• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Talk in the Second and Foreign Language Classroom:A Review of the Literature

    2013-03-27 06:27:13
    當(dāng)代外語研究 2013年12期

    Teachers College, Columbia University, USATeachers College, Columbia University, USATeachers College, Columbia University, USA

    Classroom interaction has long been a rich site for scholarly research, as attested by the sizable body of literature surrounding classroom discourse. This paper reviews three frameworks currently informing studies of L2 and FL classroom talk, with a focus on qualitative analyses of turn-by-turn talk between teachers and students, and amongst students themselves. Findings from studies working within (1) language socialization and sociocultural theory, (2) critical discourse analysis, and (3) conversation analysis are explored, along with the ways in which these frameworks complement and complicate one another. Finally, we briefly consider possible areas of further research and implications for teacher training.

    Keywords: classroom discourse, teacher-student interaction, qualitative research, language socialization, critical discourse analysis, conversation analysis

    Correspondence should be addressed to Catherine Box, 525 West 120th Street Box 66, New York, NY 10027, USA. Email: cmb2150@tc.columbia.edu

    INTRODUCTION

    Of the many activities that occur in the classroom,talkhappens most (Adger, 2001). While a teacher may be well-prepared to carry out the daily agenda, and supervisors may have thoughtfully constructed the teaching philosophy undergirding curricula, it is through moment-by-moment talk that teaching plans unfold. As noted by Walsh (2006), the interactive decisions that teachers make are at least as important as their preparations, since student participation and successful task completion may be helped or hindered by what teachers actually say to students (see also Hall, 1998; Nystrand, 1997; Walsh, 2002). Hall and Verplaetse (2000) pointed out that nearly all classroom activities are carried out through talk, either between teacher and student or amongst students themselves. Unsurprisingly, then, a cursory glance at research during the past fifty years reveals that classroom talk has by and large remained a fertile ground for scholarly work.

    In recent years, research on classroom discourse in second (L2) and foreign language (FL) classrooms has become increasingly popular. Interaction in these spaces takes on an especially significant role, given that it is “both the object of pedagogical attention, and the medium through which learning is accomplished” (Hall & Walsh, 2002, p. 187; see also Mori, 2002). Therefore, through examining teacher-student and student-student communication, scholars hope ultimately to gain insight into the potential link between classroom talk and second language development. Indeed, L2 researchers working within the sociocultural paradigm have maintained that language learning is not an internal assimilation of structural components of a language system, but rather a development that begins through social interaction as a learner engages in intellectual and practical activities (Hall & Walsh, 2002). Thus, a finer-grained understanding of second language development might be gained through analyzing how classroom talk engages students, and how students engage themselves in classroom talk.

    In this article, we critically review pertinent research on interaction in the L2 and FL classroom, focusing on some of the different lenses through which scholarly studies have performed classroom discourse analysis. Reviews that take into account the strengths and limitations of qualitative research on classroom discourse have been lacking (Zuengler & Mori, 2002). Thus, in an attempt to contribute to this nascent conversation, we consider findings based on transcripts of naturally-occurring, unfolding classroom talk in the L2 and FL classroom. We focus on three frameworks that have been particularly influential in generating these findings: language socialization and sociocultural theory, critical discourse analysis, and conversation analysis, with all three emphasizing the social and interactional nature of classroom talk. We begin by introducing some of the foundational works in classroom discourse that may be considered wellsprings of later developments.

    SEMINAL CLASSROOM DISCOURSE RESEARCH

    Despite the similarities in the structure of everyday conversation and classroom exchanges, findings from research have led scholars to conclude that classroom interaction differs significantly from all other talk. Nassaji and Wells (2000), for example, differentiated classroom interaction from casual conversation, reasoning that student-teacher talk typically contains a pedagogical purpose, which is not the case in conventional person-to-person interaction. Scholars have also suggested that, as in many other institutional settings, there is often an unequal distribution of conversational power in the classroom, with the teacher controlling who speaks when (Drew & Heritage, 1992; Thornborrow, 2002; Markee & Kasper, 2004; McHoul, 1978). Consequently, unlike in most conversations, not everyone has equal participation rights. Also, McHoul (1990) found that teachers often “jump in” to correct students when they do speak, a move that most interlocutors do not readily engage in during typical conversations. Mehan (1979b) explored the content of teacher talk, particularly the teacher’s use of questions in the classroom. He demonstrated that, in stark contrast to ordinary questions in everyday discussions between two people, teachers most often ask questions of students to which they already know the answer. Therefore, the teacher, unlike a usual participant in a conversation, functions as both the questioner and the primary holder of information. This creates an asymmetrical relationship, in which the teacher is both a questioner and evaluator, determining the “correctness” of the student’s answer (see also Lemke, 1990).

