• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Connecting the P300 to the diagnosis and prognosis of unconscious patients

    2015-01-18 05:49:38RanLiWeiqunSongJubaoDuSuHuoGuixiangShan

    Ran Li, Wei-qun Song, Ju-bao Du, Su Huo, Gui-xiang Shan

    Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China

    Connecting the P300 to the diagnosis and prognosis of unconscious patients

    Ran Li, Wei-qun Song*, Ju-bao Du, Su Huo, Gui-xiang Shan

    Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China

    The residual consciousness of unconscious patients can be detected by studying the P300, a wave among event-related potentials. Previous studies have applied tones, the subject’s name and other names as stimuli. However, the results were not satisfactory. In this study, we changed the constituent order of subjects’ two-character names to create derived names. The subject’s derived names, together with tones and their own names, were used as auditory stimuli in event-related potential experiments. Healthy controls and unconscious patients were included in this study and made to listen to these auditory stimuli. In the two paradigms, a sine tone followed by the subject’s own name and the subject’s derived name followed by the subject’s own name were used as standard and deviant stimuli, respectively. The results showed that all healthy controls had the P300 using both paradigms, and that the P300 in the second paradigm had a longer latency and two peaks. All minimally conscious state patients had the P300 in the frst paradigm and the majority of them had the P300 in the second paradigm. Most vegetative state patients had no P300. Patients who showed the P300 in the two paradigms had more residual consciousness, and patients with the two-peak P300 had a higher probability of awakening within a short time. Our experimental fndings suggest that the P300 event-related potential could refect the conscious state of unconscious patients.

    nerve regeneration; brain injury; cognitive neuroscience; event-related potential; P300; electrophysiology; nutritional state; minimally conscious state; consciousness; disorders of consciousness; unconsciousness; evaluation; prognosis; NSFC grants; neural regeneration

    Funding:This work was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China, No. 81371194.

    Li R, Song WQ, Du JB, Huo S, Shan GX (2015) Connecting the P300 to the diagnosis and prognosis of unconscious patients. Neural Regen Res 10(3)∶473-480.

    Introduction

    Defnitions for the vegetative state and minimally conscious state have been proposed for more than 10 years (Ashwal et al., 1994; Giacino et al., 2002). However, the rate of misdiagnosis is very high (Majerus et al., 2005; Schnakers et al., 2009). What’s more, could an unconscious patient come to be aware? When would he come to be aware? What will he be when wakes up? There is much uncertainty around these questions.

    Measurement of event-related potentials (ERPs) is an objective and easy method for evaluating the residual cognitive function of patients with disorders of consciousness (DOC) (Faugeras et al., 2011), and is more and more popular. The P300 is the most appropriate cognition-related wave with which to evaluate consciousness (Kotchoubey et al., 2005).

    As auditory stimuli, sine tones were widely used at first. However, the number of patients showing the P300 in response to these stimuli was limited (Cavinato et al., 2011). Even some healthy persons presented a small tone-evoked P300 (Holeckova et al., 2006; Fischer et al., 2008). In subsequent work, the subject’s own name (SON) was used (Holeckova et al., 2006; Perrin et al., 2006; Schnakers et al., 2008; Cavinato et al., 2011). However, both vegetative state and minimally conscious state patients showed the P300, which made it diffcult to differentiate between them (Perrin et al., 2006). Thus, another frst name was used as a deviant stimulus to overcome the above shortcomings. However, using these stimuli, different authors generated different results (Perrin et al., 2006; Schnakers et al., 2008). Cavinato’s study (2011) made some improvements. He used another first name as the deviant stimulus, but he also used two other paradigms with different difficulties. He thought the modulation of P300 latency according to the different tasks might predict the recovery of vegetative state to minimally conscious state. However, they could not identify differences between minimally conscious state patients and healthy controls because they both showed P300 modulation (Cavinato et al., 2011). This might be explained by salient differences between another frst name and SON, which lead to a ceiling effect, with a similar effect being seen in minimally conscious state and control subjects. What’s more, the diffculty in generating another frst name limits its use (Perrin et al., 2006; Eichenlaub et al., 2012; Ruby et al., 2013). It is necessary to fnd a stimulus that is familiar to subjects, but whichhas small differences from SON such that the differentiation of it and SON is more diffcult, thereby eliminating the ceiling effect.

    According to the concept of derived pseudo-words (changing the constituent order of the two-character real words) (Wang et al., 2009), SON is a real word and a subject’s derived name (SDN) is a derived pseudo-word. For example, if “xiao ming” is the SON, then “ming xiao” is the SDN. The same constituents in SDN and SON verify the familiarity of the SDN, while the similarity of SON and SDN increases the difficulty in differentiating them. What’s more, the SDN is easy to generate and has a similar duration to the SON.

    In the present study, we used three kinds of stimuli: a 1,000 Hz tone, the SON and the SDN. SON could elicit a larger response when uttered by a familiar voice (Holeckova et al., 2006); therefore, SON and SDN were both called by a familiar voice. These stimuli were arranged into two paradigms: (1) a 1,000 Hz tone as a standard stimulus and SON as a deviant stimulus; and (2) SDN as a standard stimulus and SON as a deviant stimulus. The expectation was that the frst paradigm would produce similar results to previous studies. The creative second paradigm would potentially allow identifcation of a higher consciousness level, and so could further select patients that might be able to regain consciousness.

    Subjects and Methods

    Subjects

    Seventeen healthy controls aged 24—56 years (8 males and 9 females, mean age 32.0 ± 8.5 years) were recruited through advertisements at Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University (Beijing, China). Thirteen DOC patients (two in a coma, six in a vegetative state and five minimally conscious state patients) aged 16—80 years (7 males and 6 females, mean age 42 ± 16 years) were recruited from the Department of Rehabilitation Medicine of Xuanwu Hospital, China between June 2013 and August 2014.

    In patients, clinical examination was performed using the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R). This scale includes six subscales: auditory, visual, motor, oromotor, communication, and arousal functions, and these subscales comprise 29 items. Apart from the arousal subscale, items marked “□”denote emergence from minimally conscious state and those marked “*” denote minimally conscious state. Items with no mark denote a vegetative state.

    According to the arousal subscale, patients in a coma are unarousable. Among them, two healthy controls and five DOC patients had a smoking history of 10—34 years. The general conditions of the included patients are shown in Table 1. Informed consent was given by the healthy controls themselves or the patients’ family members. Ethical approval was provided by the ethics committee of the hospital.

