• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Considerations for Observation as a Practice in Dance Training

    2018-01-23 23:06:25USMadisonMcGrew
    當(dāng)代舞蹈藝術(shù)研究 2018年1期

    [US]Madison A.McGrew

    I.Introduction

    The training dancers gain throughout their career is highly specific, uniquely shaping the way their brains process information related to movements beyond mere action execution[1].Not only does dance training change the way dancers execute movement,recent research has illustrated that dance training alters the way dancersseemovement[1].This phenomenon has generated a body of research that points to professional dancers for insights about the influence of motor expertise on action observation, execution,and understanding.While findings have emphatically advanced the conversation on how the study of dance contributes to advancements in our knowledge of learning and human cognition, there is currently little discourse on how those f ndings can be applied back to dance to further enhance training.

    In addition to the common actions shared by a lar ge portion of the human population, such as walking and grasping, a dancer’s motor repertoire is composed of specialised and intransitive actions that do not require a physical object or tar get to drive the movement or achieve a goal, such as pirouetting[1].One unit of this motor repertoire is termed a motor representation[1].Wilmerding and Krasnow maintain that the aim of teaching dance is to train these motor representations[2].The present paper will seek to explore the central f ndings in recent action-observation network (AON) research in relation to motor learning theory and praxis in order to highlight the role of observation as an important practice in training motor representations speci f c to dance.It will explore the chief question: How can our knowledge about AON be reinforced pedagogically to promote ef fective observational practices for dancers endeavouring to learn,refine, and maintain motor representations in dance class and while injured?

    II.The Action-Observation Network

    Dance exists for a multitude of reasons, from cultural integration to commercial entertainment;however, it can also be ar gued that dance, at its essence, exists to be observed.There is a transaction that occurs between performer and spectator, and the currency of this transaction is movement.One executes the movement and the other perceives it, thus introducing the concept that action and perception are represented within the same framework.The first forays into an action-perception system occurred in the 1990s using single-unit recording on the ventral premotor cortices of monkeys[3].The outcome of these recordings demonstrated that neurons in the ventral premotor cortex discharged when the monkey made specific movements and when the monkey observed the experimenter making the same movements[3].Thus,a mirror-like relationship was discerned between the visual action the neurons responded to and the motor response they coded for[3].The first evidence of these“mirror” neurons in humans came from studies using transcranial magnetic stimulation[1,4,5].The localization of such neurons in the premotor, supplementary motor,and parietal cortices nodded to the importance of the motor system for the observation of actions[1,6].The premotor cortex in particular encodes detailed action plans for complex movements, such as dance sequences that require information relating to its visual, auditory,temporal, spatial, and social features, its kinematics, and its composition.Research into the mirror neuron system suggests that action observation activates these action plans[4].

    After multiple studies were conducted on primates and humans using a variety of techniques, from motion capture to brain imaging, researchers corroborated the idea that when watching someone perform an action,an individual’s brain will simulate the performance of that action, and the notion of an AON emerged[4,6,7].The AON is a network that broadly encompasses all of the neural regions involved in visual analysis and motor sequencing, effectually unifying the processes involved in action observation and action execution[5,8].Under this union, it is conceivable that an observed action is understood from the inside as a motor possibility and not just from the outside as a visual experience[5].Moreover, evidence suggests that the interactions within the AON are not solely for the purpose of immediate imitation but are sensitive to the lasting ef fects of physical and perceptual experience as well[8].This provides the bedrock for higher mental functions, such as observational learning[4].

    When observation is considered in the context of dance, it has clear motoric objectives.Whether it is to perfect one’s own repertoire by watching the teacher or to synchronize with others in a group, dancers learn by reproducing the movements they observe[9,10].Therefore,it is reasonable to assume that motor stimulation through observation, as evidenced by the workings of the AON,has important applications for dancers learning, re f ning,and maintaining skills in the dance studio and while injured[4].

    In a study that used functional neuroimaging to characterize the neural underpinnings of observational learning with and without the added bene f t of physical practice, Cross et al.found that observational learning alone can lead to robust response changes within the AON[8].Findings from this study also indicated a high correspondence between neural regions activated by passive observation and physical practice[8].These active regions—the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the cerebellum—work together to assist in the procedural learning of motor sequences and the flexible recall of previously learned sequences, which is a critical facet of observational learning[8].Taken together, these f ndings suggest that physical and observational learning share common neural substrates[8].Therefore, action observation may be as ef fective as physical practice in facilitating dance training[8].

    Perhaps the most interesting aspect of Cross et al.’s research was that participants were not instructed to observe movement with the intent to learn or perform it[8].Hence, it would seem that observational learning occurs even when attention is not focused.This begs the question: If learning can be facilitated passively,what happens when observation is enacted with direct attention?