    Although early work on interaction in educational settings did not focus specifically on the L2 or FL classroom, these first forays into uncovering patterns of discourse in the classroom sparked research that laid the foundation for decades of scholarship, including analysis of discourse in language learning classrooms. Seminal work on coding teacher—and sometimes student—talk (e.g., Bellack, Kliebard, Hyman, & Smith, 1966; Fanselow, 1977; Flanders, 1970; Sinclair & Coulthard, 1975), revealed that classroom talk was systematic, and hence, could be categorized, quantified, and analyzed. According to Sinclair and Coulthard, there exists at least a tacit understanding amongst students and teachers concerning who speaks and when the talk occurs. They specify that this highly structured exchange consists of a teacherinitiationin the form of a question, resulting in a studentresponse, which is in turn followed by teacherfeedbackto the given response. The uncovering of the initiation-response-feedback (IRF) sequence has undergirded much of the research on classroom discourse that has since ensued, and has certainly influenced those working within the frameworks examined here. We will refer to this three-part dialogue asIRF, although it has been termed otherwise (e.g., Lemke, 1990; McHoul, 1978, 1990; Mehan, 1979). While Sinclair and Coulthard’s (1975) findings concerning the IRF sequence have informed subsequent research on classroom talk, the descriptive nature of their work assumed a detached, neutral tone towards classroom research. They neither condoned nor critiqued the pervasive IRF in educational settings, nor did they consider the effect of such talk on student participation or its effectiveness with regards to learning.

    Subsequent researchers have expanded on this work, often taking a stance concerning the advantages and drawbacks of the IRF in the classroom. Some go further, challenging the usefulness of this type of descriptive coding when examining classroom discourse and eschewing the imposition of pre-set categories on talk that might be highly contingent on the activities at hand. Instead, they examine talk as it unfolds in moment-by-moment interactions, considering how such talk encourages or constrains subsequent dialogue. It is with this point that we begin our review. The work examined here stems from three distinct theoretical views concerning how talk operates in the classroom and how teachers and learners co-construct meaning through verbal exchanges within the classroom walls. Indeed, these frameworks do not espouse identical views on the role of context in interactions. Nevertheless, they all do work from the understanding that classroom language use goes beyond the static, unilateral descriptive labeling of verbal exchanges. These approaches have served to inform much recent qualitative work on classroom discourse in the L2 and FL classrooms.

    LANGUAGE SOCIALIZATION (LS) AND SOCIOCULTURAL THEORY (SCT): UNDERSTANING DEVELOPMENT

    Language socialization (LS) and sociocultural theory (SCT) stem from separate intellectual roots—LS championed by the linguistic anthropologists Ochs and Schieffelin (1984; also Shieffelin & Ochs, 1986) and SCT grounded in the Vygotskyan tradition of Soviet psychology. Nevertheless, the two have much in common in their stance towards the learning process (Duff, 2007). Both emphasize the importance of social interaction, especially interaction over time between novices and those who are capable peers, caregivers or experts. In the first work on language socialization, Ochs and Schieffelin (1984) traced adult-child interactions across three cultures. Through a close examination of observed verbal exchanges between mothers and their young children, they noted that the ways in which mothers spoke to their pre-verbal infant children differed cross-culturally. These linguistic differences are reflective of contrasting cultural beliefs concerning the appropriate ways to address interlocutors, and the children’s ensuing language development seemed to signal both an acquiring of language and an understanding of these cultural views on speaking. The findings in their case studies (Schieffelin & Ochs, 1986) led to the argument that language learning is an interactive, sociocultural process between more and less competent community members, in which two important and distinct process occur: language is taught (children are socialized touselanguage), and culture is taught (children are socializedthroughlanguage). In sociocultural theory, the centrality of such interaction between more or less competent members is epitomized in classic concepts such as scaffolding (Wood, Burner, & Ross, 1976), mediation (Lantolf & Thorne, 2006), and the zone of proximal development (ZPD) (Vygotsky, 1978).

