• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Exploring Picturebooks Through the Model of Double-Scope Stories

    2017-03-11 05:55:24XiaofeiShi
    Language and Semiotic Studies 2017年1期

    Xiaofei Shi

    Soochow University, China

    Exploring Picturebooks Through the Model of Double-Scope Stories

    Xiaofei Shi

    Soochow University, China

    This article investigates the picturebook as the aesthetic object that integrates the verbal narrative and the visual narrative, and hinges on interactions between the two levels of narratives. Often drawing on the theory of semiotics, picturebook scholars have revealed the complex interactions between the verbal and the visual, the demands that a picturebook may put on the reader, and the hermeneutic nature of picturebook reading. Two significant issues, however, still need to be further explored: why the reader can possibly relate and integrate the verbal and the visual, and why some readers may turn to pictures more than to words, or vice versa. I propose to explore the two issues primarily through the model of double-scope stories within the theoretical framework of cognitive criticism. I argue that the universality of the intellectual and imaginative activity of conceptual blending makes it possible for the reader to relate pictures to words, or vice versa, and to integrate them. Though to make meaning from the picturebook as a double-scope story requires the reader to integrate the two levels of narratives, the verbal and the visual can sometimes constitute two relatively independent input stories. Readers may be more oriented towards either one of them, partially due to the difference in their innate cognitive structures. The arguments will be illustrated with a close analysis of Lane Smith’s picturebookGrandpa Green.

    picturebook, cognitive criticism, conceptual blending, double-scope story

    1. The Two Levels of Narratives in Picturebooks

    The word “picturebook” has become a household word in the area of children’s literature.“Picture” and “book” are put together as one word without a hyphen in between to denote the close interrelationship and interaction between the verbal and the visual,1which isdeemed characteristic of the picturebook medium (e.g. Hallberg, 1982; Schwarcz, 1982; Nodelman, 1988; Pullman, 1989; Lewis, 1996; Sipe, 1998; Nikolajeva & Scott, 2006). Picturebooks are not any books that contain pictures, but those where the visual and the verbal share in the workload of conveying meanings and developing narratives. The books in which pictures only serve as illustrations for words and do not add anything substantial to words are often excluded from the category of the picturebook. As mapped out in Nikolajeva and Scott’s important book-length studyHow Picturebooks Work(2006, pp. 2-6), current approaches to picturebooks include for instance the investigation of picturebooks for educational purposes, especially as a vehicle for facilitating socialisation and language acquisition (e.g. Spitz, 1999), the examination of picturebooks as art objects with a particular emphasis on the design and artistic technique (e.g. Lacy, 1986), historical and international surveys of picturebooks that focus on their thematic variety (e.g. Bader, 1976), and so on. Recent decades have witnessed a surge of aesthetic approaches to picturebooks—seeing picturebooks as aesthetic objects that consist in both words and pictures, and hinge on interactions between the two levels of narratives. Some scholars for instance draw metaphors from music to describe the visual-verbal relationship in a picturebook, such as the “duet” (Cech, 1983-1984, p. 118), or “counterpoint” (Pullman, 1989, p. 167) between words and pictures. Among various aesthetic approaches, the theoretical framework of semiotics has often been drawn on, because it can offer significant insights into the essential characteristics of and differences between the visual and the verbal.

    Picturebook scholars have revealed the complex interactions between the verbal and the visual, the demands that a picturebook may put on the reader, and the hermeneutic nature of picturebook reading. Lewis employs the notion of “polysystemy” to describe the reading of a picturebook as “the piecing together of text out of different kinds of signifying systems” (1996, p. 105). Nikolajeva and Scott argue that picturebooks communicate“by means of two separate sets of signs, the iconic and the conventional” (2006, p. 1). Iconic signs are those in which the sign is a direct representation of its signified, whereas conventional signs possess no direct relationship with the signified (p. 1). The examples that Nikolajeva and Scott give are: the picture of a printer is an iconic sign, a direct representation of the object printer; the word “print” is a conventional sign, as there is no direct relationship between the word “print” and the object signified. In a picturebook, the primary function of pictures, complex iconic signs, is to “describe or represent,” and that of words, complex conventional signs, is to “narrate” (p. 1). The reader’s movement between the visual and the verbal constitutes a hermeneutic circle, starting with the verbal, proceeding to the visual, and returning to the verbal with a renewed understanding and interpretation, or in the other way around (p. 2). The significant contribution of Nikolajeva and Scott is to develop a spectrum of the word-image dynamics. For instance, the visual and the verbal can be “symmetrical” with or “complementary” to each other. The verbal can “expand” or “enhance,” and even completely “counterpoint” (that is, contradict) the visual, and vice versa. In some cases, a “sylleptic” picturebook offers the verbal narrativeand the visual narrative independent of each other (pp. 8-26).