    III.Observation in Dance Class

    Attention is an important process in observational learning[11].When a person observes a model, he or she translates the perceived information into a code that forms the basis of a movement representation that is then stored in long-term memory in a process termed memory consolidation[11].There are four governing principles to memory consolidation, one of which is attention.

    Learning is best facilitated when an individual’s attention is focused on one task[11].One research study demonstrated that participants who engaged in an isolated movement-related activity during the time interval between the demonstration of a movement sequence and the attempt to reproduce that movement sequence recalled fewer movements than those who did not engage in the isolated movement-related activity[11].This decrease in recall suggests that a diverted attentional focus affects memory consolidation.In dance classes across all genres, dancers are seen“marking” movement sequences—or dancing the phrases in a less than complete manner—as they learn them[12].Ballet dancers might use their arms to mark what their feet will do during a barre exercise,or contemporary dancers might mark floor-work,walking up-right through the space while the teacher demonstrates the sequence “full-out” on the floor.Does this type of marking divert dancers’ attention and impact their capacity to learn?

    Marking for oneself can be seen as advantageous,particularly when a dancer is rehearsing a piece of choreography and wants to reserve ener gy but also desires to review sequence and examine spatiality.But what about marking for oneself in a dance class setting? Marking enables a dancer to physically simulate a process necessary to generate a conception of target motor representations that are more vivid and complete than mental images alone[12].Kirsh maintains that marking primes the neural systems involved in creating tar get motor representations, thereby activating cortical areas responsible for the full-out versions[12].Given that marking implicates motor preparation, spatial planning, and proprioceptive monitoring, it is proposed that even more areas of the cortex are activated in marking than in mental rehearsal[12].However, because the AON encompasses all of the neural regions accountable for perceptual processing, it can be ar gued that observing serves the same purpose as marking.And if motor programmes are indeed activated the same way in marking as they are in observing, why mark altogether?

    Kirsh contends that marking is also a method of anchoring a projection to a tar get—meaning that by having an external representation to “l(fā)ean on” a dancer can better conceptualize a movement idea[12].This comes from the notion of a distributed vehicle of thought, for which there is an inner part and an outer part that work in tandem to support a clearer thought[12].For Kirsh, the inner part is the mental structure and the outer part is the marked, physical structure[12].While this makes sense in the instance of the dancer rehearsing a piece of choreography, there is still question about the application of marking in a dance class setting.If,according to Hutchinson, the physical form does not need to be self-created[12], could not the external part on which the mental structure is projected be an observed demonstration?

    In a study conducted by Calvo-Merino et al.,dancers who viewed familiar movement material showed strong activation of brain areas that are consistent in episodic memory and contribute to recall from the longterm storage of allocentric information—these areas are the cingulate, retrosplenial, and parahippocampal regions[4].It is postulated that observation without a secondary attentional task, such as marking, could activate these more speci f c brain regions and reinforce memory consolidation.Although contrary to Kirsh’s belief that the addition of marking aids memory consolidation by reducing a dancer’s cognitive load,observing without marking supports the paradigm of augmented rest within a dance class[12].Augmented rest(or distributed practice) is not only beneficial in that it offers the dancer time for physical recovery, but it allows the nervous system time to process new motor patterns[11].Furthermore, observing could be superior to marking in reducing a dancer’s mental effort because there is no need for a dancer to conceptually represent the details of a movement phrase when the representation is already present.

    Cross et al.states that observational learning is not def ned by the type of instruction; rather, it is def ned by the learner not performing concurrent physical practice at the time instructions are provided[8].Thus, dancers should be encouraged to completely cease activity during demonstration to observe, re f ect, and interrogate presented information.As little as 12 seconds with no interference can promote memory retention and transfer better than the immediate repetition of a movement sequence[8].And in keeping with the consideration that observational learning is a form of augmented rest, Schmidt and Lee posit that the most successful strategy when employing demonstration is interspersing demonstrated information with practice attempts—as opposed to having all demonstrated information precede physical practice—so that the dancer can be engaged in the problem-solving aspect of learning while still receiving rest between attempts[13].

    The present paper makes a recommendation against dancers marking for themselves in dance class,but what about dance teachers marking movement phrases for dancers? In an ef fort to direct a dancer’s attentional focus during observation, it is recommended that dance teachers make the crucial aspects of a motor representation explicit so that students actively engage in the problem-solving process[2,11].There of course remains a question of if “crucial aspects” of movement even exist when, in dance, movement itself is crucial.Yet in light of the query over marking, it can be asked: What are the most crucial aspects to demonstrate full out, and what aspects are acceptable to mark? It is agreed that in a mark, only certain aspects of a movement phrase are attended to, but whether or not these are the “crucial aspects” remains uncertain.