    Vegetative state patients

    According to the CRS-R scale (Kalmar and Giacino, 2005), patients who showed no behaviors marked “□” or “*” were diagnosed as being in a vegetative state. They had to be in a vegetative state for > 1 month to exclude natural recovery. Patients should show normal or slightly delayed brain stem auditory-evoked responses. Patients with unstable clinical conditions or a previous history of hearing disorders, neurological, psychiatric disorders were excluded.

    Minimally conscious state patients

    According to the CRS-R scale (Kalmar and Giacino, 2005), patients who showed one or more behaviors marked “*”were diagnosed as being in a minimally conscious state. They had to be in a minimally conscious state for > 1 month to exclude natural recovery. Patients should show normal or slightly delayed brain stem auditory-evoked responses. Patients with unstable clinical conditions or a previous history of hearing disorders, neurological, psychiatric disorders were excluded.

    Coma patients

    According to the CRS-R scale (Kalmar and Giacino, 2005), patients who were unarousable were deemed to be in a coma. These patients had to be in a coma for > 1 month to exclude natural recovery. Patients should show normal or slightly delayed brain stem auditory-evoked responses. Patients with unstable clinical conditions or a previous history of hearing disorders, neurological, psychiatric disorders were excluded.

    Healthy controls

    Persons with no history of hearing disorders were included. Persons with a previous history of neurological or psychiatric disorders were excluded.

    Clinical assessment

    After admission, patients were allowed to familiarize themselves to the ward environment for 3—4 days and for their clinical conditions to stabilize. At the same time, DOC patients were repeatedly assessed with the CRS-R scale, daily. According to the CRS-R scale, patients were divided into three groups, including coma patients (n= 2), vegetative state patients (n= 6) and minimally conscious state patients (n= 5).

    To examine the prognostic value of the ERP study, longitudinal behavioral assessments were performed at 1, 2, 3, 6, 9, and 12 months using the CRS-R scale if patients left the hospital after the ERP test. If patients were in the hospital, besides the time point above, their state of consciousness could be known at any time.

    Procedure

    There were three stimuli: a 1,000 Hz tone, the SON and the SDN. The 1,000 Hz tone lasted for 100 ms and was generated using Adobe Audition software (Adobe, Beijing Fistar Technology Limited Company, Beijing, China). The two-character SON was uttered by a frst-degree family member. For the unmarried subjects, the SON was called by their best friend. The SON provider was told to speak the SON within a time of approximately 600 ms. The SDN was similar to the SON, but was formed by reversing the constituent order ofthe SON. The mean durations of SON and SDN were 504 ms and 519 ms for healthy controls. The mean durations of SON and SDN were 591 ms and 596 ms for DOC patients. The intensity of all three stimuli was 90 dB. The stimuli were delivered binaurally through earphones with a pseudorandom inter-stimulus interval of 0.8—1.2 seconds including 100 ms as a pre-stimulus baseline. A paradigm consisted of 800 stimuli, of which 120 were the deviant stimuli. Standard stimuli and deviant stimuli were given pseudo-randomly.

    The three stimuli were arranged into two paradigms. In the frst paradigm, also called the TO paradigm, participants were presented a 1,000 Hz tone as standard stimuli and SON as deviant stimuli. The second paradigm, also called the DO paradigm, included SDN as a standard stimulus and SON as a deviant stimulus. The probability of being presented with the deviant stimuli was 0.15. The program was presented using E-prime 2.0 software (Carnegie-Mei Long University and University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA). The order of presentation of these two paradigms was counterbalanced across all subjects.

    ERPs of healthy controls were collected in the ERP experiment room. Subjects were told to clean their hair 1 day before test to decrease the electric resistance of the scalp. All subjects were asked to eat so as not to be too full or hungry. Because the P300 needed to be obtained without participants’ special attention, the healthy controls were asked to watch a silent movie to divert their attention (Holeckova et al., 2006, 2008; Muller-Gass et al., 2007). In the present study, the healthy controls were instructed to watch the nature documentary series ‘Wild China’ and to ignore the auditory stimuli. ERPs were collected at the patients’ bedsides. As for healthy controls, clean hair and appropriate energy were still necessary to assure the experiment could proceed smoothly. Because their attention resources were defcient, they did not need to watch the silent movie.

    ERP recording

    The healthy controls performed the experiment while sitting in a comfortable chair. ERP recording from DOC patients was completed with them lying in bed. ERPs were recorded from central line electrodes Fz, Cz and Pz with 64-channel electrode caps (Neuroscan Inc., Charlotte, NC, USA) according to the extended international 10—20 system. The reference electrode was placed on the nose. The ground electrode was near the FPz. Vertical electrooculogram and horizontal electrooculogram were recorded using two pairs of electrodes. For the vertical electrooculogram, the two electrodes were placed above and below the left eye. For the horizontal electrooculogram, the two electrodes were placed 10 mm from the lateral canthi of the eyes. The electrode impedance was below 5 kΩ throughout the experiment. The bandpass was 0.1—100 Hz and the sampling rate was 1,000 Hz.

    data analysis

    EOG artifacts were corrected using the method proposed by Semlitsch et al. (1986). The EEG was segmented into the epochs from 100 ms pre-stimulus to 600 ms post-stimulus. Then, baseline adjustment was done to ensure that all ERP segments had the same origin. The EEG segment contaminated by amplifier clipping, bursts of electromyography activity, or peak-to-peak defection exceeding ± 100 μV were excluded from averaging. The EEG segments were averaged separately for deviant and standard stimuli. At least 50 responses to deviant and standard stimuli were needed.

    Peak detection was used to get the amplitudes and latencies of the P300. The latency and amplitude of the peak was assessed inside a time-window around the latency of the peak in the grand average graph. The P300 component was defned as the largest visible positive peak between 250 ms and 500 ms in the TO paradigm for healthy controls. With the DO paradigm, the P300 was a complex wave with two peaks. The frst and second peaks were called DO1 and DO2, respectively. The DO1 and DO2 waves were defined as the largest visible positive peak between 380 ms and 500 ms and between 500 ms and 600 ms, respectively, in the DO paradigm for healthy controls. The P300 amplitude and latency in DOC patients were calculated manually one by one.

