• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Antipredator Behavioral Responses of Native and Exotic Tadpoles to Novel Predator

    2015-12-13 02:20:38FangZHANGJuanZHAOYujieZHANGKevinMESSENGERandYongWANG
    Asian Herpetological Research 2015年1期

    Fang ZHANG, Juan ZHAO, Yujie ZHANG, Kevin MESSENGERand Yong WANG*

    1College of Life Sciences, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, 241000, China

    2Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, Alabama Agricultural and Mechanical University, Normal, Alabama, 35762, USA

    Antipredator Behavioral Responses of Native and Exotic Tadpoles to Novel Predator

    Fang ZHANG1, Juan ZHAO1, Yujie ZHANG1, Kevin MESSENGER2and Yong WANG2*

    1College of Life Sciences, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, 241000, China

    2Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, Alabama Agricultural and Mechanical University, Normal, Alabama, 35762, USA

    Factors related to the invasion process, such as high abundance of invaders, residence time, and functional distinctiveness, are well documented, but less attention has been given to the effects of antipredator strategy of invasive species during colonization. In this study, we explored the antipredator strategy of an introduced species by comparing the predator avoidance behaviors of two native anuran species and one introduced (“exotic”) species in the presence of different predators. The two native anuran species used in the study were Black-spotted Pond Frog Rana nigromaculata and Terrestrial Frog Rana limnocharis. The introduced (invasive) species used was American bullfrog Lithobates catesbeianus. Chinese pond turtle Chinemys reevesii, Red-backed rat snake Elaphe rufodorsata, and Big-headed turtle Platysternon megacephalum were used as predator species. Chinese pond turtles and Red-backed rat snakes are native predators of Black-spotted Pond Frogs and Terrestrial Frogs, while Big-headed turtles are novel (“unfamiliar”) to the two frogs. All three predator species are novel (“unfamiliar”) to the American bullfrog. The results show that tadpoles of the two native species displayed behaviors of recognizing the two native predators, but did not display the capability of identifying the novel predator. Results from our study also suggest that American bullfrog tadpoles exhibited strong antipredator behavioral responses by displaying the capability of identifying “unfamiliar” predators without cohabitation history and prior exposure to them. Such antipredator behavioral responses could have resulted in more favorable outcomes for an invading species during the invasive introductory process.

    tadpoles, Bullfrog, antipredator responses, chemical cues, novel predator

    1. Introduction

    Biological invasions threaten biodiversity and lead to species loss and extinction (Ficetola et al., 2007). Factors influencing a successful invasion may include: high abundance of invaders, residence time, functional distinctiveness, and disturbance or other changes to the invaded environment (Parker et al., 1999; Sax et al., 2005; Strayer et al., 2006). Understanding the mechanisms of these invasions is currently one of the greatest ecological challenges (L?vei, 1997). In particular, understanding the mechanisms that facilitate the success of an invading species is of considerable importance to the management of the invaders; however, such understanding can be particularly diffi cult to attain when interactions between native and invading species involve more than a single developmental stage (Kiesecker and Blaustein, 1998). Furthermore, the mechanisms that enable invading species to thrive at the expense of native species are also often unclear (Lodge, 1993).

    Although differences in propagule pressure in the fi rst stage are important for a successful invasion (Li et al., 2006), we believe that the success of an invasion also depends on the interactions with their own predators and competitors. In other words, successful invasion may be infl uenced not only by the interactions with native prey, but also by the invader’s interactions with new predators, native or not, in the colonized area (Sih et al., 2010). According to the “enemy release hypothesis” (ERH), thelack or scarcity of an invading species’ natural enemy in the introduced environment could contribute to the invading species’ establishment and proliferation (Colautti et al., 2004).

    For an anuran, one of the primary causes of death for its tadpoles is predation (Alford and Richards, 1999). Many studies have shown that after extensive cohabitation with a predator population, tadpoles of a prey population may develop antipredator behaviors (defenses) such as the capability of recognizing and responding to those predators without any prior exposure (Kats et al., 1988; Kiesecker and Blaustein, 1997; Pearl et al., 2003; Marquis et al., 2004; Sih et al., 2010). These antipredator behaviors could help in the reduction of mortality of the prey tadpole population during the predation process (Marquis et al., 2004; Sih et al., 2010). Sih (1987) classifi ed these anitpredator defenses into two categories: pre-encounter (i.e. the prey tadpoles’ ability to recognize predation-related chemical cues released by predators) (Wilson and Lefcort, 1993) and postencounter (i.e. the release of certain toxic chemicals by the prey tadpoles to reduce their palatability to predators) (Formanowicz and Brodie, 1982). Regardless of which antipredator defense is used, the ability of the invading anuran’s tadpoles to recognize non-native predator is vital to a successful invasion. These effective antipredator behaviors could reduce direct predation, thereby resulting in more individuals to attend the establishment stage (Lodge, 1993).

    Although antipredator behaviors are important to an invading species during colonization, few studies have been conducted to evaluate the effects of these behaviors. Identifying and understanding antipredator strategies of a particular invading species may be crucial to the prevention of future biological invasions process by other alien species. We could focus our effort on interfering with or impairing the antipredator responses of the invading species. This effort may increase the likelihood of predation of the invading species tadpoles, thereby achieving effective control of the invasion.

    The American bullfrog, Lithobates catesbeianus (also known as Rana catesbeiana) is native to eastern North America, but has been introduced in over forty countries and four continents over the last century (Lever, 2003). This species is listed in the Global Invasive Species Database as one of the “100 of the World’s Worst Invasive Alien Species” (ISSG, 2008). Bullfrogs can affect native amphibian populations through predation and competition for food and habitat (Kats and Ferrer, 2003; Pearl et al., 2004). Adult bullfrogs act as generalist predators and often prey on other amphibians (Blaustein and Kiesecker, 2002; Kats and Ferrer, 2003). In addition, large bullfrog tadpoles often outcompete the anuran larvae of native species (Ficetola et al., 2007). Consequently, through the combined impacts made by tadpoles and adults, this naturalized bullfrog species has caused population decline or local extinction of native amphibians in some places (ISSG, 2002; Kats and Ferrer, 2003; Ficetola et al., 2007).