    Compared to Nikolajeva and Scott’s study, Sipe (1998) offers a more elaborate explanation of the hermeneutic circle of reading picturebooks by associating the hermeneutic circle with the essential difference between the verbal narrative and the visual narrative. Sipe builds his arguments on the tradition that Lessing (1766/1984) starts by distinguishing arts grounded in simultaneity of perception (such as painting, sculpture) from arts grounded in successivity of perception (such as music, literature). Simultaneity of perception corresponds to the mode of space in which we experience the world, and successivity of perception the mode of time. A painting and a sculpture can be taken in all at once, whereas in listening to a piece of music or reading literature, our experience unfolds in a linear progression of time. The hybrid art forms that are based on both simultaneity and successivity of perception include opera, drama, film, and picturebook. Jakobsen takes up the tradition started by Lessing yet with modifications. He suggests that words are not purely, butprimarilytemporally linear, and pictures are not purely, butprimarilysimultaneous or spatial (1971, p. 340). Drawing on Lessing and Jakobsen, Sipe therefore argues that the essential difference between the verbal narrative and the visual narrative in a picturebook creates a tension—the primarily temporal nature of the verbal urges us to read on, whereas the primarily spatial nature of the visual seduces us to pause, to stop. This tension gives rise to the hermeneutic nature of picturebook reading:

    to go backward and forward in order to relate an illustration to the one before or after it, to relate the text on one page to an illustration on a previous or successive page; or to understand new ways in which the combination of the text and picture on one page relate to preceding or succeeding pages. (1998, p. 101)

    Sipe moreover borrows the concept of transmediation from semiotics to account for the reader’s movement across sign systems in a potentially inexhaustible sequence (pp. 101-103). Sipe’s theory, however, still does not solve the significant issue ofwhy it is possibleto relate words to pictures, or in the other way around, to relate pictures to words, and to integrate the two levels of narratives. It moreover does not explain why some readers may turn to pictures more than to words, or vice versa. This difference in the way readers attend the visual and the verbal is particularly relevant to the discussion of picturebooks, if we remember that picturebooks are often shared between adults and children. Adults and children may display different tendencies towards the verbal or the visual—empirical studies have demonstrated that children would pour over the visual (e.g. Arizpe & Styles, 2016), while adults often rush through the plotline stated in the verbal.

    2. The Model of Double-Scope Stories

    I propose to explore two issues, that is, (1) why it is possible to relate the visual and the verbal to each other and to integrate the two levels of narratives, (2) how the readercan possibly display different tendencies towards the verbal and the visual, primarily through the model of double-scope stories within the theoretical framework of cognitive criticism. Referring substantially to experimental psychology, neuroscience and brain research, cognitive criticism provides a cross-disciplinary approach to the issues of reading, literacy and literature (Stockwell, 2002, pp. 1-11). Cognitive criticism in literary studies blossomed towards the end of the 1980s, though its exploration in the area of children’s literature is quite a recent phenomenon. To employ cognitive criticism in literary studies, scholars link strands of literary scholarship to human cognitive-affective skills, capacities, and mechanisms rather than directly borrow theories from cognitive science. In other words, the essence of cognitive criticism is not to embellish literary scholarship with scientific hard facts, but to seek a “rapprochement” between these areas traditionally deemed completely separate, and to see what new insights can be engendered from this rapprochement (Richardson, 1999, p. 158). Stockwell moreover demonstrates that cognitive criticism is fundamentally a new way of thinking about literature (2002, p. 6). Nikolajeva further elaborates that the particular explanatory potential of cognitive criticism lies in the issue of how a text is constructed in a way that can “maximise” or“optimise” readerly activity (2014, p. 4). Cognitive criticism therefore affords a way to connect potential readerly activity to the affordances of the picturebook.

    The notion of double-scope stories corresponds to the cognitive activity of conceptual blending. Human beings construct mental spaces at the same time when they think, talk, and act (Fauconnier & Turner, 2002, p. 40). These mental spaces are “conceptual packets” (p. 40) or “domains” (Stockwell, 2002, p. 107), which consist in long-term schematic knowledge (also referred to as “frames”) and long-term specific knowledge (Fauconnier & Turner, 2002, p. 40). Conceptual blending occurs when two mental spaces are combined “selectively and under constraints” to create another mental space, from which new structure and meaning arise (Turner, 2002, p. 10). Taken apart for the purpose of analysis, conceptual blending can be seen as happening in three stages: 1) mapping between elements of two mental spaces, in other words, finding counterpart connections in them, 2) projecting selectively different elements of the mental spaces to a blend, and 3) emergent structure arising from the blend (Fauconnier & Turner, 2002, pp. 40-44; Turner, 2003, p. 127). Conceptual blending, as a universal cognitive ability shared by adults and children, occurs at lightening speed often without conscious awareness. It is involved in every aspect of human activity from everyday bodily action to imaginative creation (Fauconnier & Turner, 2002, pp. 17-31). Turner further proposes the notion of “doublescope story” as a literary concept to denote that “input stories with different (and often clashing) organizing frames … are blended into a third story whose organizing frame includes parts of each of the input organizing frames” (2003, p. 128). New meaning and structure arise from within the blended story (p. 128). Turner asserts that children even as young as 2 years old should have no difficulty in understanding blended stories (p. 134), and further argues that blending is the “mainstay” of children’s literature (2002, p. 12).

    The model of double-scope stories is especially applicable to the picturebook medium,because the picturebook fundamentally embraces the mode of a double-scope story. The spectrum of the visual-verbal dynamics in the picturebook varies from symmetrical to counterpointing. New meaning and structure arise from the blend that is the result of assembling the two input stories conveyed in the verbal and the visual, which operate on not necessarily clashing yet indeed different organising frames—temporality and spatiality. I am not suggesting a binary division of the visual and the verbal into spatiality and temporality, because the visual can also convey the sense of temporality, for instance, through motion lines, and the verbal surely can construct the spatial aspect of the fictional world. Particularly important in this regard though, is that in picturebooks the visual has more potential for constructing spatiality whilst the verbal for temporality, as Nikolajeva and Scott (2006, pp. 139-172) and Sipe (1998, pp. 98-101) have demonstrated amongst others. The model of double-scope stories and the relevant concept of conceptual blending have particular explanatory potential in exploring how the reader can possibly relate and integrate the visual and the verbal in terms of the innate cognitive structures. I argue that the universality of the intellectual and imaginative activity of conceptual blending enables the reader to relate pictures to words, or vice versa, and to integrate them. Though to make meaning from the picturebook as a double-scope story requires the reader to integrate the two levels of narratives, the verbal and the visual can sometimes constitute two relatively independent input stories. Readers may be more oriented towards either one of them, partially due to the difference in their innate cognitive structures. Moreover, I suggest the blended story that results from assembling the two input stories may constitute the input story for another round of the formation of a new double-scope story, which therefore brings in an alternative perspective on the hermeneutic circle of picturebook reading.