    Statistical analysis

    All data showed a normal distribution and are presented as the mean ± SD. Comparisons between the P300 latencies and amplitudes of healthy controls were performed. Two-way analysis of variance with repeated measures was conducted with condition (TO, DO paradigms) and scalp distribution (Fz, Cz, Pz) as factors, using SPSS 16.0 software (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). A Bonferroni test forpost-hoccomparisons was adopted when results reached signifcance atP< 0.05. When appropriate, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were made.

    Results

    P300 of healthy controls

    Figure 1 shows that SON induced a positive component in both the TO and DO paradigms between 300 ms and 600 ms. The positive component in the TO paradigm had one peak, which was called the P300. However, in the DO paradigm, there were two peaks, which were called a P300 complex wave. The frst and second peaks were called DO1 and DO2, respectively. The average amplitudes of TO, DO1 and DO2 were 6.79, 4.51, 3.90 μV, respectively. There was no significant difference between the amplitudes of DO1 and DO2. The amplitude tended to be higher in the TO paradigm than in the DO paradigm. There was a signifcant difference in amplitudes between scalp locations (P< 0.001, with LSD correction).Post-hocanalysis revealed that the P300 amplitude was slightly higher at Cz and Pz compared with Fz (P= 0.001 for Fz and Cz,P= 0.001 for Fz and Pz).

    The average latencies of TO, DO1 and DO2 were 386.49, 446.49, and 540.39 ms, respectively. Analysis of variability showed that the latencies of P300 were prolonged in response to different levels of stimuli complexity (P< 0.001, with least significant difference correction). The DO paradigm had two specifc P300 waves, and the latencies of both were signifcantly longer than that in the TO paradigm (P<0.001 for TO and DO1,P< 0.001 for TO and DO2, with the least signifcant difference correction). In the DO paradigm, the latency of the second peak was signifcantly longer than that for the first peak (P< 0.001 for DO1 and DO2, with LSD correction). The latency did not differ among Fz, Cz and Pz. Table 2 shows the exact values of the amplitudes and latencies of Fz, Cz, Pz in the TO and DO paradigms.

    Table 1 The general conditions of patients with disorders of consciousness

    Table 3 The P300 latencies and amplitudes in patients with disorders of consciousness

    Table 2 The average latencies and amplitudes of P300 at Fz, Cz, Pz in healthy controls

    Figure 1 Grand averages of P300 in healthy controls at Fz, Cz, Pz in the TO and dO paradigms.

    Figure 2 The P300 at Cz in 13 patients (No. 1-13) with disorders of consciousness.

    Table 4 The prognosis of patients with disorders of consciousness

    P300 in dOC patients

    The responses of 13 DOC patients are shown in Figure 2. According to the ERP results, the patients were classifed into four groups: the frst group is those who showed no P300 in either the TO or the DO paradigm; the second group is those who had no P300 in the TO paradigm but showed a P300 in the DO paradigm; the third group is those who had a P300 in the TO paradigm but no P300 in the DO paradigm; and the fourth group is those who showed the P300 in both TO and DO paradigms.

    Apparently, the EEG of the two coma patients was flat. The vegetative state patients were scattered among the fourgroups. Four of six vegetative state patients belonged to the first two groups. They showed no P300 in the classical TO paradigm, regardless of whether they showed a P300 in the DO paradigm. Only two of the six vegetative state patients belonged to the latter two groups. They showed a P300 in the classical TO paradigm frstly, and then in the DO paradigm. The P300 of the two vegetative state patients (No. 7, No. 8) in the TO paradigm had a relatively shorter latency and lower amplitude. The P300 of vegetative state patient No. 6 in the DO paradigm, who had no P300 in the TO paradigm, had a very low amplitude. The P300 of vegetative state patient No. 8 in the DO paradigm, who had a P300 in the TO paradigm, had an appropriate latency and slightly lower amplitude. All minimally conscious state patients were in the latter two groups. They all showed a P300 between 300 ms and 600 ms in the TO paradigm, and the P300 had a latency and amplitude similar to the average values in healthy controls. Four of fve minimally conscious state patients also showed a P300 in the DO paradigm with relatively suitable latency, but lower amplitude apart from patient No. 10. Minimally conscious state patient No. 10 not only had a P300 in the DO paradigm, but the P300 was a two-peak one. The latencies and amplitudes of these two peaks were both close to the normal values.

    Patients No. 7 and No. 9 both belonged to the third group, but had different clinical diagnoses. Although the P300 latencies were similar, the P300 amplitude of minimally conscious state patient No. 9 in the TO paradigm was obviously larger than that in vegetative state patient No. 7. Vegetative state patient No. 8 and minimally conscious state patients No. 10—13 all belonged to the fourth group, but the P300 values of the minimally conscious state patients were closer to the normal values. The P300 latencies and amplitudes of individual patients are given in Table 3.

    The prognosis of dOC patients

    As shown in Table 4, patients without a P300 in the TO paradigm had a poor prognosis regardless of the result in the DO paradigm. The prognosis of patients with a P300 in the TO paradigm was varied. Patients in the third group could be vegetative state or minimally conscious state. However, these patients could still be discriminated easily based on the amplitude and latency of P300. Patients with a lower amplitude and/or longer latency tended to be vegetative state patients; otherwise, they were minimally conscious state patients. The remaining four minimally conscious state patients in the fourth group had a P300 in both the TO and DO paradigms, and had a good prognosis. All of the patients in this group eventually awoke. The minimally conscious state patient who had a two-peak P300 wave similar to the healthy controls awoke after 10 days. Thus, the P300 in the DO paradigm might be critical to the conscious level.

    discussion

    The results of this study verified that all healthy controls showed reactions to SON, but that there was a one-peak P300 in the TO paradigm and a two-peak P300 with a longer latency and a smaller amplitude in the DO paradigm. The two coma patients showed no response to SON. Some vegetative state patients but almost all minimally conscious state patients showed reactions to SON. Patients who had the P300 in both the TO and DO paradigms seemingly had the opportunity to regain consciousness, especially the ones with a two-peak P300 in the DO paradigm.