    American bullfrogs were initially introduced in China during the late 1950s and early 1960s for food trade and commercial farming. (Liu et al., 2010). Based on the information collected from local bullfrog farmers in Nanling County, Anhui Province and the local Committee of Agriculture, we were able to determine that bullfrogs have been established in Nanling County for approximately 25 years. Currently, in many parts of China, the invasions of bullfrogs not only resulted in the decrease in the distribution of native species, but have also resulted in the decline of local frogs populations, to the extent of some local extinctions. (Wu and Li, 2004). We expected the success of this particular invasive species may have been attributed to the capability of its tadpoles having more sensitive and effective antipredator responses to novel predators than other native prey species.

    In our study, we sought to compare predator avoidance behaviors of two native tadpole anurans and one introduced tadpole anuran when exposing them to chemical cues from different predator species, novel or native. We suspected bullfrog tadpoles would reduce their activity level more in response to chemical cues from novel predators than that of native tadpole anurans. We also suspected that bullfrog tadpoles would response more vigorously to chemical cues from novel predators who find them palatable than those predators who find them unpalatable. Since reducing actively levels will protect tadpoles from being captured by predators (Kats and Dill, 1998), having sensitive and effective antipredator responses to novel predators will mean more individuals to attend the establishment stage.

    The two native anuran species we used were Blackspotted Pond Frog R. nigromaculata and Terrestrial Frog R. limnocharis. The introduced (invasive) species used was the American bullfrog Lithobates catesbeianus. Chinese pond turtle Chinemys reevesii, Red-backed rat snake Elaphe rufodorsata, and Big-headed turtle Platysternon megacephalum were used as predator species. The populations of Black-spotted Pond Frog and Terrestrial Frog coexist extensively in the wild with the populations of both the Chinese pond turtle and the Red-backed rat snake species (Chen, 1991). In other words, Chinese pond turtles and Red-backed rat snakes are native predators of the two native anurans. The population of Big-headed turtle has never coexisted with the two native anurans in the wild, thus making the predator novel (“unfamiliar”) to the pond frogs and terrestrial frogs. Tadpoles of the invasive species came from a commercial bullfrog farm, where the bullfrog population has had no exposure to or interaction with Chinese pond turtles, Redbacked rat snakes or Big-headed turtles, thus making all three predator species novel (“unfamiliar”) to the bullfrogs.

    2. Materials and Methods

    2.1 Animal collection and maintenance During April 2010, we collected tadpoles of three anuran species by hand net. Tadpoles of the two native species, Black-Spotted Pond Frog and Terrestrial Frog, were collected in several small ephemeral ponds and paddy fields in Wuhu County, Anhui Province, China, where native or novel predators were absent. Chinese pond turtles and Red-backed rat snakes are common in these types of ephemeral ponds and paddy fi elds, but mountain stream turtles, Big-headed turtle, do not inhabit them. Tadpoles of the invasive species, American bullfrog, were collected from a commercial bullfrog farm in Nanling County, Anhui Province, China. These tadpoles were reared from eggs in a commercial bullfrog farm, where Chinese pond turtles, Red-backed rat snakes, and Big-headed turtles did not inhabit.

    Tadpoles of all three anurans were transported in separate plastic containers to a laboratory in Anhui Normal University and housed individually in vitreous aquariums (18 cm × 25 cm and 10 cm high) with water at an ambient temperature of about 25 °C, and under a natural photoperiod (13h:11h light:dark). The tadpoles were fed every two days with a commercial bullfrog tadpole food. No contact with the scent or visual stimuli was allowed before the tadpoles were tested. The tadpole’s developmental stage (Gosner’s stage: 28; see Gosner, 1960) was standardized within and among the different species.

    We captured freshwater turtles in Jing County that are common predators of the tadpoles (Chen, 1991) and used them as predator scent donors. The two scent donor species, adult female Chinese pond turtles and adult female Big-headed turtles, were captured in Jing County in June 2008 and raised in the lab. The turtles were fed two to four appropriately-sized spiny eel (Mastacembelus aculeatus) twice a week during their non-hibernation period. We captured the adult female Red-backed rat snakes, common predators of the tadpoles (Chen, 1991), at the Tiantangzhai National Nature Reserve in Jinzhai County in July 2010. We used them as predator scent donors and fed them two appropriately-sized spiny eel once a week during our trial period. We obtained non-predatory goldfish (Carassius auratus) from a commercial dealer to be used as the source of neutral scent. Predators, turtles and snakes, were not fed 18 to 24 h preceding an experiment and were never fed any amphibians.

    2.2 Chemical stimuli We placed turtles and snakes in separate aquariums. Each aquarium was filled with 5 L of purified water and left overnight to collect the individual’s scent. We extracted and packaged the scented water separately in 10 mL portions and tightly sealed to prevent cross-contamination and frozen until use. The brand of purified water used in our experimental trials was Yeling purified water. The neutral stimulus was prepared by placing goldfish, in groups of two, into 3-litre aquariums with purified water for 3 days. Fish were not fed during this short period to avoid any contamination by food odor. Then, 10 mL of water was drawn from each aquarium, packaged, and frozen until use.

    2.3 Experimental procedure In order to determine whether anuran tadpoles assess predation risk from native and novel predators differently, we designed a “blind”experiment (Rohr and Madison, 2001; Gonzalo et al., 2007; Polo-Cavia et al., 2010) to analyze swimming activity levels of tadpoles in water scented with chemical cues from the different predators. We observed 15 individual tadpoles of each anuran species in five different treatments (‘control water’ versus ‘non-predatory fish scent’ versus ‘Chinese pond turtle’ versus ‘Bigheaded turtle’ versus ‘Red-backed rat snake’) in a random sequence. The tadpoles rested for one day between trials to avoid any infl uence from the previous trial.

    The ‘control water’ treatment was used as a control group; this is where tadpoles were exposed to a predatorfree environment. The non-predatory ‘goldfish scent’treatment was used to test any possible modifi cations in activity level of the tadpoles due to the presence of any“strange” or unfamiliar scent in the water.