    Marking for others is only reliable when used to convey information about space, position, and structural form—information that is grossly visually processed[12].In watching a mark, observers miss out on certain attributes, such as intensity, motion dynamics, and f ne detail that are important to learning motor representations.Therefore, if a teacher’s aim in marking is to prepare the dancer motorically, why mark? Moreover, marking is so idiosyncratic, especially in less codified dance genres, such as contemporary,that when one marks, observers may not perceive the full-out movement in the marked version unless the observers have been primed to the full-out movement previously[12].Consequently, marking is rendered ineffective when learning novel movement phrases.Dancers rarely mark a phrase they do not know; neither should teachers when demonstrating new material.Indeed, Magill stresses that when demonstrating key features of a movement skill, the demonstrator should be certain that all observers can clearly see the features[11].This suggests that dance teachers, if able,should demonstrate movement phrases full out.If not able, teachers should have a student demonstrate the phrase full out and provide feedback as necessary.Teachers should strive to mark only in circumstances when motor schemata have been stabilised through extensive practice (for example, in a professional-level ballet class).

    In a dance class, ensuring that all observers can clearly see the demonstration may warrant rotating students more frequently in the studio.It has long been presumed that the use of mirrors enhances learning,but there is evidence indicating visual information from the mirror is misleading, and thus diminishes kinaesthetic awareness and motor learning[2].Using the mirror to demonstrate or observe movement, especially when studios are crowded, may not enable the most effective visual processing.This is another instance when rotating a class may be beneficial.It can be argued that in a crowded studio, it is not advantageous to shuffle around that many individuals, and that the mirror is an invaluable tool to see movement from perspectives not probable without the mirror present.In that case, dance teachers, rather, should move around the studio themselves to demonstrate movement phrases from as many dif ferent angles and locations as possible.Nevertheless, the teacher should continue to demonstrate novel movement phrases as frequently as necessary because the more a dancer observes a demonstration, the more opportunity the dancer will have to acquire the movement pattern and parameterize the action[11].

    Research has invariably emphasised the importance of a visual model in learning.If a dance teacher cannot demonstrate a movement phrase full out, Wilmerding and Krasnow propose having a more advanced dancer act as the model for other students[2].However, there is evidence to suggest that the skill level of the model is irrelevant.According to one study, subjects who watched an expert perform a task and subjects who watched a novice perform a task exhibited the same level of benef t in learning the task compared to subjects who did not watch a model at all[13].On the other hand,there is research to support that observational learning is better facilitated with the use of a novice model over an expert model.Schmidt and Lee agree that novices get very little insight from watching experts unless they are specifically cued on what to observe[13].In another study, considerable learning was achieved in subjects who observed a modelled task and were subsequently presented with the model’s feedback in comparison to subjects who only observed the modelled task[13].Presumably, this is because not only does the observer perceive the visual or auditory information associated with the performed movement, but he or she receives the feedback from the model’s performance, and then witnesses the model’s attempt to apply the feedback to the next performance[13].Magill also adds that the observation of a novice by another novice discourages strict visual imitation of the model’s performance and stimulates the observer to engage in the problem-solving process[11].

    Herein lies the case for watching peers and attending to their feedback in dance class.While anecdotally, this is already a practise that is fostered by dance teachers, it remains a motor learning principle that merits emphasis.Within a class structure, dancing in groups is believed to help learners gain familiarity with movement phrases while allowing performers more space to move.When novel movement phrases are performed in groups, the individuals not dancing take to the sides of the studio and watch the individuals in the performing group.The dancers who take to the sides often use this time to mark the phrase as it is being performed.However, because motor stimulation and memory consolidation are promoted by observation alone, a strategy that discourages concurrent physical practice may be as ef fective as marking, but with the added advantage of being less distracting and more space-conscious.

    Lastly, a note on verbal feedback supplementing action observation: Visual information does not necessarily benefit from synchronous auditory information[6].A novice dancer or a dancer learning a novel movement phrase will need to divide his or her attention between performing the phrase and remembering any verbal instruction[11].Therefore, a minimal amount of verbal feedback should be of fered to a moving, learning dancer so as not to exceed the individual’s attention-capacity limits[11].Unfortunately,providing too much verbal information can be like providing no verbal information at all.For this reason,it may not be constructive for dance teachers to relay corrections while dancers are performing.Rather, it may be of greater benef t to both the moving, learning dancer and the rest of the class to wait until after the dancers have finished performing to of fer verbal feedback.Teachers should direct the correction to the dancers to which it applies, and have the rest of the class listen to the feedback and observe its consequent application attempts.