    The responses to SON in the TO paradigm were induced in all of the healthy controls, consistent with the results of previous studies (Perrin et al., 2006; Schnakers et al., 2008; Cavinato et al., 2011; Faugeras et al., 2011). SON is used in everyday life, and is thought to be familiar to subjects. Functional MRI and ERP test results also showed that SON called by a familiar voice could induce stronger cognitive activity (Holeckova et al., 2006, 2008). SON in the present study was called by the subject’s first-degree family member or best friend. Therefore, the P300 was robustly induced in the TO paradigm. Then, the reactions of DOC patients to SON were observed. The results showed that all minimally conscious state patients and some vegetative state patients had the P300. Previous studies have been controversial around this point. Cavinato thought that all minimally conscious state patients and some vegetative state patients (6/11) showed the P300 to SON, consistent with the results of the present study (Cavinato et al., 2011). Resetti et al. (2013) found that the novelty P300 was found in all minimally conscious state patients and a large proportion of vegetative state patients. However, Fisher et al. (2010) thought the novelty P300 was not different between minimally conscious state patients and vegetative state patients, but that it had a close relationship with their etiology (being less frequent in cases of anoxia than in other etiologies). As the stimuli were similar, these controversial results might be a result of the different levels of consciousness of the DOC patients, although they were all classifed as minimally conscious state or vegetative state. However, the results also showed that evaluating the consciousness level with only the TO paradigm is far from sufficient. Because most studies found that all minimally conscious state patients had the P300, it is possible that this task was used because it is somewhat easy to do. However, a more diffcult paradigm is badly needed.

    A previous study demonstrated that transparently derived pseudo-words, which still resemble real words, are processed similar to real words unlike opaque pseudo-words which are apparently different from real words (Raettig and Kotz, 2008). Coch et al. (2012) did their research with a real word (dark), a pronounceable pseudo-word (darl), a non-pronounceable nonword (rdka), and the letter-in-xs stimuli (dxxx) and found that the real word and pronounceable pseudo-word (which resembled the real word) both elicited a larger N400 than did the nonword and letter-in-xs stimuli. They both elicited a larger N400 than the nonword and letter-in-xs stimuli (Coch et al., 2012). In the present study, SDN was chosen as the standard stimulus. It resembled the SON, so we supposed SDN might had a similar quality with SON based on the studies of Raettig and Coch, which made it difficult to discriminate from SON. Because therehave been no prior studies using SDN, the DO paradigm was used with healthy controls frst. All healthy controls had a P300 in the DO paradigm, which verified its utility. The P300 latency in the DO paradigm was longer than that in the TO paradigm. Because complex stimuli require more time to process and the information-processing speed would be slower (Hong et al., 2013; van Dinteren et al., 2014), the DO paradigm was really a more complex paradigm. The similarity of SON and SDN decreased the difference between standard and deviant stimuli and increased the diffculty of this group. The surprising result was that there were two peaks in the DO paradigm between 300 ms and 600 ms. It is possible that this refects cognitive activity. Subjects might have identified the SDN as the SON by automatically changing the order of the constituent letters. This might indicate a higher function of the brain, as suggested by the present results in DOC patients. The P300 amplitude and latency were close to the normal values in minimally conscious state patients, while those in vegetative state patients were greatly different from normal values. This fnding is also similar to those of Cavinato et al. (2011).

    Previous studies all failed to evaluate minimally conscious state or vegetative state at length. Perrin et al. (2006) studied the response to SON with another’s first name as the background. He found that all minimally conscious state patients and three of five vegetative state patients had the P300, but he still could not differentiate vegetative state from minimally conscious state patients if they all showed a P300 (Perrin et al., 2006). Faugeras’s experiment used fve tones as standard and deviant stimulus; these stimuli also changed. He found that only two of 22 vegetative state patients had P300-like responses, and these patients showed signs of consciousness within 3 to 4 days following ERP recording. However, he did not observe minimally conscious state patients (Faugeras et al., 2011). In the present study, with the help of the DO paradigm, vegetative state patients were classified into four groups and minimally conscious state patients were classifed into two groups. Thus, the consciousness level of DOC patients could be evaluated further on the basis of minimally conscious state or vegetative state. Even if patients were all evaluated as minimally conscious state according to their clinical behavior and scales, they all had different consciousness levels in reality. If the evaluation was done better, the prognosis prediction might be more accurate.

    As to the prognosis, some studies used a tone and SON as a research tool. Fischer et al. (2008) showed that, in coma patients who had been in a coma for < 3 months, a P300 was highly correlated with awakening. Cavinato suggested et al. (2009) that vegetative state patients with the P300 tended to have more chance of waking but there were no minimally conscious state patients in that study. Therefore, the present results could not provide any consolidated evidence for prognosis prediction. The present results are in accordance with Fischer’s results. Only the SON result could not accurately reflect the prognosis. Vegetative state or minimally conscious state patients with a P300 in the TO paradigm could remain in a DOC, but could also awaken. However, with the DO paradigm, it was easy to tell which patient would regain consciousness. Only those patients with a P300 in both the TO and DO paradigms would regain consciousness. Patients with a two-peak P300 in the DO paradigm would regain consciousness soon, while those with a onepeak P300 would take a longer time to regain consciousness. During this procedure, there might be a two-peak P300 that was not detected.

    With the help of the TO and DO paradigms, vegetative state and minimally conscious state patients were divided into many subgroups. That is to say, disorders of consciousness were classifed into more levels instead of just vegetative state and minimally conscious state. Thus, the evaluation of consciousness level was clearer. Accurate evaluation was a precondition of prognosis prediction. Therefore, the present results are benefcial for prognosis predicting.

    Although these results are exciting, there are some limitations. The number of patients could be further expanded. Due to the number of beds and available research time, there was no time to recruit more subjects. If possible, subjects should include locked-in patients or patients with brain injury but no disorders of consciousness. These kinds of subjects would be helpful to give a more detailed explanation of the results and fnd out more about how the P300 response is regulated differently among these subjects.

    In the present study, vegetative state patients could be differentiated from minimally conscious state patients using auditory ERPs. Long-term follow-ups verified that the present design could provide a way to predict consciousness recovery. In future clinical work, patients prone to being aware will receive direct and timely treatment, while seriously injured patients will be admitted to nursing homes rather than receiving endless treatments. This will reduce the waste of medical resources. Future work using functional MRI is needed to observe whether SON and SDN activate similar brain areas to verify the utility of SDN and disclose the mechanisms underlying the DO paradigm.

    Acknowledgments:We gratefully acknowledge the cooperation of the subjects included in this study and we are grateful to Lun Zhao from Beijing Yiran Sunny Electronic Technology Co., Ltd., Beijing, China for his contribution to the statistic analysis.

    Author contributions:RL designed the study, collected and analyzed the data, wrote the paper and performed statistical analysis and obtained funding. WQS designed the study, revised the paper, obtained funding, provided technical support and supervised the study. JBD collected data and provided material support. SH provided technical support and supervised the study. GXS collected and analyzed data. All authors approved the final version of the paper.