    Tadpoles were tested individually in U-shaped gutters (101 cm × 11.4 cm × 6.4 cm) sealed at both ends (see methods in Polo-Cavia et al., 2010). The internal part of each gutter was quartered with four crossing lines tocreate fi ve subdivisions of equal surface. We fi lled each gutter with 3L of purified water at outside environment temperature (25 °C). We placed a single tadpole covered with a release cage (made of glass: 21 cm × 7.6 cm and 6.4 cm high) in the middle of the central subdivision of each gutter, and waited 5 minutes for acclimation. Then, we put our test solution (scented iced aliquots) at one end of the gutter (either left or right) by stratified randomization, and waited for an additional 5 minutes for the ice to melt. We began our trials by slowly lifting the release cage. This is called a “blind” experiment because the observer was not aware of which test solution was placed in the gutter. Observations were made through a small opening (approximately 15 cm × 8 cm) in an opaque white plastic curtain to minimize disturbance to the test tadpoles. The instantaneous scan sampling method was used to monitor each tadpole continuously for 30 minutes. We counted the number of times each tadpole went through the crossing lines during the entire 30-minute observation period (Rohr and Madison, 2001; Gonzalo et al., 2007; Polo-Cavia et al., 2010) and recorded the results in 1-minute scan interval. Each 1-minute scan interval represents the number of times a tadpole went through the crossing lines within that one minute (30 scan intervals per tadpole). Diffusion of chemicals in still water may be a slow process, however, we are confi dent that within the entire 30-minute observation period, all test tadpoles were exposed to our test solutions (Polo-Cavia et al., 2010).

    To assess palatability, each palatability experiment contained one predator and 10 prey tadpoles all at the same stage of development (Gosner 28 stages as those tested) in one separate polyethylene tank (35 cm × 23 cm × 22 cm) that was not used in our earlier experiments (detailed procedures can be seen in Wu et al., 2008). We used three individuals per predator species group to minimize the possibility of individual taste preferences and all predators were starved for approximately 48 h prior to the experiment. The experiment was repeated three times for all three anuran species with each predator. Each predator was used only once in a single replicate. After 24 h, we recorded the numbers of tadpoles remaining to determine the rate of consumption. Tadpoles that were dead (bitten by predators) but uneaten were replaced so that each predator had the opportunity to consume an equal number of live tadpoles.

    2.4 Statistical analyses Swimming activity levels for each tadpole (represented by the number of lines crossed) were log transformed to ensure normality (Shapiro-Wilk’s test) and the difference in activity level of the same individual tadpole across the five treatments with water containing different chemical cues (within-subject factor: ‘control water’ versus ‘non-predatory fish scent’versus ‘Chinese pond turtle’ versus ‘Red-backed rat snake’ versus ‘Big-headed turtle’) was tested using oneway repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs). Additionally, homogeneity of variance (Levene’s test) was also tested, and the results showed that the variances were not signifi cantly heterogeneous. Pairwise multiple comparisons were made using Tukey’s honestly signifi cant difference tests (Polo-Cavia et al., 2010).

    3. Results

    When the activity levels of the three species of anuran tadpoles were tested, we found signifi cant differences in the activity levels demonstrated by the tadpoles between each predator cue treatments (Figure 1). Bullfrog tadpoles showed signifi cantly lower activity levels in water treated with chemical cues from Chinese pond turtles and Bigheaded turtles than in control water (F4,70= 11.45; P <0.0001). They did not show signifi cant differences in the overall activity levels between water treated with nonpredatory fish scent and Red-backed rat snake scent in comparison to their activity in control water (Table 1).

    Tadpoles of Black-spotted Pond Frog and Terrestrial Frog reduced their activity in water with chemical cues from Chinese pond turtles and Red-backed rat snakes. They did not significantly reduced their activity in the water treated with chemical cues from Big-headed turtles (Black-spotted Pond Frog: F4,70= 8.02; P = 0.474; Terrestrial Frog: F4,70= 10.02; P = 0.191) and nonpredatory fi sh and control water (Table 1).

    In the palatability trials, Chinese pond turtles consumed 93% of American bullfrog tadpoles, 97% of Black-spotted Pond Frog tadpoles, and 90% of Terrestrial Frog tadpoles. The Big-headed turtles consumed 100% of American bullfrog tadpoles, 100% of Black-spotted Pond Frog tadpoles, and 87% of Terrestrial Frog tadpoles. The Red-backed rat snakes consumed 100% of Black-spotted Pond Frog and Terrestrial Frog tadpoles, but none of the American bullfrog tadpoles offered to them.

    4. Discussion

    In our study, we observed that all three of the tadpole species, native or exotic, did not exhibit any antipredator behavioral responses in water treated with a neutral chemical cue (non-predatory fish). Such observation confirmed our belief that antipredator responses in amphibian tadpoles are mediated by water-borne chemicalcue, and modification in activity levels occurs when tadpoles perceived predatory threats.

    Some studies have demonstrated that anurans tadpoles from populations extensively cohabitating with predatory species are able to recognize and respond to these predators without any prior exposure or contact (Kats et al., 1988; Kiesecker and Blaustein, 1997). The results showed that two native tadpoles (Black-spotted Pond Frog and Terrestrial Frog) significantly reduced their swimming activities when scent stimuli from the native predators (Chinese pond turtles and Red-backed rat snakes) were present. Since tadpoles of the two native species were collected in ponds and paddy fields where native or novel predators were absent, such reduction in activity levels indicated that two native tadpole species may be capable of “innately” detecting the presence of native predators.

    Moreover, in some cases, although the potential prey do not have an innate ability to recognize the predators, they are capable of learning through experience what are and are not considered as predators (e.g. fi sh: Mathis and Smith, 1993; Hazlett, 2003). Experience could include the exposure to an alarm chemical cue released by an injured conspecific. Several studies have demonstratedthat this type of acquired predator recognition in both fi sh and tadpoles by pairing alarm cues with the visual or chemical cues of a predator (Fish: e.g. Chivers and Smith, 1994, 1995; Larson and McCormick, 2005; Tadpoles: Mandrillon and Saglio, 2005). Since the populations of Black-spotted Pond Frog and Terrestrial Frog species share the same distribution areas in the wild as the populations of Chinese pond turtle and Red-backed rat snake species, they may have developed antipredator behavioral responses to these two predator species through learning.