    IV.Observation While Injured

    The present paper has addressed how observation can be used to augment learning within a dance class,but how might observation be utilised to supplement an injured dancer’s rehabilitation programme?Seeking alternative ways for injured dancers to remain integrated in dance classes can positively support their healing[14].One of these alternate routes is through inclass observation.Daniels proposes that injured dancers be invited to observe classes with a guided writing response form[14].While the form is a practical idea for class settings in which a grade is required, there is reason to believe that it should not be completed during an actual class.Injured dancers required to attend to a guided writing response form while watching a dance class may become too concerned with responding to prove their engagement that they never fully engage with what they watch.If continued learning, motor maintenance, and class integration is the goal of having an injured dancer observe, then writing during a class may shift the dancer away from that goal.Moreover,Daniels submits that the injured dancer can opt to either observe the class as a whole or watch a single peer[14].Implications of the AON have only been extrapolated from research studies involving one individual observing another individual.Therefore, a dancer may opt to observe one peer at a time, as it is unclear if a dancer will receive the same benefit by observing the class as a whole.

    Mental imagery, a type of mental rehearsal that generates visual or kinaesthetic aspects of movement,has been chronicled for its success as an alternative dance training method in maintaining neuromuscular connections for the injured dancer[7,9,14].Yet because mental imagery and action observation share many cortical circuits and both strive to simulate motor representations, it seems logical that a practice of action observation could also be recommended to injured dancers as an alternate mode of training, either in combination with mental imagery or as its own modality.Furthermore, if mental imagery is regarded as fundamental to dance practice, then so too should action observation, for action observation is proven to enable effective mental imagery[7].

    Mental imagery and action observation have historically been viewed as discrete modalities, and there are mixed results across studies that strive to compare their advantages[7].However, there is new evidence that supports the super-additive advantages of their combined application[15].The basis for its aggregate value is that action observation removes the necessity for individuals to generate their own mental image while still providing visual, auditory,and temporal cues for successful performance[7].This in turn liberates attentional space for the individuals to image crucial aspects of the movement (whatever those may be)[7].Under this assumption, action observation could very well be the external part on which mental imagery is projected in a distributed vehicle of thought.Experiments cited by Eaves et al.advocate for the pairing of action observation and mental imagery as a tool for motor learning and rehabilitation.In one study in particular, cortical-motor activity was significantly increased during a joint exercise of action observation and motor imagery in comparison to a practice of action observation or mental imagery alone[7].Therefore, an injured dancer may receive the most bene f t in terms of maintaining his or her motor repertoire by observing a peer perform a movement while simultaneously imagining themselves perform that movement.

    In an experiment by Cross, Hamilton, and Grafton that investigated the extent to which observing and imaging actions requires prior physical experience,modern dancers were asked to either observe actions,imagine their own performing actions, or both observe and imagine actions[8].While there was an evident increase in brain activity with combined observation and imagination regardless of embodiment, the more embodied the movement was within the dancer,the stronger the brain activity was[8].This adds to the research of Calvo-Merino et al.that found that previous physical experience with an observed movement was necessary for forceful activation in the AON[4,10].Therefore, it may behove injured dancers to do everything within their physical ability to have an experience with a movement task in class.For instance, if a dancer is side-lined with a lower leg injury and the class is learning a novel movement representation, he or she should still endeavour to have a full-out experience with the upper body.In addition, Calvo-Merino et al.also revealed that there was greater premotor, parietal, and cerebellar activity in dancers who saw movements from their own motor repertoire[10].Therefore, perhaps a more inconsequential conclusion is that when observing a dance class with gender-specific movements,injured male dancers should focus on observing male movements, whereas injured female dancers should focus on observing female movements.

    V.Conclusion

    The present paper seeks to unpack principles from motor learning theory and evidence from existing AON research to construct critiques of and considerations for current observational practices in dance class and while injured.While it is true, action observation cannot satisfy all of what full-out physical practice can—dance, after all, is a physical art form—it can ef fectively facilitate and/or supplement learning.By considering observation in the dance studio as well as observation for injured dancers, it can be concluded that dancers and teachers should be more intentional in how they approach observation.Although dancers and teachers may feel that by merely observing, dancers are being “l(fā)azy,” it is actually true that observation markedly exercises the motor mechanisms imperative to learning, re f ning, and maintaining motor representations.

    There are a few limitations to the ar guments outlined in this paper.Because motor networks have not yet begun to crystallize in early development,this paper is more directed toward class settings and circumstances that occur during a dancer’s preprofessional and professional years.Arguments in this paper are based on the assumption that imaging is relatively easy for dancers, especially having not suffered from neurological impairment.Furthermore,arguments in this paper are constructed on evidence largely derived from still-image laboratory studies.Because of major methodological constraints, there are no studies that explore brain activity in the moving body or within a true dance environment.There is no certainty that studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation carry back into a dance context.