    Conficts of interest:None declared.

    Ashwal S, Cranford R, Bernat J, Celesia G, Coulter D, Eisenberg H, Myer E, Plum F, Walker M, Watts C (1994) Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state. N Engl J Med 330:1499-1508.

    Cavinato M, Freo U, Ori C, Zorzi M, Tonin P, Piccione F, Merico A (2009) Post-acute P300 predicts recovery of consciousness from traumatic vegetative state. Brain Inj 23:973-980.

    Cavinato M, Volpato C, Silvoni S, Sacchetto M, Merico A, Piccione F (2011) Event-related brain potential modulation in patients with severe brain damage. Clin Neurophysiol 122:719-724.

    Coch D, Mitra P, George E (2012) Behavioral and ERP evidence of word and pseudoword superiority effects in 7- and 11-year-olds. Brain Res 1486:68-81.

    Eichenlaub JB, Ruby P, Morlet D (2012) What is the specifcity of the response to the own frst-name when presented as a novel in a passive oddball paradigm? An ERP study. Brain Res 1447:65-78.

    Faugeras F, Rohaut B, Weiss N, Bekinschtein T, Galanaud D, Puybasset L, Bolgert F, Sergent C, Cohen L, Dehaene S (2011) Probing consciousness with event-related potentials in the vegetative state. Neurology 77:264-268.

    Fischer C, Dailler F, Morlet D (2008) Novelty P3 elicited by the subject’s own name in comatose patients. Clin Neurophysiol 119:2224-2230.

    Fischer C, Luaute J, Morlet D (2010) Event-related potentials (MMN and novelty P3) in permanent vegetative or minimally conscious states. Clin Neurophysiol 121:1032-1042.

    Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DI, Kelly JP, Rosenberg JH, Whyte J, Zafonte R (2002) The minimally conscious state defnition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology 58:349-353.

    Holeckova I, Fischer C, Giard MH, Delpuech C, Morlet D (2006) Brain responses to a subject’s own name uttered by a familiar voice. Brain Res 1082:142-152.

    Holeckova I, Fischer C, Morlet D, Delpuech C, Costes N, Mauguière F (2008) Subject’s own name as a novel in a MMN design: a combined ERP and PET study. Brain Res 1189:152-165.

    Hong JS, Lee JH, Yoon YH, Choi JH, Shin JE, Kim SM, Park YG (2013) The assessment of reliability of cognitive evoked potential in normal person. Ann Rehabil Med 37:263-268.

    Kalmar K, Giacino JT (2005) The JFK coma recovery scale-revised. Neuropsychol Rehabil 15: 454-460.

    Kotchoubey B, Lang S, Mezger G, Schmalohr D, Schneck M, Semmler A, Bostanov V, Birbaumer N (2005) Information processing in severe disorders of consciousness: vegetative state and minimally conscious state. Clin Neurophysiol 116:2441-2453.

    Majerus S, Gill-Thwaites H, Andrews K, Laureys S (2005) Behavioral evaluation of consciousness in severe brain damage. Prog Brain Res 150:397-413.

    Muller-Gass A, Macdonald M, Schr?ger E, Sculthorpe L, Campbell K (2007) Evidence for the auditory P3a refecting an automatic process: elicitation during highly-focused continuous visual attention. Brain Res 1170:71-78.

    Perrin F, Schnakers C, Schabus M, Degueldre C, Goldman S, Brédart S, Faymonville M-E, Lamy M, Moonen G, Luxen A (2006) Brain response to one’s own name in vegetative state, minimally conscious state, and locked-in syndrome. Arch Neurol 63:562-569.

    Raettig T, Kotz SA (2008) Auditory processing of different types of pseudo-words: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 39:1420-1428.

    Risetti M, Formisano R, Toppi J, Quitadamo LR, Bianchi L, Astolf L, Cincotti F, Mattia D (2013) On ERPs detection in disorders of consciousness rehabilitation. Front Hum Neurosci 7:775.

    Ruby P, Blochet C, Eichenlaub JB, Bertrand O, Morlet D, Bidet-Caulet A (2013) Alpha reactivity to complex sounds differs during REM sleep and wakefulness. PLoS One 8:e79989.

    Schnakers C, Perrin F, Schabus M, Majerus S, Ledoux D, Damas P, Boly M, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Bruno MA, Moonen G (2008) Voluntary brain processing in disorders of consciousness. Neurology 71:1614-1620.

    Schnakers C, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Giacino J, Ventura M, Boly M, Majerus S, Moonen G, Laureys S (2009) Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment. BMC Neurol 9:35.

    Semlitsch HV, Anderer P, Schuster P, Presslich O (1986) A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology 23:695-703.

    van Dinteren R, Arns M, Jongsma ML, Kessels RP (2014) P300 development across the lifespan: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 9:e87347.

    Wang Q, Huang H, Mao L (2009) N400 repetition effect in unidentifable Chinese characters: evidence for automatic process. Neuroreport 20:723-728.

    Copyedited by McGowan D, Norman C, Wang J, Yang Y, Li CH, Song LP, Zhao M

    10.4103/1673-5374.153699

    http://www.nrronline.org/

    Accepted: 2015-02-03

    青春草亚洲视频在线观看| 看免费成人av毛片| 亚洲在久久综合| 国产成人一区二区在线| kizo精华| 亚洲第一青青草原| 1024香蕉在线观看| 亚洲成av片中文字幕在线观看 | 国产精品麻豆人妻色哟哟久久| 丰满饥渴人妻一区二区三| 亚洲成人一二三区av| 在线观看www视频免费| 黄网站色视频无遮挡免费观看| 免费日韩欧美在线观看| 黄色视频在线播放观看不卡| 精品少妇一区二区三区视频日本电影 | 天天躁狠狠躁夜夜躁狠狠躁| 在线观看一区二区三区激情| 国产亚洲精品第一综合不卡| 亚洲国产看品久久| 欧美激情 高清一区二区三区| 亚洲精品一二三| 捣出白浆h1v1| 国产欧美亚洲国产| 欧美日韩一区二区视频在线观看视频在线| 欧美最新免费一区二区三区| 黄频高清免费视频| 亚洲一码二码三码区别大吗| 欧美日韩一区二区视频在线观看视频在线| 中文字幕另类日韩欧美亚洲嫩草| 久久久久久久久久人人人人人人| 欧美精品高潮呻吟av久久| 国产精品香港三级国产av潘金莲 | 久久久久久久久免费视频了| 欧美少妇被猛烈插入视频| 亚洲情色 制服丝袜| 91精品国产国语对白视频| 天天躁日日躁夜夜躁夜夜| 国产精品久久久久成人av| 丝袜脚勾引网站| 99热全是精品| 晚上一个人看的免费电影| 亚洲国产成人一精品久久久| 成年av动漫网址| 欧美97在线视频| 黄片小视频在线播放| 国产在线免费精品| 在线天堂最新版资源| 伊人亚洲综合成人网| 成人黄色视频免费在线看| 最近最新中文字幕免费大全7| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 精品国产乱码久久久久久小说| 久久精品久久久久久噜噜老黄| 日本wwww免费看| 少妇的丰满在线观看| 国产高清国产精品国产三级| 一区二区三区四区激情视频| 欧美日韩国产mv在线观看视频| 一本大道久久a久久精品| 在线观看www视频免费| 天堂俺去俺来也www色官网| 久久精品久久久久久久性| 亚洲国产精品国产精品| 精品视频人人做人人爽| 深夜精品福利| 免费大片黄手机在线观看| 丝袜美腿诱惑在线| 成人亚洲精品一区在线观看| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看| 久久精品国产亚洲av高清一级| av网站免费在线观看视频| 2022亚洲国产成人精品| 成人国产麻豆网| 亚洲伊人久久精品综合| 日本色播在线视频| 国产精品av久久久久免费| 国产成人精品久久二区二区91 | 久久婷婷青草| 尾随美女入室| av.在线天堂| 黄片无遮挡物在线观看| 18禁裸乳无遮挡动漫免费视频| xxx大片免费视频| 黄片播放在线免费| 久久久久精品性色| 乱人伦中国视频| 久久精品aⅴ一区二区三区四区 | 交换朋友夫妻互换小说| 精品人妻偷拍中文字幕| 日韩中字成人| 激情五月婷婷亚洲| 咕卡用的链子| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 国产乱人偷精品视频| 精品一区二区三卡| 亚洲成色77777| 1024视频免费在线观看| 性少妇av在线| 制服人妻中文乱码| 亚洲婷婷狠狠爱综合网| 黄色视频在线播放观看不卡| 黄色 视频免费看| 人妻 亚洲 视频| 成人亚洲精品一区在线观看| 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区| 日韩在线高清观看一区二区三区| 国产在线一区二区三区精| 久久久久久久亚洲中文字幕| 在线观看免费视频网站a站| 五月开心婷婷网| a级毛片在线看网站| 看非洲黑人一级黄片| 欧美中文综合在线视频| 久久精品久久精品一区二区三区| 亚洲,一卡二卡三卡| 一二三四在线观看免费中文在| 亚洲国产欧美日韩在线播放| 免费少妇av软件| 亚洲男人天堂网一区| 国产精品av久久久久免费| 91国产中文字幕| 极品人妻少妇av视频| 午夜免费鲁丝| 欧美日韩国产mv在线观看视频| 国产精品99久久99久久久不卡 | 久久精品久久久久久久性| 日日啪夜夜爽| 亚洲男人天堂网一区| 男女边摸边吃奶| av免费在线看不卡| 成人18禁高潮啪啪吃奶动态图| 精品卡一卡二卡四卡免费| 国产黄色视频一区二区在线观看| 亚洲国产欧美日韩在线播放| 男女无遮挡免费网站观看| 国产精品一区二区在线观看99| 精品人妻偷拍中文字幕| 十分钟在线观看高清视频www| 久久影院123| 婷婷色综合www| 亚洲av综合色区一区| 久热这里只有精品99| 久久久久久久久久人人人人人人| 久久免费观看电影| 国产97色在线日韩免费| 国产成人av激情在线播放| 国产精品免费视频内射| 天天躁夜夜躁狠狠躁躁| 精品一品国产午夜福利视频| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱| 免费女性裸体啪啪无遮挡网站| 一级片免费观看大全| 国产成人91sexporn| 久久青草综合色| 国产精品免费视频内射| 国产精品久久久av美女十八| 精品人妻熟女毛片av久久网站| 美女脱内裤让男人舔精品视频| 只有这里有精品99| 日韩制服骚丝袜av| 色视频在线一区二区三区| 国产成人aa在线观看| 国产野战对白在线观看| 国产精品二区激情视频| 欧美精品人与动牲交sv欧美| 男人操女人黄网站| 欧美精品av麻豆av| 久久久久网色| 丝袜喷水一区| 黄色配什么色好看| 亚洲av中文av极速乱| 亚洲一级一片aⅴ在线观看| 丝袜美足系列| 黄色 视频免费看| 一区二区日韩欧美中文字幕| 丝袜美腿诱惑在线| 亚洲天堂av无毛| 久久久久人妻精品一区果冻| 国产精品久久久久久av不卡| 丰满饥渴人妻一区二区三| 赤兔流量卡办理| 精品酒店卫生间| 观看av在线不卡| 国产成人av激情在线播放| 麻豆乱淫一区二区| 亚洲精品av麻豆狂野| 我要看黄色一级片免费的| 乱人伦中国视频| 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区| 久久人妻熟女aⅴ| 美女国产视频在线观看| av在线播放精品| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 一区福利在线观看| 永久免费av网站大全| 日韩中文字幕视频在线看片| 天天躁日日躁夜夜躁夜夜| 欧美日韩亚洲高清精品| 91精品三级在线观看| 国产极品天堂在线| 少妇的丰满在线观看| 亚洲国产最新在线播放| 午夜精品国产一区二区电影| 欧美激情 高清一区二区三区| 欧美激情高清一区二区三区 | 久久99一区二区三区| 精品一区二区免费观看| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 日韩中文字幕欧美一区二区 | 久久99热这里只频精品6学生| 亚洲欧美中文字幕日韩二区| 久久国产精品男人的天堂亚洲| www.av在线官网国产| 只有这里有精品99| 国产片内射在线| 国产精品99久久99久久久不卡 | 久久久精品免费免费高清| 亚洲国产精品一区二区三区在线| 中文字幕人妻熟女乱码| 午夜免费鲁丝| 婷婷成人精品国产| 人人妻人人爽人人添夜夜欢视频| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 国产精品亚洲av一区麻豆 | 一区二区三区乱码不卡18| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 久久这里有精品视频免费| 制服丝袜香蕉在线| 可以免费在线观看a视频的电影网站 | 免费观看a级毛片全部| 亚洲国产精品一区三区| 亚洲欧美日韩另类电影网站| 国产色婷婷99| 中文字幕人妻丝袜制服| 高清黄色对白视频在线免费看| 成人国语在线视频| 91精品伊人久久大香线蕉| 久久人人97超碰香蕉20202| 国产免费现黄频在线看| av免费观看日本| 久久综合国产亚洲精品| 亚洲精品第二区| 日韩欧美一区视频在线观看| 高清黄色对白视频在线免费看| 国产欧美日韩综合在线一区二区| 精品第一国产精品| 久久久久久免费高清国产稀缺| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看| 男女啪啪激烈高潮av片| 一边亲一边摸免费视频| 成年人午夜在线观看视频| 少妇被粗大的猛进出69影院| 免费高清在线观看视频在线观看| 国产激情久久老熟女| 国产黄色视频一区二区在线观看| 精品一区二区三卡| 欧美精品人与动牲交sv欧美| 夫妻午夜视频| 一区二区三区激情视频| 欧美人与善性xxx| 国产精品无大码| 色播在线永久视频| 国产日韩欧美亚洲二区| 在线天堂最新版资源| av又黄又爽大尺度在线免费看| 久久久久人妻精品一区果冻| 国产精品国产三级国产专区5o| 一区二区日韩欧美中文字幕| freevideosex欧美| 丰满乱子伦码专区| 亚洲五月色婷婷综合| 欧美在线黄色| 久久久久久久久久久久大奶| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看 | 国产极品粉嫩免费观看在线| 国产av国产精品国产| 久久人人97超碰香蕉20202| 99精国产麻豆久久婷婷| 99热网站在线观看| 在线 av 中文字幕| 国产午夜精品一二区理论片| 久久精品亚洲av国产电影网| 日日爽夜夜爽网站| av线在线观看网站| 一级毛片黄色毛片免费观看视频| 亚洲国产av新网站| 午夜老司机福利剧场| 免费黄频网站在线观看国产| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区黑人 | 高清在线视频一区二区三区| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| www.熟女人妻精品国产| 久久久久久免费高清国产稀缺| 在线精品无人区一区二区三| 亚洲熟女精品中文字幕| 热re99久久国产66热| 国产日韩欧美在线精品| 精品福利永久在线观看| 成人18禁高潮啪啪吃奶动态图| 精品国产超薄肉色丝袜足j| 女人久久www免费人成看片| 亚洲成国产人片在线观看| 国产成人aa在线观看| 在线天堂中文资源库| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 青青草视频在线视频观看| 自线自在国产av| 成人亚洲欧美一区二区av| 又粗又硬又长又爽又黄的视频| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区国产| 亚洲成av片中文字幕在线观看 | 国产综合精华液| 丝瓜视频免费看黄片| 男女下面插进去视频免费观看| 免费久久久久久久精品成人欧美视频| a 毛片基地| 人人妻人人爽人人添夜夜欢视频| 日产精品乱码卡一卡2卡三| 男女国产视频网站| 精品国产超薄肉色丝袜足j| 国产成人精品在线电影| 一本色道久久久久久精品综合| 在现免费观看毛片| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区| 视频区图区小说| 精品久久久精品久久久| 少妇的逼水好多| av免费观看日本| 热99国产精品久久久久久7| 亚洲人成网站在线观看播放| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| 美国免费a级毛片| 一区福利在线观看| 成人黄色视频免费在线看| videossex国产| 午夜福利乱码中文字幕| 国产黄频视频在线观看| 啦啦啦中文免费视频观看日本| 国产爽快片一区二区三区| 久久精品国产亚洲av高清一级| 国产精品欧美亚洲77777| 老汉色∧v一级毛片| 狠狠婷婷综合久久久久久88av| 男女高潮啪啪啪动态图| 999久久久国产精品视频| 亚洲国产精品一区二区三区在线| 一级毛片电影观看| 亚洲成人一二三区av| 最近中文字幕2019免费版| 亚洲综合色网址| 老汉色∧v一级毛片| 欧美日韩一区二区视频在线观看视频在线| 久久久久国产一级毛片高清牌| 狂野欧美激情性bbbbbb| 91精品三级在线观看| 一本色道久久久久久精品综合| 人妻少妇偷人精品九色| 中文字幕人妻丝袜一区二区 | 亚洲av.av天堂| 丝袜脚勾引网站| 欧美黄色片欧美黄色片| 性少妇av在线| 久久精品夜色国产| 女人久久www免费人成看片| 亚洲内射少妇av| 男人舔女人的私密视频| 日本-黄色视频高清免费观看| 亚洲欧洲日产国产| 三上悠亚av全集在线观看| 人成视频在线观看免费观看| 观看美女的网站| 欧美在线黄色| 五月伊人婷婷丁香| 在现免费观看毛片| 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看| 少妇被粗大的猛进出69影院| 亚洲国产av新网站| 99热全是精品| 国产有黄有色有爽视频| 久久热在线av| 国产一区有黄有色的免费视频| 91午夜精品亚洲一区二区三区| 观看美女的网站| 国产免费又黄又爽又色| 婷婷色综合大香蕉| 亚洲国产精品成人久久小说| www.av在线官网国产| 久久久精品94久久精品| 免费黄网站久久成人精品| 一级,二级,三级黄色视频| 一级毛片我不卡| 午夜福利,免费看| 各种免费的搞黄视频| 国产成人欧美| 中文字幕人妻丝袜制服| 久久99热这里只频精品6学生| 在线观看人妻少妇| 只有这里有精品99| 免费在线观看视频国产中文字幕亚洲 | 免费看av在线观看网站| 人人妻人人澡人人爽人人夜夜| 久久久国产一区二区| 美女视频免费永久观看网站| 亚洲人成77777在线视频| 欧美精品av麻豆av| videos熟女内射| 久久精品亚洲av国产电影网| 99久久精品国产国产毛片| 亚洲在久久综合| 亚洲综合色惰| 国产精品久久久久久精品古装| 亚洲成色77777| 久久综合国产亚洲精品| 日本91视频免费播放| 91aial.com中文字幕在线观看| 最近手机中文字幕大全| 欧美激情 高清一区二区三区| 26uuu在线亚洲综合色| 曰老女人黄片| 亚洲经典国产精华液单| 国产男人的电影天堂91| 18禁动态无遮挡网站| 最新的欧美精品一区二区| 美国免费a级毛片| 亚洲国产日韩一区二区| www.