    Table 1 Results (P values ) of the Tukey’s tests examining the responses of Black-spotted Pond Frog, Terrestrial Frog, and American Bullfrog tadpoles to the fi ve different treatments

    Big-headed turtles inhabit cold (12 °C-23 °C) fastfl owing mountain streams with boulders and gravel in the substrate, and rarely have the chance to encounter either Black-spotted Pond frogs or Terrestrial frogs, which inhabit flat area. Accordingly, Big-headed turtles would be considered an unfamiliar species to Black-spotted Pond frogs and Terrestrial frogs (Chen, 1991). The results showed that activity levels of the native tadpole species (Black-spotted Pond Frog and Terrestrial Frog) did not decline when exposed to chemical cues from the novel predator species (Big-headed turtles). Such results suggested that two native tadpole species may not be capable of “innately” recognizing predator species, whom they have not shared a coevolutionary history with in the wild. Moreover, results of some studies have suggested that some amphibians lack the ability to distinguish between ‘‘dangerous’’ and ‘‘less threatening’’ predators (Hazlett et al., 2000). The lack of such ability could possibly be due to historically low levels of predation, or because the predators of these amphibians possess the ability to break down or mask cues allowing for such discrimination (Wirsing et al., 2005).

    In the current study, tadpoles of American bullfrog significantly reduced their swimming activity when exposed to the chemical cues of the Chinese pond turtle and the Big-headed turtle. However, when exposed to the chemical cue of Red-backed rat snakes, the tadpoles of the American bullfrog did not significantly reduce their swimming activity. The bullfrog tadpoles were collected from eggs reared in a commercial bullfrog farm, where none of the predator species inhabit. If all three predator species are novel (“unfamiliar”) to American bullfrogs, why did the variation in antipredator behaviors exist when exposed to the two turtle species and the snake species?

    The palatability experiment showed that while Chinese pond turtles and Big-headed turtles consumed most of the American bullfrog tadpoles (Chinese pond turtles 93%, and Big-headed turtles 100%), Red-backed rat snakes consumed none of the bullfrog tadpoles offered to them. Studies have shown that the palatability of a tadpole to a specifi c predator would affect the tadpole’s behavioral responses. For example, Kiesecker et al. (1996) found that tadpoles of Bufo boreas modified their behavior in response to three predators that fi nd them palatable (two insects and one snake), but did not respond to cues of two predators that find them unpalatable (one newt and one fi sh). Furthermore, other studies have shown that tadpoles of American bullfrog from their native distribution reduced activity more in response to dragonfly larvae than in response to native mud minnows, which find American bullfrog less palatable than dragonfl y (Relyea, 2001). The unpalatability of species may account for their lack of response to chemical cues of a sympatric predator (Laurila et al., 1997).

    In the current study, bullfrog tadpoles significantly reduced their swimming activity when exposed to the chemical cues of the Chinese pond turtle and the Bigheaded turtle, who fi nd them palatable. However, bullfrog tadpoles did not significantly reduced their swimming activity when exposed to chemical cues of the Redbacked rat snakes, who fi nd them unpalatable. We suspect the unpalatability of the American bullfrog tadpoles to the predator may have accounted for the lack of antipredator behavioral responses exhibited by the tadpoles when exposed to chemical cues of Red-backed rat snakes. The combination of the reduction in activity level and the results of our palatability experiment suggested thatAmerican bullfrog tadpoles may be capable of “innately”identifying novel (“unfamiliar”) predators without prior co-occurrence and without prior experience (Pearl et al., 2003). The variations in activity levels to different novel predators also indicated that bullfrog tadpoles might be capable of identifying more dangerous novel predators who might find them palatable, thereby modifying their behaviors to respond to the threat of predation.

    Hazlett (2000) considered that such behavioral modification exhibited by the bullfrog tadpoles in response to more dangerous novel predators could have contributed to their success as an invasive species. The ability of the invasive species tadpoles to discriminate the chemical cues from novel predator might represent a competitive advantage. During colonization, an invading species can sometimes become a prey species in the colonized area due to its lack of a coevolutionary history with native predators. If the antipredator behaviors of the invading species are based on the “familiar” predator theory, then, the invaders will exhibit nonexistent or ineffective antipredator responses, which may lead to an unsuccessful introduction. Invasive species tadpoles may be able to respond to novel predators’ cues because cues from the novel predators may be similar to cues from the tadpoles’ native predators. Accordingly, we suspect there may be similar cues exhibited by the bullfrog’s native predators and the behaviors of our novel predators, the Chinese Pond turtle and Big-headed turtle.

    Our results suggested that the three tested anuran tadpoles were able to differentiate between predator cues and may reduce their activity level as a function of both coexistence history and palatability. Faced with the risk of predation, the prey may display various defense mechanisms to avoid or reduce such risk, thereby maximizing their survival rate (Sih, 1980). In the current study, we demonstrated that American bullfrog tadpoles were capable of distinguishing scents from novel predators by exhibiting antipredator behavioral responses. Accordingly, tadpoles of American bullfrog in the wild may also be able to discriminate novel predators without cohabitation history and prior exposure to them. Strong antipredator behavioral responses may result in a favorable outcome in the establishment and prevalence of this invasive species during the colonization process.

    Acknowledgements We would like to thank P. CHEN and S. Y. ZHAO for their assistance with the laboratory work. Financial support is provided by the Doctor’s Start-up Foundation of Anhui Normal University and the National Natural Science Foundation of China. We are grateful to K. CHEN for her revision of the English language of this manuscript. We would also like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

    References

    Alford R. A., Richards S. J. 1999. Global amphibian declines: A problem in applied ecology. Annu Rev Ecol Syst, 30: 133-165

    Blaustein A. R., Kiesecker J. M. 2002. Complexity in conservation: Lessons from the global decline of amphibian populations. Ecol Lett, 5: 597-608

    Chen B. H. 1991. Anhui Amphibia and Reptilia. Hefei: Anhui Press of Sciences and Technology (In Chinese)

    Chivers D. P., Smith R. J. F. 1994. The role of experience and chemical alarm signaling in predator recognition by fathead minnows, Pimephales promelas. J Fish Biol, 44: 273-285

    Chivers D. P., Smith R. J. F. 1995. Free-living minnows rapidly learn to recognize pike as predators. J Fish Biol, 46: 949-954

    Colautti R. I., Ricciardi A., Grigorovich I. A., Maclsaac H. J. 2004. Is invasion success explained by the enemy release hypothesis? Ecol Lett, 7: 721-733