    日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 亚洲国产精品国产精品| 成人亚洲精品一区在线观看 | 亚洲va在线va天堂va国产| 中文字幕制服av| h视频一区二区三区| 国产精品一区二区在线观看99| 中文欧美无线码| 亚洲欧洲日产国产| 日韩亚洲欧美综合| tube8黄色片| 久久久久久久久久久免费av| 99久久精品热视频| 午夜老司机福利剧场| 如何舔出高潮| 性高湖久久久久久久久免费观看| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 亚洲欧美日韩卡通动漫| 国产精品蜜桃在线观看| 成人免费观看视频高清| 精品一区二区免费观看| 一级毛片我不卡| 午夜精品国产一区二区电影| 女性生殖器流出的白浆| 免费播放大片免费观看视频在线观看| av卡一久久| 欧美日韩亚洲高清精品| 伦理电影大哥的女人| 亚洲精品乱码久久久久久按摩| 自拍欧美九色日韩亚洲蝌蚪91 | 国产乱人偷精品视频| 伦理电影大哥的女人| 观看av在线不卡| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 亚洲精品第二区| 亚洲丝袜综合中文字幕| 亚洲激情五月婷婷啪啪| 99九九线精品视频在线观看视频| 久久久久国产精品人妻一区二区| 国产在线男女| 天天躁夜夜躁狠狠久久av| 国产成人精品婷婷| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 日韩视频在线欧美| 日韩av不卡免费在线播放| 高清av免费在线| 99久国产av精品国产电影| 中文在线观看免费www的网站| 一个人看的www免费观看视频| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 婷婷色综合www| 97在线人人人人妻| 日本-黄色视频高清免费观看| 色婷婷av一区二区三区视频| 一本一本综合久久| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 午夜日本视频在线| 国产视频首页在线观看| 亚洲av综合色区一区| 亚洲无线观看免费| 3wmmmm亚洲av在线观看| 99久久综合免费| 蜜桃久久精品国产亚洲av| 黄色一级大片看看| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| 亚洲va在线va天堂va国产| 观看免费一级毛片| 中文天堂在线官网| 亚洲av电影在线观看一区二区三区| 少妇 在线观看| 成人午夜精彩视频在线观看| 久久99蜜桃精品久久| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 国产精品久久久久久久久免| av天堂中文字幕网| 国产成人精品婷婷| 久久99热6这里只有精品| 欧美老熟妇乱子伦牲交| 日韩伦理黄色片| 高清不卡的av网站| 免费黄频网站在线观看国产| 国产永久视频网站| 亚洲精品国产色婷婷电影| 国产乱来视频区| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验| 在线观看三级黄色| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产| 免费人成在线观看视频色| 交换朋友夫妻互换小说| 日韩成人av中文字幕在线观看| 超碰97精品在线观看| 伦精品一区二区三区| 国产精品成人在线| 天美传媒精品一区二区| 亚洲图色成人| 纵有疾风起免费观看全集完整版| 成人影院久久| 国产精品国产三级国产专区5o| 久久亚洲国产成人精品v| freevideosex欧美| 久久ye,这里只有精品| 亚洲精品成人av观看孕妇| 亚洲欧美成人精品一区二区| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 在线 av 中文字幕| 国产成人一区二区在线| 亚洲欧洲国产日韩| 亚洲国产色片| 精品少妇黑人巨大在线播放| 亚洲欧洲国产日韩| 乱系列少妇在线播放| 欧美日本视频| 九九久久精品国产亚洲av麻豆| 欧美zozozo另类| 久久久久久久大尺度免费视频| 色综合色国产| 五月玫瑰六月丁香| 一级av片app| 黄色配什么色好看| xxx大片免费视频| 人妻少妇偷人精品九色| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 午夜福利影视在线免费观看| 黄色欧美视频在线观看| 亚洲成人中文字幕在线播放| 亚洲精品国产av成人精品| 免费少妇av软件| 22中文网久久字幕| 啦啦啦视频在线资源免费观看| av线在线观看网站| 国产乱人视频| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清| 香蕉精品网在线| h日本视频在线播放| 中国三级夫妇交换| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 十八禁网站网址无遮挡 | 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆| 国产黄色免费在线视频| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 夜夜爽夜夜爽视频| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 最近中文字幕高清免费大全6| 久久精品国产a三级三级三级| 欧美日韩一区二区视频在线观看视频在线| 免费大片18禁| 91精品国产九色| 