• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Addressing risk of bias in trials of cognitive behavioral therapy

    2015-12-09 06:33:40KatherineBUTTONMarcusMUNAF
    上海精神醫(yī)學(xué) 2015年3期
    關(guān)鍵詞:廣泛性網(wǎng)狀心理治療

    Katherine S. BUTTON*, Marcus R. MUNAFò

    Addressing risk of bias in trials of cognitive behavioral therapy

    Katherine S. BUTTON1,*, Marcus R. MUNAFò2

    cognitive behavioral therapy; psychotherapy; generalized anxiety disorder; randomized controlled trial; meta-analysis; network meta-analysis; psychological placebo

    1. Introducti on

    A recent network meta-analysis by Zhu and colleagues[1]compared two different comparators (psychological placebo and waitlist control) in trials assessing the effectiveness of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) for the treatment of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD).CBT was found to be superior to waitlist control and(to a lesser extent) to psychological placebo. Moreover,psychological placebo was also superior to waitlist control, which might be interpreted as evidence of the need to control for the non-specific effects of therapy,such as therapist and researcher contact ti me. However,we argue that ‘psychological placebo’ is a misnomer as it fails to meet key criteria for controlling for placebo effects.

    Zhu and colleagues also identified problems with study quality in this literature. Using the GRADE criteria for study quality (http://www.gradeworkinggroup.org/intro.htm) developed by the Cochrane Collaboration,they found that the quality of the evidence supporting the conclusion that CBT was effective was poor.[2]Eight of the 12 studies they identified were classified as at ‘high risk of bias’ and the quality of evidence as a whole was classified as ‘moderate’, suggesting that the evidence for the effectiveness of CBT for GAD is not yet robust. This is surprising given the confidence many practitioners have in the effectiveness of CBT. However,the evidence-base for psychotherapy as a whole suffers from well-documented methodological and conceptual problems that prevent adequate placebo control and,thus, undermine the strength of casual inferences.[3-5]This commentary discusses these problems and suggests potential solutions.

    2. Causal inference, placebo effects, and the importance of blinding

    Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) are the gold standard for causal inference, and underpin evidence based practice.[6]Concealed random allocation ensures both measured and unmeasured confounders are randomly distributed across treatment arms. The control condition or comparator excludes changes that are not caused by the intervention, such as natural recovery over ti me, and placebo effects (i.e., differences in outcomes due to psychological factors such as expectancy of improvement). Placebo effects are powerful, producing clinically important improvements that correlate with expectations of improvement.[3]Blinding of participants, clinicians, and researchers to treatment allocation prevents the introduction of bias in the reporting and assessment of outcomes.

    In medical trials, placebo effects are controlled for by means of an identical but ‘non-active’ comparator,such as a sugar pill for antidepressants or sham surgery for deep brain stimulation. The patient can be blinded as to whether they are in the active or placebo condition, and expectancy of improvement can be held constant across both treatment arms. Placebo effects can then be confidently excluded as a potential cause of any observed treatment effect. However, RCTs can yield biased results if they fail to meet these standards for blinding.[6]

    Results of RCTs may be biased by psychological factors, such as a desire among participants or researchers for the trial to ‘work’ (demand characteristics) influencing outcome assessment,[4]or when participants self-report outcomes in a manner which they think desirable to the trial team. Researchers may treat patients in the active and comparator arms differently, or complete the outcome assessment in a biased way. Blinding of patients and researchers is therefore crucial to ensure unbiased results.[7]The extent and success of blinding often depends on the type of comparator; for example, it is relatively easy to blind all participants and researchers when a sugar pill is used as the comparator, but it may not be possible to blind the surgical team when comparing an actual surgery and a sham surgery.

    2.1 The problems with psychological placebos

    The purpose of a placebo is to control for the psychological effects of administering a treatment.[3]To achieve this, the placebo must appear identical to the active treatment but be missing the ‘a(chǎn)ctive ingredient’, and the patient must be blinded. These conditions do not map well to psychotherapy. CBT targets psychological processes, so disentangling placebo effects from treatment effects is challenging. It is difficult to create an ‘inactive’ but comparable psychological control, as the more similar the psychological placebo appears to CBT the more‘a(chǎn)ctive’ it is likely to be. Relaxati on therapy or supportive consultation are often used as control conditions for CBT trials, but these may still have therapeutic effects.Finally, and critically, it is impossible to blind participants and therapists, as CBT involves active engagement with its theoretical rationale.[4]We concur with others who suggest that the concept of a ‘psychological placebo’ is both methodologically and conceptually flawed.[3]

    Lack of blinding is arguably the main cause of the high risk of bias in CBT trials. None of the 12 studies identified in Zhu and colleagues’ review had blinded the treating therapists or the participants to their treatment allocation so the risk of bias for these items was rated as ‘high’. All of the studies had blinded assessors of various outcome measures, but in 6 of the 12 studies the main outcome measure was based on the results of a self-reported scale completed by the participant(i.e., it was not assessed by the blinded assessor). It is possible, although not straightforward, to blind outcome assessment. However, even when the assessors are effectively blinded to treatment allocation, bias could still occur due to demand characteristics if patients are aware of their treatment allocation and answer the blinded assessor’s questions in a biased manner. Thus failure to control for differences in patient expectations,and demand characteristics, coupled with subjective self-reported outcome assessment, introduces a high risk of bias.