    Ficetola G. F., Thuiller W., Miaud G. 2007. Prediction and validation of the potential global distribution of a problematic alien invasive species- the American bullfrog. Divers Distrib, 13: 476-485

    Formanowicz D. R., Brodie Jr. E. D. 1982. Relative palatabilities of members of larval amphibian community. Copiea, 1982: 91-97

    Gonzalo A., López P., Martín J. 2007. Iberian green frog tadpoles may learn to recognize novel predators from chemical alarm cues of conspecifi cs. Anim Behav, 74: 447-453

    Gosner K. L.1960. A simplifi ed table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on identifi cation. Herpetologica, 16: 183-190

    Hazlett B. A. 2000. Information use by an invading species: Do invaders respond more to alarm odors than native species? Biol Invasions, 2: 289-294

    Hazlett B. A. 2003. Predator recognition and learned irrelevance in the crayfi sh Orconectes virilis. Ethology, 109: 765-780

    ISSG Invasive Species Specialist Group. 2002. Global Invasive Species Database: Rana catesbeiana. Retrieved from www.issg. org/database/species/ecology.asp? si=80&fr=1&sts=tss

    ISSG Invasive Species Specialist Group. 2008. Retrieved from http://www.issg.org/database/welcome/.

    Kats L. B., Petranka J. W., Sih A. 1988. Antipredator defenses and the persistence of amphibian larvae with fi shes. Ecology, 69: 1865-1870

    Kats L. B., Dill L. M. 1998. The scent of death: chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey animals. Ecoscience, 5: 361-394

    Kats L. B., Ferrer R. P. 2003. Alien predators and amphibian declines: Review of two decades of science and the transition to conservation. Divers Distrib, 9: 99-110

    Kiesecker J. M., Chivers D. P., Blaustein A. R. 1996. The use of chemical cues in predator recognition by western toad tadpoles. Anim Behav, 52: 1237-1245

    Kiesecker J. M., Blaustein A. R. 1997. Population differences inresponses of Red-Legged Frogs (Rana aurora) to introduced bullfrogs. Ecology, 78: 1752-1760

    Kiesecker J. M., Blaustein A. R. 1998. Effects of introduced bullfrogs and smallmouth bass on microhabitat use, Growth, and survival of native red-legged frogs (Rana aurora). Conserv Biol, 12: 776-787

    Larson J. K., McCormick M. I. 2005. The role of chemical alarm signals in facilitating learned recognition of novel chemical cues in a coral reef fi sh. Anim Behav, 69: 51-57

    Laurila A., Kujasalo J., Ranta E. 1997. Different anti-predator behavior in two anuran tadpoles: Effects of predator diet. Behav Ecol Sociobiol, 40: 329-336

    Lever C. 2003. Naturalized amphibians and reptiles of the world. New York: Oxford University Press

    Li Y. M., Wu Z. J., Duncan R. P. 2006. Why islands are easier to invade: human infl uences on bullfrog invasion in the Zhoushan archipelago and neighboring mainland China. Oecologia, 148: 129-136

    Liu X., Li Y. M., Mcgarrity M. 2010. Geographical variation in body size and sexual size dimorphism of introduced American bullfrogs in southwestern China. Biol Invasions, 12, 2037-2047

    Lodge D. M. 1993. Biological invasion: Lessons for ecology. Trends Ecol Evol, 8:133-137

    L?vei G. L. 1997. Global change through invasion. Nature, 388: 627-628

    Mandrillon A. L., Saglio P. 2005. Prior exposure to conspecific chemical cues affects predatoro recognition in larval Common Toad (Bufo bufo). Archiv für Hydrobiologie, 164:1-12

    Marquis O., Saglio P., Neveu A. 2004. Effects of predators and conspecific chemical cues on the swimming activity of Rana temporaria and Bufo bufo tadpoles. Arch Hydrobiol, 160: 153-170

    Mathis A., Smith R. J. F. 1993. Fathead minnows, Pimephales promelas, learn to recognize northern pike, Esox lucius, as predators on the basis of chemical stimuli from minnows in the pike’s diet. Anim Behav, 46: 645-656

    Parker I. M., Simberloff D., Lonsdale W. M., Goodell K., Wonham M., Kareiva P. M., Williamson M. H., Von Holle B., Moyle P. B., Byers J. E. 1999. Impact: Toward a framework for understanding the ecological effects of invaders. Biol Inv, 1: 3-19

    Pearl C. A., Adams M. J. Schuytema G. S., Nebeker A. 2003. Behavioral responses of anuran larvae to chemical cues of native and introduced predators in the Pacific Northwestern United States. J Herpetol, 37: 572-576

    Pearl C. A., Adams M. J., Bury R. B., McCreary B. 2004. Asymmetrical effects of introduced bullfrogs (Rana catesbeiana) on native Ranid frogs in Oregon. Copeia, 2004:11-20

    Polo-Cavia N., Gonzalo A., López P., Martin, J. 2010. Predator recognition of native but not invasive turtle predators by na?ve anuran tadpoles. Anim Behav, 80: 461-466

    Relyea R. A. 2001. The relationship between predation risk and antipredator responses in larval anurans. Ecology, 82: 541-554

    Rohr J. R., Madison D. M. 2001. A chemically mediated trade-off between predation risk and mate search in newts. Anim Behav, 62: 863-869

    Sax D. F., Stachowicz J. J., Gaines S. D. 2005. Species invasions: insights into ecology, evolution, and biogeography. Sunderland MA: Sinauer Associates

    Sih A. 1980. Optimal behavior: Can foragers balance two confl icting demands? Science, 210: 1041-1043

    Sih A. 1987. Predators and prey lifestyles: An evolutionary and ecological overview. In Kerfoot W. C., Sih A. (Eds.), Predation: Direct and Indirect Impacts on Aquatic Communities. Hanover: University Press of New England, 203-224

    Sih A., Bolnick D. I., Barney B. 2010. Predator-prey na?veté, antipredator behavior, and the ecology of predator invasions. Oikos, 119: 610-621

    Strayer D. L., Eviner V. T., Jeschke J. M., Pace M. L. 2006. Understanding the long-term effects of species invasions. Trends Ecol Evol, 21: 645-651

    Wilson D. J., Lefcort H. 1993. The effect of predator diet on the alarm response of red-legged frog, Rana aurora, tadpoles, Anim Behav, 46: 1017-1019

    Wirsing A. J., James D. R., Dennis L. M. 2005. Can prey use dietary cues to distinguish predators? A test involving three terrestrial amphibians. Herpetologica, 61: 104-110

    Wu Z. J., Li Y. M. 2004. Causes and conservation strategies of amphibian population declines. Chin J Ecol, 23: 140-146 (In Chinese)

    Wu Z. J., Cai F. J., Jia Y. F., Lu J. X., Jiang Y. F., Huang C. M. 2008. Predation impact of Procamerus clarkia on limnocharis tadpoles in Guilin area. Biodivers Sci, 16: 150-155 (In Chinese)

    Prof. Yong Wang, from Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, Alabama Agricultural and Mechanical University, Alabama, USA, with his research focusing on birds and reptiles ecology.