    3. Exemplifying With a Close Analysis of Grandpa Green

    I will illustrate my arguments with a close analysis of Lane Smith’s picturebookGrandpa Green(henceforthGrandpa). When analysing the primary text, to more fully illustrate the input stories, I draw on two other concepts within the theoretical framework of cognitive criticism, that is, schema and brain laterality, which will be explained in detail when pertinent. The readers that I mention, for instance, the adult reader, the (young) child reader, are implied readers constructed and evoked by the text as opposed to real, flesh-and-blood readers.Grandpatells the story of a great-grandfather who goes through various experiences in his lifetime. For instance, he grows up on a farm, gets chickenpox in nursery school, reads stories about secret gardens and wizards, steals his first kiss in junior school, goes to a world war, meets his future wife, and gets married. He and his great-grandchild play and enjoy horticulture together in an exquisitely crafted garden, where memories are passed down in the various shapes of topiary groves and trees.

    In a first reading, until coming to the tenth spread, the reader may find it difficult to find the connections between the little boy represented in the visual and “he” that appears throughout the verbal narrative. They may interpret the little boy depicted in the visualas the person who goes through all the events narrated in the verbal, that is, the visual point of view corresponds with the verbal narrative voice—third-person omniscient. The ensuing problem however is that in the visual the little boy does not show the slightest trace of growing up or aging. He goes to nursery school, junior school, and war, and gets married as a little boy. The sentence in the lower right corner of the ninth spread“At least to hearhimtell it” (original emphasis) however challenges the interpretation that the little boy depicted in the visual corresponds to “he” in the verbal. Apparently, a narrator retelling what s/he has been told hides behind the verbal narrative. The identity of the verbal narrator is immediately revealed, when the reader turns to the tenth spread. On this spread, the verbal text says, “They had children, lots more grandchildren, a greatgrandchild,me” (spread 10; my emphasis), while the visual presents the little boy wave his hands as if to draw the reader’s attention, and show to the reader “Oh, this isme.” The tenth spread prompts the reader to map the visual image of the little boy onto the narrator of the story presented in the verbal, that is, the little boy is the great-grandson retelling the great-grandfather’s life stories. This interpretation is reinforced on the fourteenth spread, which depicts the little boy bring to an old man the floppy straw hat, while the verbal text on the previous spread says, “and he sometimes forgets things like his favourite floppy straw hat” (spread 13).

    InGrandpatherefore, the visual presents an input story, in which a little boy plays in a garden full of topiary images with an old man. The organising frame of this input story is spatiality and play. The verbal presents another input story, where the great-grandson narrates the life experiences he hears from his great-grandfather. Its organising frame is temporality, and the narrative act of retelling, which suggests human interaction. The tone in which the great-grandson retells his great-grandfather’s life stories is matter-of-factly, almost cold. For instance, “He grew up on a farm with pigs and corn and carrots” (spread 3), “In nursery school he got chickenpox” (spread 4), “After senior school his wish was to study horticulture” (spread 6), “but he went to a world war instead” (spread 7). It is the visual that renders the great-grandfather’s life experiences into vivid, emotionally charged and powerful images. Mapping across the two input stories, we can get: the little boy in the picture is the counterpart of the great-grandson in the verbal, the old man is the counterpart of the great-grandfather, and most of the topiary images correspond to the episodes from the great-grandfather’s life experiences. These elements (the little boy—the great-grandson, the old man—the great-grandfather, the topiary images—the greatgrandfather’s life experiences) are projected into the blended story, where new meaning emerges: the spatial concept garden acquires a temporal dimension, that is, the part of the great-grandfather’s life that is irreversibly lost; the spatiotemporal concept garden gains another layer of cognitive-affective meaning since it is also the great-grandfather’s mindscape, in other words, what he remembers and wants to share with his great-grandchild.

    Compared to the verbal input story, the visual one is more closely bound up with conceptual blending, as the topiary images are an immediate visualisation of the blending of conceptual domains. The first spread for instance represents a gigantic grove shapedinto a baby. The baby is green all over, and a curly sprout bristles, resembling the hair of a newborn. Tears well out, and on the next spread are revealed to be fountain water springing out of a hose. The fifth spread depicts groves shaped into the characters fromThe Wonderful Wizard of Ozthat the great-grandfather read when he was a child staying home from school, including the lion, the straw man, the tin man, and the dog Toto. On the seventh spread, the cruelty of the war that the great-grandfather went to is toned down through integrating the war images with the groves. For instance, the fireball shooting from the cannon is represented as a small ball of green leaves protruding from a tree stump, and the fires are represented as red flowers. The topiary images constantly integrate two conceptual domains: trees and people, trees and animals, trees and fictional characters, trees and war, and so on. The topiary images that rely on conceptual blending can create the effect of schema disruption. Schema in the context of literary analysis refers to readers’ existing knowledge and experience of a recurrent pattern that they bring to bear on a text (Stockwell, 2002, pp. 75-89), in other words, static repertoires (Herman, 2002, p. 89; Oziewicz, 2015, p. 58; Trites, 2014, p. 38). To take the first spread as an example, readers (both adult and child) have a stronger schema of babies as human beings than as trees. Schema disruption presents a challenge to readers’ existing repertoire of knowledge and experience, resulting in changes in their existing schema, which, in turn, causes them to view the familiar in a different light, that is, the effect of defamiliarisation (p. 79). The device of defamiliarisation effectively grasps readers’ attention from the very beginning of the text.