精华液| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆| 午夜av观看不卡| 亚洲欧美精品综合一区二区三区 | 又大又黄又爽视频免费| 亚洲av国产av综合av卡| 精品国产露脸久久av麻豆| 人人妻人人添人人爽欧美一区卜| 一级毛片 在线播放| 精品国产国语对白av| 欧美人与善性xxx| 高清av免费在线| 精品亚洲乱码少妇综合久久| 中文欧美无线码| 五月天丁香电影| 90打野战视频偷拍视频| 两个人看的免费小视频| 午夜福利乱码中文字幕| 亚洲一级一片aⅴ在线观看| www.自偷自拍.com| 亚洲天堂av无毛| 久久久久国产精品人妻一区二区| 国产精品av久久久久免费| 国产精品久久久久久av不卡| 国产成人一区二区在线| 91久久精品国产一区二区三区| 国产日韩欧美亚洲二区| 久久久久精品久久久久真实原创| 丝袜脚勾引网站| 国产成人精品久久久久久| videossex国产| 国产一区二区激情短视频 | 日本欧美国产在线视频| 久久久国产精品麻豆| 精品第一国产精品| 80岁老熟妇乱子伦牲交| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 精品亚洲成a人片在线观看| 国产熟女午夜一区二区三区| av女优亚洲男人天堂| 国产成人精品久久久久久| 国产成人a∨麻豆精品| 伦精品一区二区三区| 欧美少妇被猛烈插入视频| 美女午夜性视频免费| 亚洲成国产人片在线观看| 国产在线一区二区三区精| 国产精品秋霞免费鲁丝片| 欧美 日韩 精品 国产| 街头女战士在线观看网站| 97精品久久久久久久久久精品| 精品国产一区二区三区久久久樱花| 午夜久久久在线观看| 国产乱人偷精品视频| 日韩精品有码人妻一区| 在线亚洲精品国产二区图片欧美| 国产黄色免费在线视频| 久久久久国产精品人妻一区二区| 在线天堂最新版资源| 精品国产国语对白av| 亚洲人成77777在线视频| 亚洲成人手机| 国产一区二区激情短视频 | 午夜激情av网站| 伊人亚洲综合成人网| 国产 精品1| 国产女主播在线喷水免费视频网站| av国产久精品久网站免费入址| 亚洲精品美女久久久久99蜜臀 | 99精国产麻豆久久婷婷| 9191精品国产免费久久| 波野结衣二区三区在线| 国产免费视频播放在线视频| www.av在线官网国产| 看免费av毛片| 十八禁网站网址无遮挡| 两个人看的免费小视频| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看| 亚洲内射少妇av| 亚洲精品久久成人aⅴ小说| 午夜久久久在线观看| 在线观看免费视频网站a站| 又粗又硬又长又爽又黄的视频| 极品少妇高潮喷水抽搐| 十分钟在线观看高清视频www| 狠狠精品人妻久久久久久综合| 国产精品成人在线| 国产成人aa在线观看| 亚洲激情五月婷婷啪啪| 男人爽女人下面视频在线观看| 精品少妇黑人巨大在线播放| 丰满迷人的少妇在线观看| 人人妻人人爽人人添夜夜欢视频| 精品一区在线观看国产| 欧美变态另类bdsm刘玥| 亚洲欧洲精品一区二区精品久久久 | 国产成人av激情在线播放| 伦精品一区二区三区| 午夜免费鲁丝| 在线天堂中文资源库| 少妇猛男粗大的猛烈进出视频| 欧美人与善性xxx| 久久久久久久精品精品| 老汉色av国产亚洲站长工具| 免费观看性生交大片5| 女人久久www免费人成看片| 精品少妇内射三级| 叶爱在线成人免费视频播放| 超碰成人久久| 亚洲精品aⅴ在线观看| 九色亚洲精品在线播放| 亚洲av国产av综合av卡| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 成人黄色视频免费在线看| 丝袜在线中文字幕| 欧美日韩视频精品一区| 免费看av在线观看网站| 91aial.com中文字幕在线观看| 韩国av在线不卡| 亚洲精品,欧美精品| 国产精品久久久久久精品古装| 国产精品成人在线| 女人被躁到高潮嗷嗷叫费观| 亚洲欧洲国产日韩| 久久人人97超碰香蕉20202| 午夜av观看不卡| 18禁动态无遮挡网站| 国产一区二区在线观看av| tube8黄色片| 色哟哟·www| a级片在线免费高清观看视频| 欧美少妇被猛烈插入视频| 亚洲欧美成人精品一区二区| 国产日韩一区二区三区精品不卡| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看| 搡老乐熟女国产| 欧美人与善性xxx| 亚洲,一卡二卡三卡| 午夜福利在线免费观看网站| av电影中文网址| 永久网站在线| 九色亚洲精品在线播放| 欧美亚洲 丝袜 人妻 在线| 久久久久久免费高清国产稀缺| 久久99精品国语久久久| 如何舔出高潮| 日本wwww免费看| 天天躁狠狠躁夜夜躁狠狠躁| 有码 亚洲区| 亚洲第一青青草原| 亚洲,一卡二卡三卡| 免费高清在线观看日韩| 国产精品久久久久成人av| 伊人亚洲综合成人网| 青春草国产在线视频| 欧美人与性动交α欧美精品济南到 | 曰老女人黄片| 国产黄色免费在线视频| 国产黄色视频一区二区在线观看| 1024视频免费在线观看| 另类精品久久| 国产一区二区在线观看av| 午夜影院在线不卡| 国产视频首页在线观看| 国产探花极品一区二区| 亚洲成人手机| 欧美xxⅹ黑人| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 国产精品无大码| 黑人巨大精品欧美一区二区蜜桃| 极品人妻少妇av视频| 日韩视频在线欧美| 搡老乐熟女国产| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 久久国内精品自在自线图片| 视频在线观看一区二区三区| 飞空精品影院首页| 欧美日韩一级在线毛片| 国产一区有黄有色的免费视频| 大片电影免费在线观看免费| 亚洲,欧美精品.| 国产1区2区3区精品| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看| 春色校园在线视频观看| 永久免费av网站大全| 精品国产超薄肉色丝袜足j| 久久久精品国产亚洲av高清涩受|