    E-mail: yong.wang@aamu.edu

    2 April 2014 Accepted: 10 March 2015

    色吧在线观看| 欧美另类亚洲清纯唯美| 高清日韩中文字幕在线| 成人特级黄色片久久久久久久| 黄色配什么色好看| 国内久久婷婷六月综合欲色啪| 最近在线观看免费完整版| 日韩av在线大香蕉| 美女大奶头视频| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 小蜜桃在线观看免费完整版高清| 国产亚洲欧美98| 一边摸一边抽搐一进一小说| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 日本五十路高清| 看片在线看免费视频| 一卡2卡三卡四卡精品乱码亚洲| 久久九九热精品免费| 免费观看人在逋| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 嫩草影视91久久| 国产视频一区二区在线看| av在线亚洲专区| 又爽又黄a免费视频| 日本在线视频免费播放| 成年免费大片在线观看| 日本一二三区视频观看| 一个人免费在线观看电影| 亚洲精品粉嫩美女一区| www日本黄色视频网| 欧美+亚洲+日韩+国产| 国产精品98久久久久久宅男小说| 国产精品电影一区二区三区| 国内久久婷婷六月综合欲色啪| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 中文字幕精品亚洲无线码一区| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 久久久久久久久久黄片| 日韩欧美精品v在线| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 婷婷亚洲欧美| 国产亚洲精品av在线| 国产精品电影一区二区三区| 99热这里只有是精品50| 直男gayav资源| 国产v大片淫在线免费观看| 色视频www国产| 无人区码免费观看不卡| 久久久国产成人免费| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看 | 99国产极品粉嫩在线观看| 国产精品亚洲一级av第二区| 国产白丝娇喘喷水9色精品| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区| 免费av观看视频| 看黄色毛片网站| 久久人妻av系列| 久久久久久久久久久丰满 | 最新在线观看一区二区三区| 亚洲一级一片aⅴ在线观看| 色哟哟哟哟哟哟| 尾随美女入室| 欧美激情国产日韩精品一区| 国产精品乱码一区二三区的特点| 欧美最黄视频在线播放免费| 啦啦啦观看免费观看视频高清| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产 | 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 真实男女啪啪啪动态图| 色av中文字幕| 亚洲男人的天堂狠狠| 在线看三级毛片| bbb黄色大片| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 亚洲黑人精品在线| 成年女人永久免费观看视频| 久久婷婷人人爽人人干人人爱| 精品久久久久久,| 日本黄色视频三级网站网址| 黄色日韩在线| 欧美人与善性xxx| 国产在线精品亚洲第一网站| 欧美极品一区二区三区四区| 老司机午夜福利在线观看视频| x7x7x7水蜜桃| 国产中年淑女户外野战色| 成人国产一区最新在线观看| 精品一区二区三区av网在线观看| 观看免费一级毛片| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 在线播放国产精品三级| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 欧美xxxx性猛交bbbb| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 亚洲成人久久爱视频| 人妻少妇偷人精品九色| 99精品久久久久人妻精品| 毛片女人毛片| 一本久久中文字幕| 国产淫片久久久久久久久| 国产精华一区二区三区| 美女cb高潮喷水在线观看| 精品无人区乱码1区二区| 国产欧美日韩一区二区精品| 在线观看午夜福利视频| 免费人成视频x8x8入口观看| 一个人观看的视频www高清免费观看| 狠狠狠狠99中文字幕| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 制服丝袜大香蕉在线| 日日干狠狠操夜夜爽| 99国产精品一区二区蜜桃av| 高清毛片免费观看视频网站| 夜夜爽天天搞| 给我免费播放毛片高清在线观看| 久久亚洲真实| 嫩草影院入口| 国产亚洲精品久久久com| 午夜精品在线福利| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 一进一出抽搐动态| 美女 人体艺术 gogo| 人妻少妇偷人精品九色| 久久久久性生活片| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产 | 成年女人看的毛片在线观看| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 国产精品一区二区性色av| 神马国产精品三级电影在线观看| 亚洲美女黄片视频| 91av网一区二区| 国产精品一及| 国产国拍精品亚洲av在线观看| 色综合站精品国产| 亚洲av第一区精品v没综合| 欧美色视频一区免费| 久久久久精品国产欧美久久久| 婷婷亚洲欧美| 国产伦精品一区二区三区视频9| 亚洲狠狠婷婷综合久久图片| 成人av在线播放网站| 淫妇啪啪啪对白视频| 亚洲一区二区三区色噜噜| 亚洲av美国av| videossex国产| 别揉我奶头 嗯啊视频| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av| 日韩欧美精品v在线| 国产精品女同一区二区软件 | 日韩精品青青久久久久久| 国产成年人精品一区二区| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 久久精品国产亚洲av天美| 精品福利观看| 国产精品,欧美在线| 在线观看午夜福利视频| 日本三级黄在线观看| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 身体一侧抽搐| 99热网站在线观看| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 在现免费观看毛片| 蜜桃久久精品国产亚洲av| 日韩在线高清观看一区二区三区 | 久久人人爽人人爽人人片va| 日韩高清综合在线| 亚洲欧美精品综合久久99| 在线国产一区二区在线| 身体一侧抽搐| 亚洲18禁久久av| 亚洲最大成人手机在线| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 免费无遮挡裸体视频| 深夜a级毛片| 淫秽高清视频在线观看| 欧美色视频一区免费| av黄色大香蕉| 日本熟妇午夜| 日本免费一区二区三区高清不卡| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 成年女人永久免费观看视频| 高清毛片免费观看视频网站| 亚洲图色成人| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 成年版毛片免费区| 日本与韩国留学比较| 亚洲av美国av| 日日干狠狠操夜夜爽| 在线看三级毛片| 国产精品电影一区二区三区| 亚洲中文字幕日韩| 91午夜精品亚洲一区二区三区 | 最新在线观看一区二区三区| 99热精品在线国产| 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 亚洲av一区综合| 国产黄色小视频在线观看| 精品无人区乱码1区二区| 免费观看精品视频网站| 欧美三级亚洲精品| 亚洲av.av天堂| 亚洲精华国产精华精| 国产视频内射| 