国产高清不卡午夜福利| 日韩av免费高清视频| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| 黄色日韩在线| av又黄又爽大尺度在线免费看| a 毛片基地| 高清视频免费观看一区二区| 国产成人91sexporn| 简卡轻食公司| 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区 | 美女中出高潮动态图| 国产精品国产三级专区第一集| 亚洲欧美日韩卡通动漫| 精品一品国产午夜福利视频| 哪个播放器可以免费观看大片| 亚洲高清免费不卡视频| 久久婷婷青草| 91aial.com中文字幕在线观看| 毛片一级片免费看久久久久| 日韩一区二区视频免费看| 街头女战士在线观看网站| 卡戴珊不雅视频在线播放| 日本欧美视频一区| 日韩精品有码人妻一区| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 九草在线视频观看| 最近最新中文字幕免费大全7| 水蜜桃什么品种好| 久久国产精品大桥未久av | 亚洲国产色片| 人人妻人人澡人人爽人人夜夜| 少妇的逼好多水| 热re99久久精品国产66热6| 国产精品一区二区在线观看99| 精品久久久久久久久av| 欧美成人午夜免费资源| 交换朋友夫妻互换小说| 国产免费一区二区三区四区乱码| 97超碰精品成人国产| 午夜日本视频在线| 日日撸夜夜添| 日韩成人伦理影院| 国产成人一区二区在线| 免费观看av网站的网址| 欧美日韩视频精品一区| 美女国产视频在线观看| 久久99热这里只频精品6学生| 老司机影院成人| 国产亚洲av片在线观看秒播厂| 成人高潮视频无遮挡免费网站| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 亚洲欧洲日产国产| 日本一二三区视频观看| 高清在线视频一区二区三区| 三级经典国产精品| 看非洲黑人一级黄片| 日韩成人av中文字幕在线观看| 韩国av在线不卡| 久久久精品免费免费高清| 亚洲欧美精品自产自拍| 18禁裸乳无遮挡动漫免费视频| 日韩一区二区三区影片| 永久网站在线| 男男h啪啪无遮挡| 久久这里有精品视频免费| 精品久久久久久久久亚洲| 国产淫片久久久久久久久| 岛国毛片在线播放| 黑人猛操日本美女一级片| 99久久人妻综合| 久久久亚洲精品成人影院| 国产成人免费无遮挡视频| 亚洲三级黄色毛片| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 免费观看在线日韩| 一区二区三区免费毛片| 国产精品.久久久| 国产精品一及| 免费看日本二区| 大话2 男鬼变身卡| 国精品久久久久久国模美| 日韩一区二区视频免费看| 国产精品av视频在线免费观看| 日韩 亚洲 欧美在线| 国产精品.久久久| 高清av免费在线| 免费黄网站久久成人精品| 久久热精品热| 日本一二三区视频观看| .国产精品久久| 久久ye,这里只有精品| 最近最新中文字幕大全电影3| 亚洲国产高清在线一区二区三| 国产精品人妻久久久影院| 国产精品久久久久久精品古装| 国产精品99久久久久久久久| 国产成人免费观看mmmm| 久久久久久久大尺度免费视频| 22中文网久久字幕| 五月天丁香电影| 国产成人精品福利久久| 丰满乱子伦码专区| 精品久久久噜噜| 日韩伦理黄色片| 久久人人爽人人片av| 精品国产露脸久久av麻豆| 久久综合国产亚洲精品| 欧美变态另类bdsm刘玥| 国产男人的电影天堂91| 尤物成人国产欧美一区二区三区| 亚洲欧美日韩无卡精品| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 欧美日韩一区二区视频在线观看视频在线| 嫩草影院入口| 少妇高潮的动态图| 在线观看美女被高潮喷水网站| 在线观看一区二区三区激情| 精品久久久久久电影网| 特大巨黑吊av在线直播| 亚洲精品视频女| 日本欧美国产在线视频| 国产精品一区二区在线观看99| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 国产精品国产av在线观看| 久久久久视频综合| 亚洲精品乱码久久久v下载方式| 在线观看免费日韩欧美大片 | 色吧在线观看| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 国产黄色免费在线视频| 亚洲人与动物交配视频| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区黑人 | 精品亚洲成a人片在线观看 | 青春草视频在线免费观看| 又大又黄又爽视频免费| 在线亚洲精品国产二区图片欧美 | 国产成人精品久久久久久| 91精品国产国语对白视频| 日韩在线高清观看一区二区三区| a级毛片免费高清观看在线播放| 老女人水多毛片| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 欧美极品一区二区三区四区| 精品人妻视频免费看| 乱系列少妇在线播放| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 欧美成人a在线观看| 亚洲性久久影院| 久久久精品94久久精品| 日韩一区二区视频免费看| 免费不卡的大黄色大毛片视频在线观看| 欧美成人精品欧美一级黄| 亚洲真实伦在线观看| 在线 av 中文字幕| 高清黄色对白视频在线免费看 | 老师上课跳d突然被开到最大视频| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 国产69精品久久久久777片| 亚洲av欧美aⅴ国产| av不卡在线播放| 黄色日韩在线| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 伦理电影大哥的女人| 久久久久人妻精品一区果冻| 中文字幕亚洲精品专区| 久久人妻熟女aⅴ| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 亚洲人成网站高清观看| 蜜臀久久99精品久久宅男| 亚洲欧美清纯卡通| 久久亚洲国产成人精品v| 中文精品一卡2卡3卡4更新| 欧美3d第一页| 老司机影院毛片| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 国产日韩欧美在线精品| 久久国产乱子免费精品| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 亚洲不卡免费看| 日韩av在线免费看完整版不卡| 国产精品久久久久久av不卡| 久久久欧美国产精品| 99热网站在线观看| 国产成人aa在线观看| 岛国毛片在线播放| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av| 韩国高清视频一区二区三区| .国产精品久久| 男女边吃奶边做爰视频| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆| 久久精品人妻少妇| 成人无遮挡网站| 国产精品国产av在线观看| xxx大片免费视频| 亚洲精品中文字幕在线视频 | videos熟女内射| 久久久亚洲精品成人影院| 精品久久久久久久末码| 少妇的逼水好多| 日本av免费视频播放| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 国产女主播在线喷水免费视频网站| 热99国产精品久久久久久7| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 国产免费一区二区三区四区乱码| 精品国产露脸久久av麻豆| 能在线免费看毛片的网站| 一个人看的www免费观看视频| 久久6这里有精品| 伊人久久精品亚洲午夜| 久久97久久精品| 99re6热这里在线精品视频| 亚洲国产精品一区三区| 国产成人freesex在线| 嫩草影院新地址| 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 青春草亚洲视频在线观看| 亚洲人成网站在线观看播放| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 在线精品无人区一区二区三 | 国产男女超爽视频在线观看| 丰满少妇做爰视频| 日本黄大片高清| 2021少妇久久久久久久久久久| 成年美女黄网站色视频大全免费 | 午夜福利视频精品| 最近中文字幕2019免费版| 午夜老司机福利剧场| 国产av码专区亚洲av| 能在线免费看毛片的网站| 精品国产露脸久久av麻豆| 夜夜骑夜夜射夜夜干| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在| 免费观看的影片在线观看| 熟女电影av网| 一本一本综合久久| 少妇的逼好多水| 成人美女网站在线观看视频| 2018国产大陆天天弄谢| 成人午夜精彩视频在线观看| 丰满迷人的少妇在线观看| 在线播放无遮挡| 免费大片18禁| 成人免费观看视频高清| 中文字幕精品免费在线观看视频 | 国产精品久久久久久精品古装| 久久国产乱子免费精品| 欧美高清性xxxxhd video| 菩萨蛮人人尽说江南好唐韦庄| 涩涩av久久男人的天堂| 大片电影免费在线观看免费| 国产久久久一区二区三区| 视频区图区小说| 伊人久久国产一区二区| 国产一区亚洲一区在线观看| 午夜视频国产福利| 天美传媒精品一区二区| 免费av不卡在线播放| 国产在视频线精品| 亚洲精品aⅴ在线观看| 亚洲国产色片| 国产精品偷伦视频观看了| 蜜桃亚洲精品一区二区三区| 99久久精品热视频| 夜夜爽夜夜爽视频| 搡老乐熟女国产| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 久久久久久久精品精品| 亚洲经典国产精华液单| 99久久精品国产国产毛片| 大话2 男鬼变身卡| 久久久久性生活片| 哪个播放器可以免费观看大片| 国产v大片淫在线免费观看| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 久久精品国产鲁丝片午夜精品| 亚洲一级一片aⅴ在线观看| 成人综合一区亚洲| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 国产欧美亚洲国产| 欧美日韩国产mv在线观看视频 | 麻豆精品久久久久久蜜桃| 在线观看免费日韩欧美大片 | 日韩一区二区三区影片| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| 免费黄频网站在线观看国产| 欧美另类一区| 欧美激情国产日韩精品一区| 毛片一级片免费看久久久久| 男女免费视频国产| 亚洲精品第二区| 成人黄色视频免费在线看| 国产深夜福利视频在线观看| 又粗又硬又长又爽又黄的视频| 亚洲精品自拍成人| 最近2019中文字幕mv第一页| 街头女战士在线观看网站| 免费看日本二区| 国产伦精品一区二区三区视频9| 国产日韩欧美亚洲二区| 一边亲一边摸免费视频| 亚洲精品一二三| 嫩草影院新地址| 久久久久久久久久人人人人人人| 女性被躁到高潮视频| 婷婷色麻豆天堂久久| 极品教师在线视频| 一本色道久久久久久精品综合| 联通29元200g的流量卡| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 国产精品.