    Although neither LS nor SCT was initially concerned with L2 and FL studies, their influence on the latter is aptly captured in Patricia Duff’s (2007) plenary address, where she articulated the compatibility of the two approaches and their implications for what she calls Second Language Socialization (SLS) research. Scholars influenced by LS and SCT argue that L2 and FL learners, like their L1 counterparts, learn through social interaction with those who are more proficient in the target language. Echoing Shieffelin and Ochs (1986), these researchers argue that language learners are members of communities, and that their participation is crucial for language development. Through classic ethnographic methods such as participant observation, interviews, and—most importantly for the purpose of this paper—discourse analysis of naturally-occurring talk, they attempt to better understand how language learners are socialized into becoming competent members of such communities (cf. Kulik & Schieffelin, 2004, concerning ethnographic method).

    Some research has focused on the socializing force of teacher talk. Poole (1992), for example, recorded verbal interactions as a participant observer in beginning-level adult ESL classes in the United States. The teachers, themselves middle-class European-Americans, tended toward speaking to the students in ways akin to what Ochs and Schiefflin (1984) uncovered in their observations of white, middle-class American caregivers and their children. For instance, like mother with their young, teachers often used IRF sequences to guide novice L2 speakers through language production. As the discourse analysis demonstrated, all interactions in the classroom were carefully steered by the teacher. Ohta (1999) tracked one student’s progress in a beginning adult Japanese class. Like Poole (1992), the teacher utilized IRF to lead students through the language production process. In this setting, the IRF format practiced by the teacher proved to be quite constraining for the student, whose contributions were restricted to the “R” portion of the dialogue. As a result, the participant was often not able to practice ways of giving feedback during interactions. This “F” slot, used to express alignment or affect, is a crucial slot in Japanese dialogue. Therefore, the teacher served as a living—albeit perhaps inhibiting—model, making expressions salient in the teacher-fronted discourse. As the semester progressed, the student was able to increasingly incorporate expressions uttered by the teacher into peer group work. Kanagy (1999) analyzed teacher modeling in a Kindergarten Japanese immersion classroom in the United States. Like the teacher in Ohta’s study, the instructor in this classroom carefully demonstrated proper greetings through slow, deliberate enunciations, with exaggerated accompanying gestures. At first, the students imitated the teacher’s moves and language, and produced partially correct chunks of words. Gradually, the teacher removed the support structures, until the students could independently participate in the greeting. For both Ohta and Kanagy, the results lent evidence to the importance of expert-novice interactions for developing FL proficiency. Such expert-novice interaction is implemented through what Kahn (2012) calls an open-task framework characterized by emerging foci in an adult ESL classroom, where the teacher and the students jointly worked through exploratory problems that were not worked into the lesson plan, but rather emerged during class activities. According to Kahn, such unplanned moments allowed for the fostering of new understandings within the learners’ zone of proximal development, an argument that has yet to be corroborated.

    Others have sought to document the socialization of cultural values or behaviors in the second or foreign language. In her study on Chinese heritage language schools, He (2000) suggested that the interactional and grammatical organizations of teacher’s directives offered rich resources for socializing the children into Chinese cultural values. Duff’s (1995) work, conducted in an EFL history classroom in a Hungarian dual-language high school, the students were well socialized into a classroom format known asrecitation, in which a teacher-selected student must answer a series a display questions posed by the teacher. By contrast, they were novices in the student-centered, cooperative classrooms typical of Hungarian dual-language programs. When initially given the task of leading lectures, the students were dependent on formulaic expressions provided by the teacher to frame or conclude their talks. As the year progressed, students exhibited their adeptness at using language more creatively through asking for clarifications from the teacher, and providing their peers with feedback on improving student lectures. Duff concluded that the students were being socialized into becoming future EFL history teachers, performing as increasingly competent apprentices of this particular community.