亚洲国产日韩欧美精品在线观看| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 长腿黑丝高跟| 免费观看的影片在线观看| 日韩欧美在线二视频| 成人高潮视频无遮挡免费网站| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 长腿黑丝高跟| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 黄色丝袜av网址大全| 少妇高潮的动态图| 91av网一区二区| 我要看日韩黄色一级片| 久9热在线精品视频| av在线老鸭窝| 国产亚洲精品久久久久久毛片| 黄片wwwwww| 日本免费a在线| 又爽又黄无遮挡网站| 日韩欧美在线乱码| 国产高清视频在线播放一区| 五月玫瑰六月丁香| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 少妇人妻一区二区三区视频| 2021天堂中文幕一二区在线观| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| 欧美黑人巨大hd| 亚洲精品456在线播放app | 男女下面进入的视频免费午夜| 噜噜噜噜噜久久久久久91| 国产视频内射| 久久久久久久久中文| 此物有八面人人有两片| 久久久精品欧美日韩精品| 美女 人体艺术 gogo| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看 | av中文乱码字幕在线| 欧美性猛交黑人性爽| 在线看三级毛片| 欧美激情国产日韩精品一区| 一级a爱片免费观看的视频| 99久久无色码亚洲精品果冻| 一卡2卡三卡四卡精品乱码亚洲| 欧美日韩瑟瑟在线播放| 色噜噜av男人的天堂激情| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 99热6这里只有精品| 免费一级毛片在线播放高清视频| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 老司机午夜福利在线观看视频| 免费av不卡在线播放| 久久婷婷人人爽人人干人人爱| 亚洲精华国产精华精| 真实男女啪啪啪动态图| 成人av一区二区三区在线看| 嫩草影院精品99| 最新在线观看一区二区三区| 国产探花极品一区二区| 久久亚洲精品不卡| a在线观看视频网站| 天堂av国产一区二区熟女人妻| 少妇人妻一区二区三区视频| 日韩高清综合在线| 久久人妻av系列| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 99九九线精品视频在线观看视频| 久久草成人影院| 少妇熟女aⅴ在线视频| 久久久久久久久大av| 国产亚洲精品久久久com| a级一级毛片免费在线观看| 亚洲乱码一区二区免费版| 欧美成人性av电影在线观看| 在线播放国产精品三级| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 成年女人永久免费观看视频| 干丝袜人妻中文字幕| 网址你懂的国产日韩在线| 国产精品一区二区三区四区久久| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 搡老熟女国产l中国老女人| 一级a爱片免费观看的视频| 久久午夜福利片| 亚洲三级黄色毛片| 日日啪夜夜撸| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 亚洲av成人精品一区久久| 欧美又色又爽又黄视频| 日本爱情动作片www.在线观看 | 国产综合懂色| 老熟妇仑乱视频hdxx| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图| 韩国av在线不卡| 不卡视频在线观看欧美| 色综合亚洲欧美另类图片| 久久久色成人| 精品久久久久久久人妻蜜臀av| 97人妻精品一区二区三区麻豆| 亚洲精华国产精华精| 免费人成在线观看视频色| 欧美日本亚洲视频在线播放| 久久久久久久精品吃奶| av黄色大香蕉| 免费观看在线日韩| 国产国拍精品亚洲av在线观看| 国产精品美女特级片免费视频播放器| 色综合婷婷激情| 国产精品无大码| 久久久久国内视频| 精品久久久噜噜| 此物有八面人人有两片| 久久6这里有精品| 老女人水多毛片| 男女啪啪激烈高潮av片| 免费人成视频x8x8入口观看| 啦啦啦观看免费观看视频高清| av在线蜜桃| 露出奶头的视频| 色尼玛亚洲综合影院| 日本免费一区二区三区高清不卡| 国产精品女同一区二区软件 | av天堂在线播放| 亚洲欧美日韩卡通动漫| 久久久国产成人精品二区| 18+在线观看网站| 3wmmmm亚洲av在线观看| 日韩亚洲欧美综合| 日韩中文字幕欧美一区二区| 中亚洲国语对白在线视频| 免费看光身美女| 乱人视频在线观看| 亚洲无线观看免费| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| 国产精品亚洲美女久久久| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 99久久精品一区二区三区| 色噜噜av男人的天堂激情| 亚洲四区av| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添av毛片 | 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 黄色配什么色好看| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| x7x7x7水蜜桃| 国产高清不卡午夜福利| 欧美日韩中文字幕国产精品一区二区三区| 色尼玛亚洲综合影院| 国产av在哪里看| 午夜老司机福利剧场| 欧美日本视频| 久久99热这里只有精品18| 国产麻豆成人av免费视频| 久久久久国产精品人妻aⅴ院| 香蕉av资源在线| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 国产乱人伦免费视频| 波多野结衣高清作品| 蜜桃亚洲精品一区二区三区| 在线天堂最新版资源| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 国产久久久一区二区三区| 国产女主播在线喷水免费视频网站 | xxxwww97欧美| 麻豆成人av在线观看| 琪琪午夜伦伦电影理论片6080| 又爽又黄a免费视频| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 国产成人影院久久av| 中文资源天堂在线| 国产精品一及| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| av天堂中文字幕网| 亚洲最大成人av| av黄色大香蕉| 日日啪夜夜撸| 久久九九热精品免费| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱 | 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 变态另类丝袜制服| 日韩欧美在线二视频| 中文字幕av成人在线电影| 99久久无色码亚洲精品果冻| 波多野结衣巨乳人妻| www.