    The verbal input story organises around a linear progression of time, starting with the sentence “He was born a really long time ago,” which continues on to the next spread—“before computers or mobile phones or television” (spread 2). The second half of the sentence gives more information on the setting. Given that the television has the longest history among the three, the story is very likely to start around the late nineteenth or early twentieth century. Adult readers, who are equipped with historical background knowledge or have lived through such an age, are able to figure out what “before computers or mobiles or television” means. For child readers, even though “computers,” “mobiles,”“television” are concrete gadgets, complementing the more abstract “a really long time ago,” they may still have difficulty in understanding the temporal aspect of the fictional world. Using the three tokens as markers of time puts high demands on child readers who grow up with computers, mobile phones, and television. They are required to strain their imagination in order to picture what life may be like without these technologies. Child readers’ possible difficulty in dealing with the three tokens may moreover have to do with their perception of time, which depends on their brain development.

    Recent studies of the brain (e.g. McGilchrist, 2012; Panksepp & Biven, 2012) have acknowledged the function of the evolutionarily most recent layer of the brain, that is, the lateralised cerebral cortex, and its distinction from and connection to the more ancient“subcortical regions” in a relationship of oppositional pairing described as “up/down”(McGilchrist, 2012, p. 9). Located in the more ancient subcortical regions structurallybelow the cerebral cortex (Panksepp & Biven, 2012, p. 2) are seven primal emotional systems, which give basic “value structures” to “higher mental activities” (p. 12). The cerebral cortex is also named “the neocortical “thinking cap” ” (p. 1), to which we owe largely the way of perceiving, experiencing and thinking about the world (e.g. McGilchrist, 2012). The bihemispheric structure (that is, the division of the cerebral cortex into the right hemisphere and the left hemisphere) gives rise to two fundamentally different ways of seeing and experiencing the world (McGilchrist, 2012; Panksepp & Biven, 2012). Human beings’ interaction with the world is synthesised from the workings of the two hemispheres that cooperate but more often conflict with each other. Though both hemispheres are involved in every human activity, there is striking difference in how the two hemispheres attend to the world and the self in it (McGilchrist, 2012). The sense of time passing is associated with sustained attention (pp. 75-76), that is, the type of attention that closely relates to a coherent perception of the world (pp. 38-39). Since the right hemisphere is the province of sustained attention, the sense of the passage of time, especially time as“somethinglived through, with a past, present and future” arises in the right hemisphere (p. 76; my emphasis). The left hemisphere however tends to perceive time as “an infinite series of static moments” rather than as “a single, unified motion” (p. 76). The left hemisphere thereby has a particular advantage where there is an emphasis on “a point in time” (p. 77). The right hemisphere, compared to the left hemisphere, matures earlier, and gets more involved in early childhood mental activities (p. 88). Presumably, this suggests that children are very likely to have a stronger sense of time as a flow, something to be “l(fā)ived through” rather than as a series of separate, static points. Very young children “l(fā)ive in”time before their awareness of the passage of time comes into being (Gozlan, 2013, para. 4-5). Therefore child readers, whose left hemisphere is less operational than the right, may have difficulty in coping with the three tokens, that is, the three static points employed as the temporal markers of the fictional world constructed inGrandpa.

    To unravel the temporal and cognitive-affective implications of the garden represented primarily as a spatial landscape in the visual ofGrandpa, the reader is required to find counterpart relations between the verbal input story and the visual one, and to integrate them into a new blend story. Compared to the verbal input story, the visual one affords a more direct, immediate visualisation of the blending of conceptual domains through portraying various topiary images that represent people, animals, fictional characters, and so on. This effectively grasps the reader’s attention from the very start, and sustains the attraction of attention throughout. Since, as Turner argues, children as young as two years old can deal with conceptual blending, the topiary images should work for both the adult reader and the child reader. The verbal input story inGrandpaorganises around a linear progression of time. The linear organisation of the verbal may pose a particular challenge to the young child reader because of the brain development. This may impact the young child reader’s way of assembling the two input stories and deriving new meaning and structure from the blend story.

    4. Conclusion

    I have argued in this article that, on the one hand, the picturebook medium that integrates two levels of narratives works and plays with the fundamental human capacity for conceptual blending. It is the intellectual and imaginative activity of conceptual blending that enables the reader to relate the visual to the verbal, or vice versa, and to integrate them. On the other hand, readers may take different senses of pleasure from the two input stories. For instance, young child readers may indulge themselves in the visual input story ofGrandpathat organises around spatiality and play rather than wrack their brains to follow the linear progression of the verbal input story, which may result from the difference in the brain development of adults and children. This, however, does not make the picturebook less enjoyable for young child readers. The relative independent pleasures that readers may possibly take in the input stories can be heightened in the medium of picturebooks because the visual based on the mode of space may be a more efficient means of offering to readers an immediate experience of the fictional world. Deploying the model of double-scope stories has particular explanatory potential for the discussion of picturebooks because the model connects the unique characteristic of the picturebook medium to the inherent cognitive activity of conceptual blending, therefore adding a potential significant theoretical dimension to picturebook research.

    Note

    1 Not all picturebooks contain both the verbal and the visual. The term wordless picturebook, sometimes also referred to as “wordless narrative” (Beckett, 2012, p. 121) or “wordless picture-book” (Nieres-Chevrel, 2010, p. 137), tends to denote “a text where the visual image carries the weight of the meaning and where … the absence of words is ‘not simple feat of artistry [instead it is] totally relevant and in keeping with topic” (Arizpe, 2014, p. 94). But the focus of this article is on picturebooks that rely on both the verbal and the visual.

    Arizpe, E. (2014). Wordless picturebooks: Critical and educational perspectives on meaningmaking. In B. Kummerling-Meibauer (Ed.),Picturebooks: Representation and narration(pp. 91-106). Abingdon: Routledge.

    Arizpe, E., & Styles, M. (2016).Children reading pictures: Interpreting visual texts(2nd ed.). Abingdon: Routledge.

    Bader, B. (1976).American picturebooks from Noah’s Ark to The Beast Within. New York:Macmillan.

    Beckett, S. L. (2012).Crossover picturebooks: A genre for all ages. London: Routledge.

    Cech, J. (1983-1984). Remembering Caldecott: ‘The Three Jovial Huntsmen’ and the art of the picturebook.The Lion and the Unicorn,7-8, 110-119.

    Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2002).The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.

    Gozlan, M. (2013, January 1). A stopwatch on the brain’s perception of time.The Guardian. Retrieved from https://www.theguardian.com/science/2013/jan/01/psychology-timeperception-awareness-research

    Hallberg, K. (1982). Litteraturvetenskapen och bilderboksforskningen.Tidskrift for Litteraturvetenskap,3-4, 163-168.

    Herman, D. (2002).Story logic: Problems and possibilities of narrative. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Jakobsen, R. (1971).Selected writings II: Word and language. The Hague: Mouton.

    Lacy, L. E. (1986).Art and design in children’s picture books: An analysis of Caldecott awardwinning illustrations. Chicago: American Library Association.

    Lessing, G. E. (1984).Laocoon: An essay on the limits of painting and poetry(E. A. McCormick, Trans.). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. (Original work published 1766)

    Lewis, D. (1996). Going along with Mr. Gumpy: Polysytemy and play in the modern picture book.Signal,80, 105-119.

    McGilchrist, I. (2012).The master and his emissary: The divided brain and the making of the western world. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Nieres-Chevrel, I. (2010). The narrative power of pictures:L’Orage. In T. Colomer, B. Kummerling-Meibauer, & C. Silva-Diaz (Eds.),New directions in picturebook research(pp.129-138). New York: Routledge.

    Nikolajeva, M. (2014).Reading for learning: Cognitive approaches to children’s literature. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Nikolajeva, M., & Scott, C. (2006).How picturebooks work. London: Routledge.

    Nodelman, P. (1988).Words about pictures: The narrative art of children’s picture books. Athens:University of Georgia Press.

    Oziewicz, M. (2015).Justice in young adult speculative fiction: A cognitive reading. Abingdon:Routledge.

    Panksepp, J., & Biven, L. (2012).The archaeology of mind: Neuroevolutionary origins of human emotions. New York: Norton.

    Pullman, P. (1989). Invisible pictures.Signal,60, 160-186.

    Richardson, A. (1999). Cognitive science and the future of literary studies.Philosophy and Literature,23(1), 157-173.

    Schwarcz, J. H. (1982).Ways of the illustrator: Visual communication in children’s literature. Chicago: American Library Association.

    Sipe, L. R. (1998). How picture books work: A semiotically framed theory of text-picture relationships.Children’s Literature in Education,29(2), 97-108.

    Smith, L. (2011).Grandpa Green. New York: Roaring Brook.

    Spitz, E. H. (1999).Inside picture books. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Stockwell, P. (2002).Cognitive poetics: An introduction. London: Routledge.

    Trites, R. (2014).Literary conceptualizations of growth: Metaphors and cognition in adolescent literature. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Turner, M. (2002). The cognitive study of art, language and literature.Poetic Today,23, 9-20. doi:10.1215/03335372-23-1-9

    Turner, M. (2003). Double-scope stories. In D. Herman (Ed.),Narrative theory and the cognitive sciences(pp.117-142). Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information.

    About the author

    Xiaofei Shi (x_shi1@163.com) obtained from the University of Cambridge the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in 2016. Her PhD thesis is titled “Reconceptualising Crossover Picturebooks: A Cognitive Approach to Crossover Picturebooks and Readers’ Engagement with Them.” Her main research interests are: children’s literature, crossover picturebooks, visual literacy, and cognitive criticism. She now lectures and researches at the School of Foreign Languages, Soochow University, China.