    Beyond the emphasis on expert-novice interaction, much analytical attention has been devoted to the socializing process or mediating benefits stemming from collaborative peer interaction. Looking at a dual language classroom, Angelova, Gunawardena, and Volk (2008) argue that peers as well as teachers can serve as experts. Their study shows the ways in which a group of students switch between expert and novice roles, depending upon whether classroom work is conducted in Spanish or English. That is, the more competent Spanish speakers take on the role of expert (and peer teacher) in Spanish-language activities, while the native English speakers do the same for work that takes place in English. Antón and DiCamilla (1998) showed the ways in which the first language was used as a mediational tool as students, working in pairs, performed word searches, attempted to translate, and organized their ideas during an essay-writing task in a Spanish FL classroom. Also situated in the Spanish FL classroom, Gutiérrez (2008) reported instances of microgenesis, or moments that trace the origins of the learning process, during collaborative activities amongst peers. Ohta (2000) closely examined the talk of two college-level L2 speakers in a Japanese FL classroom as they co-constructed a zone of promiximal development that scaffolded their performance as they carried out a translation task. Based on data from a French immersion and an adult ESL classroom, Swain (2000) argues that collaborative dialog between peers is a key form of mediated learning, which in turn plays a crucial role in the language development.

    As some research has revealed, the language socialization process does not always progress neatly (Morita, 2004), and two studies from Hall (1995; 1998) demonstrated just that. Hall (1995) found that in a first-year Spanish class, the teacher attempted to engage students in verbal interactions. However, the talk had no overarching topic to lend it coherence. Instead, it was a string of talk in the IRF format, with the same information repeated and recycled. The highly constrained nature of the interactions precluded students from becoming active participants through elaboration or inquiry. In another study with the same Spanish instructor, Hall (1998) discovered that students did not have equal access to opportunities for linguistic interactions. In fact, it appeared that two status groups had formed, the “primary” group receiving considerably more interactional opportunities from the teacher than the “secondary” group. The primary group was able to successfully initiate turns, overlap other students? talk, and engage the teacher in discussing content. On the other hand, the teacher often either ignored initiations by secondary group members or critiqued their contributions based on linguistic form, blocking them from elaborating on content. Hall (1998) concluded that this differential treatment resulted in varied opportunities for participation in the community of learners, and as a consequence, different understandings amongst the students concerning their roles and rights as Spanish language learners. Morita (2004) tracked six adult English language learners during their year-long sojourns in a Canadian university. She found that socialization is indeed a dynamic process, involving constant and evolving negotiating of identities as students socially construct their roles as members of varying classroom communities. At times, the students struggled to find or use their voice, silenced by peers or instructors, while at other moments, their presence and opinions were valued.

    Willet’s (1995) year-long research in an American elementary-school classroom resonated with Hall’s (1998) conclusion that students experience the language socialization process differently, and hence, follow different trajectories with regard to L2 development. She focused on a group of L2 students as they completed phonics seat work, and she noticed a growing disparity in language development. Indeed, three of the four L2 learners had banded together, helping one another with tasks and experimenting with increasingly larger language chunks while conversing. Their active participation and linguistic independence increased during the year. By contrast, the fourth child’s participation decreased, with his sparse linguistic contributions signaling an ‘outsider’ status. In a study of L2 learners in kindergarten with regard to student participation, Toohey (1996) concluded that participation in the classroom community is not tied to language proficiency. In fact, not all students who exhibited a strong grasp on the target language became active participants in the classroom. Some students preferred to associate themselves with those who rejected classroom activities. In order to retain a high social status in one community, it was necessary to withdraw from another one (the classroom community). On this basis, Toohey effectively posited the argument that more than one community functions in classrooms, and that the competing goals of these communities can facilitate or hinder the socialization process. She also asserted that in forcing themselves to retreat to the margins of a community, students were activelyresistingsocialization. These arguments were corroborated in Duff’s (2002) study, which discovered that some mainstreamed ESL high school learners in Canada refused to provide more than terse one-word answers to questions, even if they were capable of doing so.

    The finding that some students either cannot or will not engage in active participation in a community interacting in the target language raises some challenges for a framework that assumes complete socialization as an endpoint. Even though researchers working within language socialization recognize that the process can be iterative, they have not traditionally considered factors that may temporarily or permanently disrupt the process. At the same time, however, scholars working within critical discourse analysis consider this kind of question(s) to be a launching pad for research in the L2 and FL classroom.