色视频.com| aaaaa片日本免费| 亚洲国产高清在线一区二区三| 69人妻影院| av专区在线播放| 国产蜜桃级精品一区二区三区| 色视频www国产| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 夜夜爽天天搞| 亚洲成av人片在线播放无| 日韩欧美三级三区| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| 22中文网久久字幕| videossex国产| 日本熟妇午夜| 亚洲精品国产成人久久av| 老师上课跳d突然被开到最大视频| 国产三级在线视频| 麻豆国产av国片精品| 国产高清不卡午夜福利| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清| 免费在线观看日本一区| 香蕉av资源在线| 如何舔出高潮| 神马国产精品三级电影在线观看| 日本一二三区视频观看| 色综合色国产| 欧美日韩乱码在线| 波多野结衣巨乳人妻| 久久中文看片网| 蜜桃久久精品国产亚洲av| 色在线成人网| 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 成年女人看的毛片在线观看| 99国产精品一区二区蜜桃av| 中国美白少妇内射xxxbb| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| 久久久久久久久久久丰满 | 欧美日韩乱码在线| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 深夜精品福利| 内地一区二区视频在线| 婷婷色综合大香蕉| 韩国av在线不卡| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在 | 中出人妻视频一区二区| 亚洲一区二区三区色噜噜| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 日韩在线高清观看一区二区三区 | 午夜精品在线福利| 亚洲av五月六月丁香网| 嫩草影院精品99| 精品久久久噜噜| 久久这里只有精品中国| 成人鲁丝片一二三区免费| 中文字幕av成人在线电影| 亚州av有码| 成年女人毛片免费观看观看9| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 国产精品久久久久久av不卡| 日韩一区二区视频免费看| avwww免费| 国产成人aa在线观看| 欧美区成人在线视频| 欧美日韩乱码在线| 窝窝影院91人妻| 美女大奶头视频| 在现免费观看毛片| 人妻制服诱惑在线中文字幕| 99在线人妻在线中文字幕| 天堂影院成人在线观看| 亚洲自拍偷在线| 免费在线观看成人毛片| 精品人妻偷拍中文字幕| 精品一区二区三区视频在线| 99视频精品全部免费 在线| 免费人成视频x8x8入口观看| 看黄色毛片网站| 精品午夜福利在线看| 日本 av在线| 麻豆av噜噜一区二区三区| 一区二区三区激情视频| 男女视频在线观看网站免费| 久久久久免费精品人妻一区二区| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| 亚洲国产精品久久男人天堂| avwww免费| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| 午夜影院日韩av| 亚洲人与动物交配视频| ponron亚洲| 最近在线观看免费完整版| 在线天堂最新版资源| 高清毛片免费观看视频网站| 国产免费男女视频| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图| 中文资源天堂在线| 日韩强制内射视频| 国产精品人妻久久久久久| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 99热这里只有是精品50| 国产男人的电影天堂91| 男女那种视频在线观看| 亚洲精品色激情综合| 真实男女啪啪啪动态图| 99热只有精品国产| 国产单亲对白刺激| 一进一出抽搐gif免费好疼| a在线观看视频网站| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看| 在线观看美女被高潮喷水网站| 天堂av国产一区二区熟女人妻| 九色成人免费人妻av| 国产精品久久电影中文字幕| 精品一区二区三区视频在线观看免费| 日本a在线网址| 国产探花极品一区二区| 久久久久久久久久黄片| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 国产av在哪里看| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 色哟哟哟哟哟哟| 亚洲四区av| 国产午夜精品论理片| 亚州av有码| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 亚洲国产欧洲综合997久久,| 久久人妻av系列| 97超视频在线观看视频| 亚洲天堂国产精品一区在线| 国产伦精品一区二区三区视频9| 免费看av在线观看网站| 亚洲精品亚洲一区二区| 国产成人av教育| 国内少妇人妻偷人精品xxx网站| 亚洲欧美清纯卡通| 国产视频一区二区在线看| 欧美xxxx黑人xx丫x性爽| 亚洲专区国产一区二区| 精品免费久久久久久久清纯| 免费看光身美女| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 色5月婷婷丁香| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 成人永久免费在线观看视频| 国产伦人伦偷精品视频| 在线播放无遮挡| 亚洲av日韩精品久久久久久密| 真人做人爱边吃奶动态| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 看黄色毛片网站| 观看美女的网站| 白带黄色成豆腐渣| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| av福利片在线观看| 俺也久久电影网| 两个人的视频大全免费| 亚洲人与动物交配视频| 99国产极品粉嫩在线观看| 国产精品伦人一区二区| 黄片wwwwww| 一级av片app| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看| 国产精品av视频在线免费观看| 久久精品国产亚洲av天美| 亚洲av.av天堂| 大又大粗又爽又黄少妇毛片口| 九九热线精品视视频播放| 亚洲在线观看片| 免费高清视频大片| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添小说| 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 中文字幕高清在线视频| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 国产探花极品一区二区| 一个人观看的视频www高清免费观看| 国产成人一区二区在线| 日本a在线网址| 国产黄片美女视频| 成人无遮挡网站| 欧美区成人在线视频| 午夜精品在线福利| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 无人区码免费观看不卡| av天堂在线播放| 如何舔出高潮| 精品一区二区免费观看| 99热这里只有是精品50| 国产男人的电影天堂91| a级一级毛片免费在线观看| 琪琪午夜伦伦电影理论片6080| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 观看美女的网站| 搞女人的毛片| 少妇猛男粗大的猛烈进出视频 | 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 在线看三级毛片| 日韩中字成人| 国产高清三级在线| 综合色av麻豆| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 国产成人影院久久av| 悠悠久久av| 国产亚洲欧美98| 男女做爰动态图高潮gif福利片| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆 | 国产三级中文精品| 又爽又黄无遮挡网站| 国产主播在线观看一区二区| 国产成人a区在线观看| 色哟哟·www| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 欧美zozozo另类| 久久精品91蜜桃| 91精品国产九色| 少妇的逼好多水| 女同久久另类99精品国产91| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 精品久久国产蜜桃| 精品不卡国产一区二区三区| 成人二区视频| 亚洲av日韩精品久久久久久密| 亚洲成人免费电影在线观看| 久久草成人影院| 91久久精品国产一区二区三区| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡|