在线美女| 色精品久久人妻99蜜桃| 欧美精品啪啪一区二区三区| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 天堂av国产一区二区熟女人妻| 久久久久久久久久黄片| 亚洲美女搞黄在线观看 | 赤兔流量卡办理| 国产高清视频在线观看网站| 国产大屁股一区二区在线视频| 久久这里只有精品中国| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 成人特级黄色片久久久久久久| 最近最新免费中文字幕在线| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱 | 午夜老司机福利剧场| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱 | 精品人妻熟女av久视频| 色哟哟哟哟哟哟| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频| 欧美区成人在线视频| 亚洲av熟女| 免费看光身美女| 免费大片18禁| 亚洲无线观看免费| 国产一区二区三区在线臀色熟女| 欧美最黄视频在线播放免费| 亚洲国产欧洲综合997久久,| 日本一本二区三区精品| 美女黄网站色视频| 亚洲国产精品久久男人天堂| 中文字幕人成人乱码亚洲影| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| av在线观看视频网站免费| 久久久久性生活片| 一进一出抽搐gif免费好疼| 亚洲欧美日韩高清专用| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 日本一二三区视频观看| av天堂中文字幕网| 久久天躁狠狠躁夜夜2o2o| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 欧美在线一区亚洲| 黄色配什么色好看| 最近中文字幕高清免费大全6 | 日本一本二区三区精品| 他把我摸到了高潮在线观看| 直男gayav资源| 国产精品av视频在线免费观看| 午夜福利成人在线免费观看| 搡老岳熟女国产| 精品人妻熟女av久视频| 久久香蕉精品热| 99riav亚洲国产免费| 一级黄色大片毛片| 久久久久国内视频| 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 俺也久久电影网| 亚洲 欧美 日韩 在线 免费| 国产亚洲av嫩草精品影院| 欧美日韩黄片免| 给我免费播放毛片高清在线观看| 亚洲成人免费电影在线观看| 日本一二三区视频观看| 国产午夜福利久久久久久| 亚洲最大成人手机在线| 999久久久精品免费观看国产| 久久久色成人| 国产精品综合久久久久久久免费| 热99re8久久精品国产| 午夜福利在线在线| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类 | 丁香欧美五月| 精品午夜福利视频在线观看一区| 色噜噜av男人的天堂激情| 一卡2卡三卡四卡精品乱码亚洲| av国产免费在线观看| 校园春色视频在线观看| 久久精品夜夜夜夜夜久久蜜豆| www.熟女人妻精品国产| 国产精品免费一区二区三区在线| 九色国产91popny在线| 日本精品一区二区三区蜜桃| 中文字幕久久专区| 国产黄a三级三级三级人| 看片在线看免费视频| 欧美性猛交╳xxx乱大交人| 老司机深夜福利视频在线观看| 女人十人毛片免费观看3o分钟| 国产精品野战在线观看| 久久久精品欧美日韩精品| 两性午夜刺激爽爽歪歪视频在线观看| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 激情在线观看视频在线高清| 嫩草影院入口| 永久网站在线| 国产成人aa在线观看| 97碰自拍视频| 国产精品亚洲av一区麻豆| 日韩中字成人| 精品久久久久久久久久久久久| 欧美午夜高清在线| 亚洲三级黄色毛片| 午夜福利免费观看在线| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱 | www.熟女人妻精品国产| 亚洲18禁久久av| 国产激情偷乱视频一区二区| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| 成人国产综合亚洲| 精品久久久久久久末码| 亚洲中文字幕一区二区三区有码在线看| 国产美女午夜福利| 午夜福利18| 国产精品永久免费网站| 国产精品免费一区二区三区在线| 丁香六月欧美| 极品教师在线免费播放| 成年版毛片免费区| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 日韩精品青青久久久久久| 亚洲精品456在线播放app | 男女床上黄色一级片免费看| 赤兔流量卡办理| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 全区人妻精品视频| 给我免费播放毛片高清在线观看| 大型黄色视频在线免费观看| 亚洲精品456在线播放app | 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 国产亚洲精品av在线| 欧美zozozo另类| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 很黄的视频免费| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看| 男插女下体视频免费在线播放| 国产爱豆传媒在线观看| 久久久国产成人精品二区| 男女做爰动态图高潮gif福利片| 国产综合懂色| 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 亚洲人成网站高清观看| 亚洲av日韩精品久久久久久密| 成人高潮视频无遮挡免费网站| 美女cb高潮喷水在线观看| 亚洲av成人不卡在线观看播放网| 少妇的逼好多水| 久久久久久久久久成人| 一边摸一边抽搐一进一小说| 三级国产精品欧美在线观看| 首页视频小说图片口味搜索|