    In the studies reviewed by Zhu and colleagues,psychological placebo was operationalized as either one-to-one supportive consultation or general group discussions of psychological problems.[1]While‘psychological placebo’ is a misnomer due to the inability to blind trial participants to their treatment allocation, controlling for non-specific factors such as therapist contact time and general supportive discussions is useful when assessing the effectiveness of the specific components of CBT such as exposure therapy and reappraisal. However, such studies –which may underestimate the beneficial effects of CBT– should be relabeled as trials comparing different psychological interventions rather than as ‘placebocontrolled’ trials.

    Given these difficulties, it is unsurprising that the majority of CBT trials use comparators such as no treatment, waitlist, or treatment as usual. Of these,treatment as usual is arguably the most informative(and conservative), because it addresses the important pragmatic question “Is CBT more effective than current best treatment?”. However, the majority of studies reviewed by Zhu and colleagues used waitlist control.In a recent network meta-analysis patients randomized to waitlist control were found to do worse than those randomized to no treatment,[8]suggesting that waitlist may act as a nocebo (i.e., having an adverse effect on patients). Patients randomized to waitlist may have negative expectations of improvement as they are placed in a state of stasis rather than actively receiving treatment. Furthermore, the sense of ‘waiting’might discourage seeking treatment elsewhere. The implication is that treatment effects from comparison with waitlist are likely to be inflated, and thus provide inaccurate estimates.

    3. What can be done to improve the evidence-base for CBT?

    Does it matter that the evidence for CBT is not robust according to the GRADE criteria? More importantly,does it matter that we cannot make causal inferences about the effectiveness of CBT because of the possibility that observed effects are due to placebo (and/or nocebo) effects, or other biases arising from lack of blinding? Pragmatically, the evidence that CBT is superior to treatment as usual, irrespective of how,may be sufficient to guide the decision to support a treatment. However, the evidence reviewed by Zhu and colleagues is not ideal for addressing either of these points because it did not compare CBT to treatment as usual; comparisons with waitlist likely overestimate effectiveness, while comparisons with psychological controls likely underestimate effectiveness.

    The evidence for CBT from traditional RCT designs may never fully satisfy the most robust GRADE criteria.However, causal inferences about the effectiveness of CBT could be strengthened in at least three ways. First,by going back to the laboratory to understand the basic science questions of how CBT and placebo effects work.Second, by including measures of bias in controlled clinical trials to allow post-hoc adjustment for potential confounding in treatment effects. Third, by using sophisticated multi -arm analyses to assess the relative effects of components of interventions, and metaregression to explore how the level of risk of biases influences treatment effect.

    3.1 A call for basic science

    CBT is a complex intervention, comprising a range of behavioral and cognitive techniques. Elucidating which are ‘a(chǎn)ctive ingredients’ and which are redundant is an important basic science question, with clear clinical application. Understanding the mechanisms of change can lead to improved and more efficient therapies by focusing on those components which work and removing those that do not, and can lead to new avenues for treatment, potentially increasing the psychological treatment toolkit.

    One way to address basic questions of mechanism is to examine each component of CBT in isolation in the laboratory.[9]For example, the protocol for exposure therapy, a key behavioral technique used in CBT for anxiety disorders, was developed in the 1960s from the basic science of fear-extinction learning. The mechanism by which exposure to the fear stimulus(e.g., spiders, social situations) extinguishes the fear response and reduces anxiety is now well understood,and basic research continues to inform the optimization of related psychological therapies.[10]Mechanism studies could be routinely embedded within clinical trials, by incorporating measures of the psychological processes CBT is thought to target. This would be an efficient and cost-effective way to understand mechanisms. Basic science can also delineate the causal mechanisms by which placebos produce their effects.[3]The experimental pursuit of understanding the mechanisms of placebo effects will inform both our understanding of the active ingredients of current therapies and contribute to basic knowledge that can be used to maximize placebo effects to improve patient outcomes.More detailed understanding of placebo effects would also lead to the development of more precise measures of placebo effects which could be used to statistically adjust for these effects in RCTs (discussed below).

    3.2 A sticking plaster (bandaid) for clinical trials

    Can we control for bias introduced by inadequate blinding statistically? Repeated measures of expectancy and beliefs about the demands of the research throughout the trial would provide a means by which potential bias could be adjusted for. If such data were routinely collected, summary effect estimates could be adjusted in an evidence syntheses to account for these potential biases.

    There are theoretical reasons why we might expect that treatment effects for CBT would persist longer than placebo effects. CBT targets thinking styles, so adopting longer-term follow-up assessments (i.e., after the active treatment is completed) as the primary outcome may increase confidence in the efficacy of CBT. Similarly,research into placebo effects of antidepressants suggests that placebo effects vary with severity, being greatest for mild depression and decreasing as the severity of the depressive symptoms of participants increases.[11]If this also holds for GAD, subgroup analyses based on the severity of GAD might indirectly inform causal inference. The more we understand about the mechanisms underlying placebo and CBT effects, the better we can design pragmatic clinical trials to overcome the limitations associated with lack of blinding.

    3.3 Network meta-analysis and evidence synthesis

    Treatment effects are relative, determined as much by the comparator as by the intervention. Network-meta analysis provides a powerful tool for comparing how treatment effects vary when different comparators are employed. To retain power in pairwise comparisons in traditional meta-analysis, different comparators (which can have very different effects on treatment effects)tend to be combined into a single control group, or separated as subgroups. Network meta-analysis goes beyond the traditional meta-analysis approach by additionally allowing indirect comparisons between the various control conditions, which can be quite useful.For example, this approach helped identify the nocebo effects of waitlist control, which indirectly suggests that the treatment effects reported from trials using waitlist controls likely over-estimate the effectiveness of CBT.[8]

    One way to overcome issues of expectancy and lack of blinding is to compare the same therapy with one or more components added or removed, to assess whether or not the component(s) directly causes the treatment target. This approach should control for most forms of bias (with the possible exception of therapist blinding), as patients would expect to improve similarly in both arms (assuming they are naive to the missing ingredient). However, these types of comparison trials would require large numbers of patients to have sufficient statistical power to detect the potentially small treatment effects attributable to single techniques.Alternatively, if different trials used slightly different CBT protocols, network meta-analyti c approaches might provide a means of assessing the relative effectiveness of these protocols and, thus, assess the effects of the CBT components that are different between the trials.Finally, network meta-analysis could also be used to estimate the direction and extent of bias caused by lack of blinding and to make appropriate adjustments in the final estimates of the effectiveness of CBT.[12]

    4. Closing remarks

    In most treatment studies of CBT (and other psychotherapeutic interventions) strong causal inferences about effectiveness are not justified because of the possibility that observed effects are due to placebo (and/or nocebo) effects or other biases arising from lack of blinding. Addressing the basic science questions of how CBT and placebos work will provide a better understanding of what is causal in complex interventions. Such knowledge could lead to the development of more efficient or novel psychological interventions and to improved trial design. The better we understand placebo effects, the better we can assess and quantify them, allowing more precise measures of the placebo effect to be incorporated into clinical trials and adjusted for in statistical analyses.Similarly, we can include measures of the psychological processes thought to be targeted by CBT in clinical trials and assess how these measures change during the course of CBT treatment. Therefore, while it may be virtually impossible to remove potential bias in RCTs of psychotherapy that arise from the placebo effect and the lack of effective blinding, we can improve on the status quo by integrating basic science within applied trials to adjust for these biases and, thus, make stronger causal inferences.

    Acknowledgements

    The authors wish to thank Dr. Deborah Caldwell for her helpful comments. KSB is funded by the National Institute for Health Research School for Primary Care Research (NIHR SPCR). The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS,the NIHR or the Department of Health of the United Kingdom. The NIHR SPCR is a partnership between the Universities of Birmingham, Bristol, Keele, Manchester,Nottingham, Oxford, Southampton, and University College London. MRM is a member of the UK Centre for Tobacco Control Studies, a UKCRC Public Health Research Centre of Excellence. Funding from the Briti sh Heart Foundation, Cancer Research UK, Economic and Social Research Council, Medical Research Council, and the National Institute for Health Research, under the auspices of the UK Clinical Research Collaboration, is gratefully acknowledged.

    Con flict of interest

    The authors report no con flict of interest related to this manuscript.

    Funding

    1. Zhu Z, Zhang L, Jiang J, Li W, Cao X, Zhou Z, et al. Comparison of psychological placebo and waiti ng list control conditions in the assessment of cognitive behavioral therapy for the treatment of generalized anxiety disorder: a meta-analysis.Shanghai Arch Psychiatry.2014; 26(6): 319-331. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.11919/j.issn.1002-0829.214173

    2. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Vist GE, Kunz R, Falck-Ytter Y, Alonso-Coello P, et al. GRADE: an emerging consensus on rating quality of evidence and strength of recommendati ons.BMJ.2008; 336(7650): 924-926. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.39489.470347.AD

    3. Kirsch I. Placebo psychotherapy: Synonym or oxymoron?J Clin Psychol.2005; 61(7): 791-803. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20126

    4. Borkovec TD, Sibrava NJ. Problems with the use of placebo conditions in psychotherapy research, suggested alternatives, and some strategies for the pursuit of the placebo phenomenon.J Clin Psychol.2005; 61(7): 805-818.doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20127

    5. Flint J, Cuijpers P, Horder J, Koole SL, Munafo MR. Is there an excess of significant findings in published studies of psychotherapy for depression?Psychol Med.2015; 45: 439-446

    6. Schulz KF, Altman DG, Moher D, Group C. CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials.BMJ.2010; 340: c332. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-152-11-201006010-00232

    7. Juni P, Altman DG, Egger M. Systematic reviews in health care - Assessing the quality of controlled clinical trials.Brit Med J.2001; 323(7303): 42-46

    8. Furukawa TA, Noma H, Caldwell DM, Honyashiki M, Shinohara K, Imai H, et al. Waiting list may be a nocebo condition in psychotherapy trials: a contribution from network meta-analysis.Acta psychiatr Scand.2014; 130(3):181-192. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/acps.12275

    9. Holmes EA, Craske MG, Graybiel AM. Psychological treatments: A call for mental-health science.Nature.2014;511(7509): 287-289. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/511287a

    10. Graham BM, Milad MR. The study of fear extinction:implications for anxiety disorders.Am J Psychiatry.2011;168(12): 1255-1265. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11040557

    11. Kirsch I, Deacon BJ, Huedo-Medina TB, Scoboria A, Moore TJ, Johnson BT. Initial severity and anti depressant benefits:a meta-analysis of data submitted to the Food and Drug Administration.PLoS Med.2008; 5(2): e45. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0050045

    12. Dias S, Welton NJ, Marinho VCC, Salanti G, Higgins JPT,Ades AE. Estimati on and adjustment of bias in randomized evidence by using mixed treatment comparison metaanalysis.J R Stat Soc A Stat.2010; 173: 613-629. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-985X.2010.00639.x

    (received, 2015-03-23; accepted, 2015-04-05)

    Katherine Butt on is a National Institute for Health Research School for Primary Care Post-doctoral Fellow at the University of Bristol in the United Kingdom. Her research focuses on the interface between neuroscience and psychiatry, where she aims to translate insights from the neuroscience of social cognitive mechanisms of anxiety and depression to clinical application in primary care.Katherine read neuroscience as an undergraduate at Fitzwilliam College, University of Cambridge,before moving to the University of Bristol to complete a MEd in Psychology of Education. In January 2013 she was awarded a PhD in Psychiatry from the School of Social and Community Medicine,University of Bristol, where she was a Medical Research Council Centenary Award Fellow until starting her current fellowship in April 2014.

    解決認(rèn)知行為治療研究中存在的偏倚風(fēng)險(xiǎn)

    Butt on KS, Munafo MR

    認(rèn)知行為治療;心理治療;廣泛性焦慮障礙;隨機(jī)對(duì)照研究;meta分析;網(wǎng)狀meta分析;心理安慰劑

    Summary:A recent network meta-analysis by Zhu and colleagues reported in theShanghai Archives of Psychiatrycompared two different comparators (psychological placebo and waitlist control) in trials assessing the effectiveness of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) for the treatment of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). CBT was superior to both of these control conditions, but psychological placebo was superior to waitlist. However, we argue that the term ‘psychological placebo’ is a misnomer because the impossibility of effectively blinding participants to treatment allocation in CBT trials makes it impossible to control for placebo effects. This failure to blind participants and therapists – and the resultant high risk of bias – was the main reason Zhu and colleagues found that the overall quality of the evidence supporting the conclusion that CBT is effective for GAD is poor. This is a general problem in all psychotherapy trials, which suffer from well-documented methodological and conceptual problems that prevent adequate placebo control and undermine casual inference. We discuss these problems and suggest potential solutions. We conclude that,while it may be difficult to remove potential bias in randomized controlled trials of psychotherapy, we can improve on the status quo by integrating basic science within applied trials to adjust for these biases and,thus, improve the strength of the causal inferences.

    [Shanghai Arch Psychiatry. 2015; 27(3): 144-148. Epub 2015 Apr 24.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.11919/j.issn.1002-0829.215042]

    1School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom

    2MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, School of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom

    * correspondence: Kate.Button@bristol.ac.uk

    to prepare this commentary.

    概述:《上海精神醫(yī)學(xué)》雜志最近刊登了一篇由朱智佩及其同事撰寫的網(wǎng)狀meta分析文章。該meta分析薈萃比較了采用兩種不同的對(duì)照組(心理安慰劑組和等候治療對(duì)照組)評(píng)估認(rèn)知行為治療(cognitive behavioral therapy, CBT) 對(duì) 廣泛性焦慮障礙(generalized anxiety disorder, GAD)療效的研究。CBT優(yōu)于這兩種對(duì)照,但心理安慰劑優(yōu)于等候治療。然而,我們認(rèn)為“心理安慰劑”一詞不恰當(dāng),因?yàn)樵贑BT研究中受試者不可能完全不知道治療分配,所以研究不可能真正控制安慰劑效應(yīng)。無(wú)法使受試者和治療師雙盲以及因此而產(chǎn)生的高偏倚風(fēng)險(xiǎn)正是朱智佩及其同事發(fā)現(xiàn)支持CBT治療GAD有效的證據(jù)整體質(zhì)量差的主要原因。上述問題在所有的心理治療研究中普遍存在,方法學(xué)問題以及概念性問題限制了安慰劑對(duì)照的作用,并削弱了對(duì)因果關(guān)系的推斷作用。本文討論了這些問題并提出可能的解決方案。我們的結(jié)論是,雖然在心理治療的隨機(jī)對(duì)照研究中可能難以完全消除潛在的偏倚,但是我們可以在進(jìn)行研究時(shí)整合基礎(chǔ)科學(xué)知識(shí),來校正這些偏倚,改進(jìn)現(xiàn)狀,從而提高因果推論的強(qiáng)度。

    本文全文中文版從2015年08月06日起在http://dx.doi.org/10.11919/j.issn.1002-0829.215042可供免費(fèi)閱覽下載

    猜你喜歡
    廣泛性網(wǎng)狀心理治療
    廣泛性焦慮障礙中醫(yī)證候、體質(zhì)及其關(guān)系的研究進(jìn)展
    不同針灸療法治療尋常痤瘡的網(wǎng)狀Meta分析
    SWRH82B熱軋盤條心部異常網(wǎng)狀滲碳體組織分析及改善措施
    昆鋼科技(2022年1期)2022-04-19 11:36:16
    心理治療有七大誤區(qū)
    8種針灸療法治療原發(fā)性痛經(jīng)的網(wǎng)狀Meta分析
    淺析壯族巫醫(yī)治病中的心理治療作用
    中老年腦溢血患者采用精神護(hù)理與心理治療干預(yù)的效果觀察
    低頻重復(fù)經(jīng)顱磁刺激聯(lián)合文拉法辛治療廣泛性焦慮障礙的早期療效
    文拉法新緩釋片合并認(rèn)知行為療法治療廣泛性焦慮癥效果觀察
    二維網(wǎng)狀配聚物[Co(btmb)2(SCN)2]n的合成、晶體結(jié)構(gòu)和Pb2+識(shí)別性能
    国产又色又爽无遮挡免费看| 久久精品91无色码中文字幕| 国产黄片美女视频| bbb黄色大片| 国产一区二区激情短视频| 又粗又爽又猛毛片免费看| 国产亚洲精品av在线| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 午夜视频精品福利| 99国产精品99久久久久| 亚洲一卡2卡3卡4卡5卡精品中文| or卡值多少钱| av欧美777| 12—13女人毛片做爰片一| 可以在线观看毛片的网站| 日韩有码中文字幕| 听说在线观看完整版免费高清| 亚洲国产看品久久| 三级国产精品欧美在线观看 | 18禁黄网站禁片午夜丰满| 51午夜福利影视在线观看| 午夜a级毛片| 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 50天的宝宝边吃奶边哭怎么回事| 一本久久中文字幕| 亚洲熟妇熟女久久| 亚洲全国av大片| 欧美日本视频| 操出白浆在线播放| 最近最新免费中文字幕在线| 琪琪午夜伦伦电影理论片6080| 在线观看免费视频日本深夜| 午夜精品一区二区三区免费看| bbb黄色大片| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 成在线人永久免费视频| 欧美午夜高清在线| 日韩精品青青久久久久久| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| svipshipincom国产片| 精品一区二区三区四区五区乱码| 99国产综合亚洲精品| av在线播放免费不卡| 成人特级黄色片久久久久久久| 久久国产精品影院| 亚洲成av人片在线播放无| 精品久久久久久久久久免费视频| 1024手机看黄色片| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品 | 岛国视频午夜一区免费看| 国产男靠女视频免费网站| 亚洲国产高清在线一区二区三| 精品久久久久久久人妻蜜臀av| 亚洲国产中文字幕在线视频| 日韩高清综合在线| 亚洲av日韩精品久久久久久密| 美女午夜性视频免费| 麻豆成人av在线观看| 看免费av毛片| 久久草成人影院| 久久性视频一级片| 亚洲五月天丁香| 听说在线观看完整版免费高清| 久久久水蜜桃国产精品网| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线| 国产成人系列免费观看| 日本黄色视频三级网站网址| 女人爽到高潮嗷嗷叫在线视频| 久久午夜综合久久蜜桃| 日本黄色视频三级网站网址| 麻豆一二三区av精品| 在线播放国产精品三级| 国产成人av教育| 1024视频免费在线观看| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品 | 国产精品自产拍在线观看55亚洲| 九九热线精品视视频播放| 久久久久久久久久黄片| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线| 人人妻,人人澡人人爽秒播| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 中文字幕av在线有码专区| 国产aⅴ精品一区二区三区波| 欧美黄色淫秽网站| 久久午夜亚洲精品久久| 亚洲精品中文字幕一二三四区| 99精品欧美一区二区三区四区| 18禁黄网站禁片免费观看直播| 国产精品亚洲一级av第二区| 在线观看舔阴道视频| 欧美最黄视频在线播放免费| 国产在线精品亚洲第一网站| 成熟少妇高潮喷水视频| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 国产成人精品无人区| 精品久久久久久久毛片微露脸| 亚洲av美国av| 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 亚洲精品一区av在线观看| 成人国产一区最新在线观看| 在线观看66精品国产| 久久精品国产综合久久久| 亚洲欧美日韩高清在线视频| 黄色丝袜av网址大全| 欧美3d第一页| 国产欧美日韩一区二区精品| 人成视频在线观看免费观看| 一个人免费在线观看电影 | 香蕉av资源在线| 男女午夜视频在线观看| 久久婷婷成人综合色麻豆| 熟女电影av网| 又紧又爽又黄一区二区| 国产精品99久久99久久久不卡| 免费在线观看视频国产中文字幕亚洲| 脱女人内裤的视频| 日本三级黄在线观看| 丝袜喷水一区| 日本在线视频免费播放| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 少妇熟女欧美另类| 精品一区二区三区视频在线| 春色校园在线视频观看| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 亚洲精品影视一区二区三区av| 丰满乱子伦码专区| 久久精品久久久久久噜噜老黄 | 男女啪啪激烈高潮av片| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在 | 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 一级毛片aaaaaa免费看小| 一卡2卡三卡四卡精品乱码亚洲| 国产视频内射| 日本撒尿小便嘘嘘汇集6| 久久久国产成人精品二区| 亚洲国产精品成人久久小说 | 夫妻性生交免费视频一级片| 国产白丝娇喘喷水9色精品| 国产成人精品久久久久久| avwww免费| 欧美性感艳星| 99国产精品一区二区蜜桃av| 日本免费a在线| 综合色av麻豆| 变态另类丝袜制服| 亚洲自偷自拍三级| 精品午夜福利在线看| 一边亲一边摸免费视频| 波多野结衣高清作品| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 美女黄网站色视频| 久久精品影院6| 九九热线精品视视频播放| 中国国产av一级| 男女边吃奶边做爰视频| 成年女人永久免费观看视频| 中文字幕熟女人妻在线| 国产精品久久久久久av不卡| 久久精品国产99精品国产亚洲性色| 天堂网av新在线| 一夜夜www| 亚洲五月天丁香| 三级毛片av免费| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 国产中年淑女户外野战色| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 亚洲欧美中文字幕日韩二区| 日韩亚洲欧美综合| 春色校园在线视频观看| 亚洲四区av| 久久久色成人| 能在线免费观看的黄片| 国产美女午夜福利| 51国产日韩欧美| 热99re8久久精品国产| 1024手机看黄色片| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 亚洲最大成人手机在线| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看| 中国美女看黄片| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 高清毛片免费观看视频网站| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 亚洲高清免费不卡视频| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 欧美zozozo另类| 99久久无色码亚洲精品果冻| 免费人成视频x8x8入口观看| 两个人视频免费观看高清| 日本爱情动作片www.在线观看| 免费人成视频x8x8入口观看| 性插视频无遮挡在线免费观看| 插阴视频在线观看视频| 99久久精品国产国产毛片| 简卡轻食公司| 51国产日韩欧美| 女人被狂操c到高潮| 如何舔出高潮| 欧洲精品卡2卡3卡4卡5卡区| 久久精品国产亚洲av天美| 高清毛片免费看| av在线老鸭窝| 99热精品在线国产| 国产在线男女| 日韩成人伦理影院| 婷婷亚洲欧美| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 91精品一卡2卡3卡4卡| av专区在线播放| 国产精品电影一区二区三区| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图| 亚洲国产欧美人成| 精品人妻视频免费看| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 国产精品久久久久久亚洲av鲁大| 国产成人freesex在线| 给我免费播放毛片高清在线观看| 亚洲av熟女| 精品久久久久久久久av| 91麻豆精品激情在线观看国产| 亚洲精品久久久久久婷婷小说 | 国产免费一级a男人的天堂| 久久久久国产网址| 美女被艹到高潮喷水动态| 午夜久久久久精精品| 国产精品久久电影中文字幕| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 麻豆成人午夜福利视频| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 插阴视频在线观看视频| 国产精品久久视频播放| 我要看日韩黄色一级片| 长腿黑丝高跟| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av| av在线观看视频网站免费| 久久国产乱子免费精品| 国产三级在线视频| 91午夜精品亚洲一区二区三区| www日本黄色视频网| 99久久中文字幕三级久久日本| 亚洲无线在线观看| 精品午夜福利在线看| 淫秽高清视频在线观看| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 国产一区亚洲一区在线观看| 久久中文看片网| 免费av毛片视频| 成人毛片60女人毛片免费| 91av网一区二区| 日韩欧美精品v在线| 2022亚洲国产成人精品| 男女啪啪激烈高潮av片| 永久网站在线| 免费观看精品视频网站| 国产探花极品一区二区| 熟妇人妻久久中文字幕3abv| 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 亚洲经典国产精华液单| 简卡轻食公司| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 美女 人体艺术 gogo| 麻豆成人午夜福利视频| 成人av在线播放网站| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 日韩高清综合在线| 国产中年淑女户外野战色| 麻豆av噜噜一区二区三区| 精品无人区乱码1区二区| 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 一进一出抽搐动态| 蜜桃久久精品国产亚洲av| 久久精品国产鲁丝片午夜精品| 亚洲国产精品国产精品| 久久亚洲国产成人精品v| 女同久久另类99精品国产91| 亚洲精品自拍成人| 亚洲成人精品中文字幕电影| 99久久中文字幕三级久久日本| 亚洲一级一片aⅴ在线观看| 爱豆传媒免费全集在线观看| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| 国产精品一及| 爱豆传媒免费全集在线观看| 最近中文字幕高清免费大全6| 日韩 亚洲 欧美在线| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 夜夜夜夜夜久久久久| 中文字幕久久专区| 国产在线男女| 亚洲国产日韩欧美精品在线观看| 又爽又黄a免费视频| 国产白丝娇喘喷水9色精品| 日日啪夜夜撸| 18禁黄网站禁片免费观看直播| 久久中文看片网| 高清日韩中文字幕在线| 97超视频在线观看视频| 黑人高潮一二区| 国产国拍精品亚洲av在线观看| 国产精品国产三级国产av玫瑰| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 欧美日韩乱码在线| 国产黄片视频在线免费观看| av视频在线观看入口| av卡一久久| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 青春草国产在线视频 | 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 舔av片在线| 亚洲四区av| 日韩 亚洲 欧美在线| 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 免费看av在线观看网站| 欧美色欧美亚洲另类二区| 青春草国产在线视频 | 欧美色欧美亚洲另类二区| 成人二区视频| 国产精品麻豆人妻色哟哟久久 | 午夜精品国产一区二区电影 | 青春草国产在线视频 | 看片在线看免费视频| 免费看a级黄色片| 99九九线精品视频在线观看视频| 国产91av在线免费观看| 国产一级毛片七仙女欲春2| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| 老司机影院成人| 级片在线观看| 韩国av在线不卡| 亚洲成av人片在线播放无| 夫妻性生交免费视频一级片| 欧美三级亚洲精品| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 亚洲国产精品sss在线观看| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av| 麻豆国产97在线/欧美| 国国产精品蜜臀av免费| 美女cb高潮喷水在线观看| 亚洲五月天丁香| 搡老妇女老女人老熟妇| av国产免费在线观看| 成人国产麻豆网| 岛国在线免费视频观看| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 亚洲精品456在线播放app| 日本一二三区视频观看| 中文字幕人妻熟人妻熟丝袜美| 亚洲无线观看免费| 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 色综合色国产| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| 亚洲精品粉嫩美女一区| 国产亚洲91精品色在线| 久久久久久久午夜电影| 免费观看的影片在线观看| 婷婷色av中文字幕| 免费av观看视频| 中国美白少妇内射xxxbb| 亚洲精品乱码久久久久久按摩| 久久中文看片网| 日韩强制内射视频| 国产精品美女特级片免费视频播放器| 精品久久久久久久久久久久久| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区| 99视频精品全部免费 在线| 精品人妻视频免费看| 精品一区二区免费观看| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 日韩一区二区三区影片| 特大巨黑吊av在线直播| 国内精品久久久久精免费| 两性午夜刺激爽爽歪歪视频在线观看| 成年版毛片免费区| 国产三级在线视频| 亚洲美女视频黄频| 一级毛片电影观看 | 天堂中文最新版在线下载 | 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 在线天堂最新版资源| 成人午夜高清在线视频| 尤物成人国产欧美一区二区三区| 22中文网久久字幕| 亚洲欧美日韩卡通动漫| 久久久久九九精品影院| 久久亚洲国产成人精品v| 国产三级在线视频| 色播亚洲综合网| 美女高潮的动态| av在线亚洲专区| 三级国产精品欧美在线观看| 色噜噜av男人的天堂激情| 激情 狠狠 欧美| 美女cb高潮喷水在线观看| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验 | 欧美精品国产亚洲| 青春草亚洲视频在线观看| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 直男gayav资源| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 亚洲精品久久久久久婷婷小说 | 又爽又黄无遮挡网站| 午夜视频国产福利| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在 | 国产免费一级a男人的天堂| 又粗又硬又长又爽又黄的视频 | 国产精品国产三级国产av玫瑰| 在现免费观看毛片| 久久久久久久亚洲中文字幕| 久久鲁丝午夜福利片| 亚洲自拍偷在线| 青春草亚洲视频在线观看| 高清毛片免费观看视频网站| 成人二区视频| 看片在线看免费视频| 人妻夜夜爽99麻豆av| 亚洲欧美精品自产自拍| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 免费观看人在逋| 国产69精品久久久久777片| 欧美日韩国产亚洲二区| 国产av在哪里看| 国产三级中文精品| 免费观看人在逋| 极品教师在线视频| 国产成人午夜福利电影在线观看| 深夜a级毛片| 国产精品日韩av在线免费观看| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频| 亚洲欧美精品综合久久99| 成人欧美大片| 天天躁夜夜躁狠狠久久av| 久久午夜福利片| 成人性生交大片免费视频hd| av在线天堂中文字幕| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 亚洲经典国产精华液单| 久久国产乱子免费精品| 国产高清视频在线观看网站| 亚洲欧美中文字幕日韩二区| 中文在线观看免费www的网站| 欧美+日韩+精品| 日韩制服骚丝袜av| 精品久久久久久久末码| 精品午夜福利在线看| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 国产探花在线观看一区二区| 国产极品精品免费视频能看的| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 特大巨黑吊av在线直播| 变态另类成人亚洲欧美熟女| 国产视频首页在线观看| 在线免费十八禁| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 黄色日韩在线| av免费观看日本| 精品午夜福利在线看| 亚洲婷婷狠狠爱综合网| 一进一出抽搐动态| 青春草视频在线免费观看| 国产免费一级a男人的天堂| av免费在线看不卡| 亚洲高清免费不卡视频| 蜜桃亚洲精品一区二区三区| 特级一级黄色大片| 日本与韩国留学比较| a级毛片免费高清观看在线播放| 国产淫片久久久久久久久| 精品久久久久久久人妻蜜臀av| 国产熟女欧美一区二区| 亚洲av.av天堂| 少妇的逼好多水| 九色成人免费人妻av| 91麻豆精品激情在线观看国产| 亚洲av熟女| 青春草视频在线免费观看| 五月伊人婷婷丁香| 最近手机中文字幕大全| 中国美白少妇内射xxxbb| 亚洲欧洲国产日韩| 国产日本99.免费观看| 成人欧美大片| 少妇人妻一区二区三区视频| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 中出人妻视频一区二区| 白带黄色成豆腐渣| 国产伦精品一区二区三区视频9| 在线观看美女被高潮喷水网站| 精品久久久久久成人av| 国产探花极品一区二区| 26uuu在线亚洲综合色| 最近手机中文字幕大全| 一本精品99久久精品77| 在线天堂最新版资源| 国产av不卡久久| 国产亚洲av嫩草精品影院| 亚洲av成人精品一区久久| 国产亚洲5aaaaa淫片| 能在线免费看毛片的网站| 国产成人一区二区在线| 亚洲真实伦在线观看| 欧美成人a在线观看| 国产高清三级在线| 欧美激情久久久久久爽电影| 国产极品天堂在线| 婷婷色综合大香蕉| 国产人妻一区二区三区在| 国产高清三级在线| 欧美区成人在线视频| 亚洲av.av天堂| 日韩高清综合在线| 精品久久久久久久人妻蜜臀av| 高清午夜精品一区二区三区 | 丰满乱子伦码专区| 亚洲久久久久久中文字幕| 国产亚洲精品久久久久久毛片| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| 亚洲国产精品成人久久小说 | 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 桃色一区二区三区在线观看| 午夜精品在线福利| 国产精品99久久久久久久久| av国产免费在线观看| 国产精品久久久久久av不卡| 欧美3d第一页| 天堂中文最新版在线下载 | 亚洲精品日韩av片在线观看| 国产真实乱freesex| 波野结衣二区三区在线| 国产精品免费一区二区三区在线| 精品一区二区三区视频在线| 国产精品野战在线观看| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 能在线免费看毛片的网站| 99热6这里只有精品| 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 国国产精品蜜臀av免费| 亚洲欧美精品综合久久99| 久久99蜜桃精品久久| 一级二级三级毛片免费看| 99久久人妻综合| 2022亚洲国产成人精品| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 国产久久久一区二区三区| 亚洲激情五月婷婷啪啪| 免费看美女性在线毛片视频| 免费看日本二区| 日本免费一区二区三区高清不卡| av在线天堂中文字幕| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 国产精品国产高清国产av| 99久久久亚洲精品蜜臀av| 国产精品日韩av在线免费观看| 国产美女午夜福利| 国产精品伦人一区二区| 又爽又黄无遮挡网站| 午夜福利在线在线| 久久久久九九精品影院| 变态另类成人亚洲欧美熟女| 乱人视频在线观看| 人妻久久中文字幕网| 久久精品人妻少妇| 色播亚洲综合网| 丝袜喷水一区| 色噜噜av男人的天堂激情| 激情 狠狠 欧美| 国产伦精品一区二区三区四那| 日韩制服骚丝袜av| 亚洲国产精品久久男人天堂| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 99久久精品一区二区三区| 国产午夜精品久久久久久一区二区三区| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看 | 国产精品久久久久久久久免| 国产伦理片在线播放av一区 | 免费看av在线观看网站| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在 | 国产69精品久久久久777片| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 在现免费观看毛片| 变态另类成人亚洲欧美熟女| 欧美激情在线99| 国产精品99久久久久久久久| 男人舔奶头视频| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 麻豆国产av国片精品| 小说图片视频综合网站| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 久久亚洲国产成人精品v| 久久久久久伊人网av| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 国产精品99久久久久久久久| 美女黄网站色视频| 中文字幕制服av| 99久久久亚洲精品蜜臀av| 国产成人精品婷婷| 久久人人爽人人片av| 国产精品野战在线观看| 成人三级黄色视频| 欧美一区二区亚洲| 久久精品久久久久久久性| 日本熟妇午夜| 菩萨蛮人人尽说江南好唐韦庄 | 久久午夜亚洲精品久久| 日韩欧美在线乱码|