    国内毛片毛片毛片毛片毛片| 欧美日韩瑟瑟在线播放| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类 | 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看 | 日本撒尿小便嘘嘘汇集6| 夜夜爽天天搞| 亚洲精品在线观看二区| 在线观看免费视频日本深夜| 日韩国内少妇激情av| 欧美高清成人免费视频www| 国产aⅴ精品一区二区三区波| 亚洲avbb在线观看| 美女 人体艺术 gogo| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 亚洲精品色激情综合| 国产精品久久久久久亚洲av鲁大| 成人高潮视频无遮挡免费网站| 国产精品乱码一区二三区的特点| 少妇的逼水好多| 精品一区二区三区av网在线观看| 老女人水多毛片| 淫妇啪啪啪对白视频| 中文字幕人妻熟人妻熟丝袜美| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 成人av在线播放网站| videossex国产| 亚洲av第一区精品v没综合| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品| 国产国拍精品亚洲av在线观看| 亚洲国产欧美人成| 久久久久久久久久黄片| 一进一出好大好爽视频| 色综合亚洲欧美另类图片| 国产精品免费一区二区三区在线| 国产精品一及| 一进一出好大好爽视频| 一区福利在线观看| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 精品不卡国产一区二区三区| 欧美区成人在线视频| 日韩精品中文字幕看吧| 免费在线观看日本一区| 国产在视频线在精品| 老司机福利观看| 女的被弄到高潮叫床怎么办 | 熟女电影av网| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 成年版毛片免费区| 少妇熟女aⅴ在线视频| 亚洲无线观看免费| av在线亚洲专区| 小蜜桃在线观看免费完整版高清| 人妻丰满熟妇av一区二区三区| 国产aⅴ精品一区二区三区波| 国产色爽女视频免费观看| 精品久久久久久久久久免费视频| 欧美成人免费av一区二区三区| 又爽又黄无遮挡网站| 国产成人av教育| 免费看av在线观看网站| 国产单亲对白刺激| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆 | 久久久久久大精品| 91久久精品国产一区二区三区| 国产精品,欧美在线| 国产黄a三级三级三级人| 亚洲自偷自拍三级| 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 深爱激情五月婷婷| 我要搜黄色片| 亚洲精华国产精华精| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 久99久视频精品免费| 夜夜夜夜夜久久久久| 中出人妻视频一区二区| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频| 别揉我奶头~嗯~啊~动态视频| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av在线| 伦理电影大哥的女人| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 又粗又爽又猛毛片免费看| 亚洲乱码一区二区免费版| 国产一区二区在线观看日韩| 久久热精品热| 色噜噜av男人的天堂激情| 99久久中文字幕三级久久日本| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 黄色视频,在线免费观看| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 日日干狠狠操夜夜爽| 99riav亚洲国产免费| 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 亚洲国产精品久久男人天堂| 中文字幕久久专区| 尾随美女入室| 精品一区二区三区人妻视频| 久久精品人妻少妇| 日韩欧美三级三区| 在线观看午夜福利视频| 高清在线国产一区| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看| 91麻豆精品激情在线观看国产| 亚洲四区av| 国产色婷婷99| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 最近中文字幕高清免费大全6 | 两个人的视频大全免费| 长腿黑丝高跟| 亚洲一区高清亚洲精品| 亚洲经典国产精华液单| 老熟妇仑乱视频hdxx| 日韩欧美三级三区| 嫁个100分男人电影在线观看| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频| 人妻丰满熟妇av一区二区三区| 哪里可以看免费的av片| 中文字幕熟女人妻在线| 在线看三级毛片| 看免费成人av毛片| 99热只有精品国产| 久久久久九九精品影院| av专区在线播放| 午夜福利高清视频| 九色成人免费人妻av| 国产人妻一区二区三区在| 亚洲午夜理论影院| 午夜a级毛片| 一边摸一边抽搐一进一小说| 日韩一区二区视频免费看| 久久久午夜欧美精品| 亚洲七黄色美女视频| 国产精品久久电影中文字幕| 国产精品一区二区性色av| 成年女人永久免费观看视频| 男人狂女人下面高潮的视频| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 日本精品一区二区三区蜜桃| 国产淫片久久久久久久久| 成人精品一区二区免费| 无遮挡黄片免费观看| 久久久久久久精品吃奶| 直男gayav资源| av.在线天堂| 国产黄a三级三级三级人| 三级毛片av免费| netflix在线观看网站| 亚洲国产精品sss在线观看| 少妇猛男粗大的猛烈进出视频 | 国产亚洲精品久久久com| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆 | 搡老岳熟女国产| 中国美女看黄片| 亚洲人成网站高清观看| 日本免费一区二区三区高清不卡| 免费人成视频x8x8入口观看| 国产色婷婷99| 日本 av在线| 夜夜爽天天搞| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看 | а√天堂www在线а√下载| 内地一区二区视频在线| 中文字幕av成人在线电影| 欧美成人性av电影在线观看| 精品一区二区三区av网在线观看| 欧美日本亚洲视频在线播放| 性插视频无遮挡在线免费观看| 国产高清三级在线| 最好的美女福利视频网| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添小说| 最好的美女福利视频网| 亚洲av免费高清在线观看| 在线播放无遮挡| 一区福利在线观看| 一本久久中文字幕| 国产精品乱码一区二三区的特点| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 成人二区视频| 亚洲色图av天堂| 国产成年人精品一区二区| 18禁黄网站禁片午夜丰满| av专区在线播放| 亚洲国产精品sss在线观看| 精品久久久久久久末码| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添小说| 在线观看av片永久免费下载| 欧美成人a在线观看| 成人美女网站在线观看视频| 一级a爱片免费观看的视频| 99久久精品国产国产毛片| 麻豆一二三区av精品| 国产不卡一卡二| 久久国内精品自在自线图片| 伦精品一区二区三区| 久久这里只有精品中国| 人妻少妇偷人精品九色| 又紧又爽又黄一区二区| 免费看美女性在线毛片视频| 午夜精品一区二区三区免费看| av天堂在线播放| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 亚洲人成网站高清观看| 免费人成在线观看视频色| 亚洲成人精品中文字幕电影| 午夜精品在线福利| 国产乱人伦免费视频| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品| 观看免费一级毛片| 国产av在哪里看| 俺也久久电影网| 欧美日本视频| 99在线人妻在线中文字幕| 热99re8久久精品国产| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看| 亚洲无线在线观看| 日韩欧美在线二视频| 两个人的视频大全免费| 91在线精品国自产拍蜜月| 免费在线观看日本一区| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| 国产欧美日韩一区二区精品| 一个人看的www免费观看视频| 十八禁网站免费在线| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 麻豆成人av在线观看| 99久久中文字幕三级久久日本| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产 | 亚洲最大成人手机在线| av在线亚洲专区| 97超视频在线观看视频| 色av中文字幕| 2021天堂中文幕一二区在线观| 亚洲精品影视一区二区三区av| 国产激情偷乱视频一区二区| 精品久久久久久久久亚洲 | 97人妻精品一区二区三区麻豆| videossex国产| 亚洲中文日韩欧美视频| 99在线人妻在线中文字幕| 精品久久久久久成人av| av天堂在线播放| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频| 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 亚洲不卡免费看| 日韩欧美国产一区二区入口| 麻豆成人午夜福利视频| 国产老妇女一区| 深爱激情五月婷婷| 小蜜桃在线观看免费完整版高清| 五月玫瑰六月丁香| 九九爱精品视频在线观看| 久久久午夜欧美精品| 性色avwww在线观看| 国产探花极品一区二区| 国产成人福利小说| 精品久久久久久久久久免费视频| 久久精品国产清高在天天线| 91精品国产九色| 欧美日韩黄片免| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 18+在线观看网站| 88av欧美| 成熟少妇高潮喷水视频| 亚洲成人精品中文字幕电影| 能在线免费观看的黄片| 变态另类成人亚洲欧美熟女| 欧美成人性av电影在线观看| 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 亚洲精品粉嫩美女一区| 日韩大尺度精品在线看网址| 香蕉av资源在线| 蜜桃亚洲精品一区二区三区| 国产精品福利在线免费观看| 精品人妻熟女av久视频| 精品不卡国产一区二区三区| 亚洲五月天丁香| 欧美人与善性xxx| 国产av不卡久久| 国产av一区在线观看免费| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| 亚洲综合色惰| 亚洲第一电影网av| 国产高清激情床上av| 亚洲欧美激情综合另类| 在线天堂最新版资源| 国产黄色小视频在线观看| 大又大粗又爽又黄少妇毛片口| 一区福利在线观看| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| av中文乱码字幕在线| 99精品久久久久人妻精品| 窝窝影院91人妻| 无遮挡黄片免费观看| 国产黄色小视频在线观看| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 1000部很黄的大片| 亚洲欧美日韩无卡精品| 亚洲av第一区精品v没综合| 色尼玛亚洲综合影院| 国产三级中文精品| 日日干狠狠操夜夜爽| 又爽又黄a免费视频| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 精品人妻偷拍中文字幕| 日韩强制内射视频| 亚洲国产精品久久男人天堂| 成人av一区二区三区在线看| 69av精品久久久久久| 看片在线看免费视频| 亚洲国产精品sss在线观看| 99久久精品国产国产毛片| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱 | 麻豆av噜噜一区二区三区| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 精品国产三级普通话版| 无人区码免费观看不卡| 精品福利观看| 天堂动漫精品| 麻豆精品久久久久久蜜桃| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 午夜a级毛片| 国产淫片久久久久久久久| 中文字幕av在线有码专区| 亚洲精品一区av在线观看| 最近最新免费中文字幕在线| 中文在线观看免费www的网站| 尾随美女入室| 美女免费视频网站| 岛国在线免费视频观看| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看| 99久久九九国产精品国产免费| 日本精品一区二区三区蜜桃| 国产中年淑女户外野战色| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 免费电影在线观看免费观看| 午夜激情福利司机影院| h日本视频在线播放| 变态另类丝袜制服| 中文字幕免费在线视频6| 国内精品美女久久久久久| 欧美日韩乱码在线| 精品久久久久久久久久久久久| 22中文网久久字幕| 一区二区三区四区激情视频 | 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 免费看日本二区| 无遮挡黄片免费观看| 97超级碰碰碰精品色视频在线观看| 国产爱豆传媒在线观看| 三级毛片av免费| 中文字幕av成人在线电影| 99精品久久久久人妻精品| 国产一区二区三区视频了| 色精品久久人妻99蜜桃| 免费无遮挡裸体视频| 69av精品久久久久久| 日韩,欧美,国产一区二区三区 | 国产精品不卡视频一区二区| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 91狼人影院| 深爱激情五月婷婷| 日韩国内少妇激情av| 制服丝袜大香蕉在线| 国产伦精品一区二区三区四那| 免费不卡的大黄色大毛片视频在线观看 | 日韩亚洲欧美综合| 精品不卡国产一区二区三区| 美女cb高潮喷水在线观看| 国产精品98久久久久久宅男小说| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 在线国产一区二区在线| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| 亚洲va日本ⅴa欧美va伊人久久| 日本a在线网址| 免费看光身美女| 日韩欧美免费精品| 欧美激情久久久久久爽电影| 岛国在线免费视频观看| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看| 久久午夜福利片| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 搡老妇女老女人老熟妇| 看免费成人av毛片| 亚洲精华国产精华精| 麻豆成人av在线观看| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 九九在线视频观看精品| 嫁个100分男人电影在线观看| 国产视频一区二区在线看| 久久久久九九精品影院| 1000部很黄的大片| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 亚洲国产精品sss在线观看| 免费看日本二区| 日韩中字成人| 天堂影院成人在线观看| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 看黄色毛片网站| 极品教师在线免费播放| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 成人无遮挡网站| 国产精品亚洲一级av第二区| 国产精华一区二区三区| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 别揉我奶头~嗯~啊~动态视频| 22中文网久久字幕| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 色在线成人网| 免费看av在线观看网站| 国产高清激情床上av| av黄色大香蕉| 久久精品影院6| 中文字幕高清在线视频| 天美传媒精品一区二区| 亚洲经典国产精华液单| 国内精品宾馆在线| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 一进一出抽搐gif免费好疼| 51国产日韩欧美| 欧美日本亚洲视频在线播放| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| 草草在线视频免费看| 久久国产精品人妻蜜桃| 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 亚洲午夜理论影院| 日韩av在线大香蕉| 亚洲成人免费电影在线观看| 1024手机看黄色片| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 国产视频内射| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 国内久久婷婷六月综合欲色啪| 香蕉av资源在线| av在线老鸭窝| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 亚洲国产日韩欧美精品在线观看| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 少妇高潮的动态图| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 国产女主播在线喷水免费视频网站 | 欧美成人a在线观看| 国产熟女欧美一区二区| 午夜精品在线福利| 精品人妻1区二区| 国产单亲对白刺激| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添小说| 国国产精品蜜臀av免费| 搡老熟女国产l中国老女人| 亚洲av熟女| av福利片在线观看| 国产真实乱freesex| 亚洲精品在线观看二区| 午夜福利成人在线免费观看| 日日撸夜夜添| 亚洲中文日韩欧美视频| 女人十人毛片免费观看3o分钟| 变态另类成人亚洲欧美熟女| 国产真实乱freesex| 日本与韩国留学比较| 极品教师在线视频| 久久久久久久久大av| 99精品在免费线老司机午夜| 深爱激情五月婷婷| 又紧又爽又黄一区二区| 日韩 亚洲 欧美在线| 精品久久久久久,| 高清在线国产一区| 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 成人永久免费在线观看视频| 午夜老司机福利剧场| 91久久精品电影网| 亚洲精品456在线播放app | 国产激情偷乱视频一区二区| 一夜夜www| 波野结衣二区三区在线| 亚洲欧美日韩高清专用| 午夜福利18| 黄色丝袜av网址大全| 床上黄色一级片| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 一区二区三区免费毛片| 亚洲成人久久性| 成年免费大片在线观看| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线| 99精品在免费线老司机午夜| 搡老妇女老女人老熟妇| 动漫黄色视频在线观看| 久久99热6这里只有精品| 国产伦精品一区二区三区视频9| 日本一本二区三区精品| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 国产老妇女一区| 国产成人aa在线观看| 婷婷色综合大香蕉| 国产色爽女视频免费观看| 中文字幕熟女人妻在线| 亚洲精品一卡2卡三卡4卡5卡| 午夜精品久久久久久毛片777| 亚洲自拍偷在线| 观看免费一级毛片| 乱人视频在线观看| 国产老妇女一区| 午夜激情欧美在线| 少妇丰满av| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 国产精品爽爽va在线观看网站| 国产白丝娇喘喷水9色精品| 久9热在线精品视频| 久久国产乱子免费精品| 精品一区二区三区av网在线观看| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清| 国产精品亚洲一级av第二区| 三级毛片av免费| 久久亚洲精品不卡| 国产 一区 欧美 日韩| a级毛片a级免费在线| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 麻豆成人午夜福利视频| 国产精品无大码| 国产在线精品亚洲第一网站| 欧美性猛交黑人性爽| 黄色一级大片看看| 国产真实乱freesex| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| 成人美女网站在线观看视频| 亚洲av第一区精品v没综合| 日本-黄色视频高清免费观看| 国产视频内射| 美女黄网站色视频| 搡老熟女国产l中国老女人| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 亚洲精品在线观看二区| 天堂影院成人在线观看| 黄色视频,在线免费观看| 波野结衣二区三区在线| 国产中年淑女户外野战色| 九九久久精品国产亚洲av麻豆| 中文字幕av在线有码专区| 嫩草影院入口| 亚洲精品在线观看二区| 中文在线观看免费www的网站| 听说在线观看完整版免费高清| 精品不卡国产一区二区三区| 国产精品一区www在线观看 | 久久久精品大字幕| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 免费观看在线日韩| 婷婷丁香在线五月| 精品久久久久久久久av| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 免费看日本二区| 亚洲av成人av| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 18禁黄网站禁片午夜丰满| 黄色女人牲交| 在线观看舔阴道视频| 禁无遮挡网站| 亚洲精品成人久久久久久| 黄色欧美视频在线观看| 久久6这里有精品| 日韩一本色道免费dvd| 国内久久婷婷六月综合欲色啪| 亚洲性夜色夜夜综合| 亚洲国产高清在线一区二区三| 免费看日本二区| 亚洲熟妇熟女久久| .国产精品久久| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 亚州av有码| 韩国av一区二区三区四区| 亚洲va在线va天堂va国产| 亚洲自拍偷在线| 国产探花在线观看一区二区| 男女视频在线观看网站免费| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av在线| 观看免费一级毛片| а√天堂www在线а√下载| 老熟妇仑乱视频hdxx| 高清在线国产一区| 免费大片18禁| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 日本三级黄在线观看| or卡值多少钱| 中文亚洲av片在线观看爽| 乱系列少妇在线播放| 色综合亚洲欧美另类图片| 久久久久久久久久成人| 午夜精品久久久久久毛片777| 久久99热这里只有精品18| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验 | 亚洲av一区综合| 一进一出抽搐gif免费好疼| 色尼玛亚洲综合影院| 免费观看人在逋| 国产一区二区三区视频了| 蜜桃久久精品国产亚洲av| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看 | 国产高清三级在线| 女人十人毛片免费观看3o分钟| 人妻夜夜爽99麻豆av| 亚洲第一电影网av| 成年女人永久免费观看视频| 日本熟妇午夜| 欧美xxxx性猛交bbbb| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av| 国产免费男女视频| 无人区码免费观看不卡|