久久久| 国产亚洲精品久久久com| 欧美精品人与动牲交sv欧美| 麻豆精品久久久久久蜜桃| 久久精品久久久久久噜噜老黄| 在线观看人妻少妇| 成人影院久久| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 色婷婷久久久亚洲欧美| 久久99蜜桃精品久久| 欧美高清性xxxxhd video| 大码成人一级视频| 国产成人一区二区在线| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 国产视频内射| 99热网站在线观看| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 国产av码专区亚洲av| 欧美精品亚洲一区二区| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| 国产免费又黄又爽又色| 欧美少妇被猛烈插入视频| 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看 | 国产乱人视频| 国产欧美日韩一区二区三区在线 | 草草在线视频免费看| 男人添女人高潮全过程视频| 日本av手机在线免费观看| 精品国产一区二区三区久久久樱花 | 欧美日韩视频高清一区二区三区二| 国产精品一区二区在线观看99| 久久久久精品性色| 精品久久久久久久久亚洲| 99热这里只有是精品50| 国产综合精华液| 99视频精品全部免费 在线| a 毛片基地| 日韩中字成人| 日本欧美视频一区| 日本av手机在线免费观看| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看| 欧美精品亚洲一区二区| 男女下面进入的视频免费午夜| 夜夜爽夜夜爽视频| 最近中文字幕高清免费大全6| 亚洲国产高清在线一区二区三| 久久久久性生活片| 亚洲av.av天堂| 蜜桃在线观看..| 亚洲精品久久午夜乱码| 国产免费福利视频在线观看| 久久精品久久久久久久性| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线| av专区在线播放| 欧美zozozo另类| 边亲边吃奶的免费视频| 嘟嘟电影网在线观看| 亚洲精品色激情综合| 国产毛片在线视频| 国产在视频线精品| 国产午夜精品久久久久久一区二区三区| 涩涩av久久男人的天堂| 亚洲国产精品一区三区| 一级毛片黄色毛片免费观看视频| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 亚洲无线观看免费| 亚洲综合精品二区| 欧美精品一区二区免费开放| 国产成人精品一,二区| 色视频在线一区二区三区| 成年美女黄网站色视频大全免费 | 国产精品久久久久久精品电影小说 | 精品一区二区三区视频在线| 国产一区二区在线观看日韩| av专区在线播放| 国产精品久久久久久av不卡| 亚洲精品日韩在线中文字幕| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆| 一级二级三级毛片免费看| 三级经典国产精品| 少妇 在线观看| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| 亚洲精品一区蜜桃| av又黄又爽大尺度在线免费看| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 免费大片18禁| 久久女婷五月综合色啪小说| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看| 国产乱人视频| 嘟嘟电影网在线观看| 日韩制服骚丝袜av| 精品视频人人做人人爽| 欧美性感艳星| 国产精品一区二区在线观看99| 精品一区二区三区视频在线| 汤姆久久久久久久影院中文字幕| 亚洲精品国产av蜜桃| 国产精品人妻久久久影院| 免费看不卡的av| 少妇猛男粗大的猛烈进出视频| 在线天堂最新版资源| 亚洲欧美中文字幕日韩二区| 午夜福利在线在线| 国产欧美日韩一区二区三区在线 | 亚洲无线观看免费| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 一本久久精品| 久久久亚洲精品成人影院| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 亚洲美女视频黄频| 涩涩av久久男人的天堂| 一边亲一边摸免费视频| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添av毛片| 免费观看性生交大片5| 久久国产精品大桥未久av | 一级av片app| 又大又黄又爽视频免费| 丝袜喷水一区| 欧美另类一区| 国产探花极品一区二区| 精品少妇黑人巨大在线播放| videossex国产| 两个人的视频大全免费| 国产精品国产三级国产专区5o| 最黄视频免费看| 免费黄频网站在线观看国产| 一级毛片电影观看| 欧美人与善性xxx| 97热精品久久久久久| 天天躁夜夜躁狠狠久久av| 欧美日韩亚洲高清精品| 大码成人一级视频| 91精品一卡2卡3卡4卡| 天美传媒精品一区二区| 国产高清不卡午夜福利| 简卡轻食公司| 精品久久久久久电影网| 欧美激情国产日韩精品一区| 国产精品一区www在线观看| 日日撸夜夜添| 免费大片黄手机在线观看| 亚洲精品成人av观看孕妇| 国产精品人妻久久久影院| 伦精品一区二区三区| 六月丁香七月| av网站免费在线观看视频| 晚上一个人看的免费电影| 777米奇影视久久| 国产精品99久久久久久久久| 黄色怎么调成土黄色| 永久网站在线|