    CRITICAL DISCOURSE ANALYSIS (CDA): UNDERSTANDING POWER

    While all three frameworks examined here operate with the argument that language is socially co-constructed through interaction, Critical Discourse Analysis (CDA) alone insists that language is inextricably intertwined with ideologies and notions of power embedded in the dominant cultures that use the language (Kumaravadivelu, 1999). CDA further claims that interactions are shaped by external sociopolitical conditions. Thus, language use does not begin with spoken or written text, but with social and political issues (Fairclough, 2001). A critical analyst approaches the data with the intent to uncover larger sociopolitical issues operating within the culture. Fairclough, Mulderrig, and Wodak (2011) argue that CDA does not adhere to one particular research method. Instead, these researchers begin with a topic; methodology is the analytical mode by which the topic is refined, and the research questions emerge, which depends upon the subject under investigation. Earlier work discussed the use ofcriticalethnography(Simon & Dippo, 1986), which includes discourse analysis, participant observation, and interviews that one would expect of a classic ethnography. Yet, critical ethnographers share the expressed goal of discovering and examining the ideological discourse that is assumed to exist in the language.

    The arguments outlined by CDA scholars have challenged many L2 and FL classroom researchers to consider ways in which power relations and cultural ideologies function in educational institutions. Popular target languages such as French, English, and Spanish have been linked to societies that have histories of colonization, racial aggressions, as well as socioeconomic and gender inequalities. Language learners are often members of formerly colonized societies and/or identify as members of populations that have been historically minoritized, whichaprioricreates a power dynamic within the classroom walls. Not only does the teacher have the authority to ask questions and evaluate student performance, but the instructor also may represent the cultural values and political stances of the communities that speak the target language as an L1. Therefore, these scholars have found it important to ask research questions concerning the ways in which cultural, racial, and gender ideologies embedded in such languages affect language development in L2 and FL learners. They are also interested in the relationship between language learning and social change (Norton & Toohey, 2004, p.1).

    Like some of the researchers working in language socialization frameworks, scholars in critical frameworks have addressed issues of learner exclusion or unequal learner access to the socialization process. Critical researchers go one step further, however, as to link notions of inequality and exclusion to larger sociopolitical forces. Gutiérrez and Larson (1994), for example, discussed the IRF structure in terms ofhegemony, or the power exerted by a dominant group over marginalized groups (Gramsci, 1971). In their study of a 10th grade ESL class, the two researchers argue that the teacher’s strict adherence to the IRF confined students to giving short answers only. When students attempted to elaborate on their answers, the teacher would interrupt or latch onto the response; at times, she simply ignored them. This lack of interactional space for students to develop their own text or inject their own content knowledge into the lesson resulted in students having little opportunities to develop a range of language skills and sociocultural knowledge necessary for entry into the academic community (cf. Hall, 1995). Based on such findings, Gutiérrez and Larson then attempted to connect the IRF structure to larger notions of power in the classroom. They contend that the teacher’s adherence to the IRF pattern allowed her to assume the position of the sole possessor of knowledge. As such, student voices were marginalized in the presence of the teacher-dominated discourse. These findings are repeated and extended in Gutiérrez, Larson, and Kreuter (1995), which also revealed how the use of IRF imposed interactional constraints on students. When one student continually resisted these rules through speaking out of turn or ignoring directions, the teacher viewed the student as not only disruptive, but also academically deficient. Although the student’s verbal expressions demonstrated her growing literacy skills, the teacher only focused on her disregard for classroom procedure, and labeled her “remedial.”

    While the teacher’s role as a member of the dominant culture was emphasized in both Gutiérrez and Larson’s (1994) and Gutiérrez, Larson, and Kreuter’s (1995) analyses of the discourse of classrooms, Canagarajah (1993) found that student resistance in an FL setting may produce differential results. More specifically, it was observed that an EFL classroom in Sri Lanka in which the entire class resisted the teacher-imposed interactional patterns. The teacher gave language instruction in the benefits of a student-centered, discussion-based classroom; unfortunately, he failed to engage his students, who were used to teacher-centered, grammar-based classes. Because of the local cultural view that teachers must impart their knowledge to students, the students would not accept an atmosphere in which knowledge was shared and students interacted with one another. As a result, many students stopped attending the class and enrolled in private tutoring classes instead in order to develop the L2 and FL skills necessary for the next course in the sequence. This way, they effectively marginalized the teacher.

    On a different note, some researchers have discovered opportunities within the classroom walls through which students can develop as language learners outside of the sanctioned student-teacher or student-student exchanges. Canagarajah (2004) analyzed moments in which L2 students were able to communicate, usually surreptitiously, outside the strict IRF format imposed by the teacher. This kind of exchange, called asafehouseby Canagarajah, usually occurred in a secondary ESL classroom as off-task or between-task talk, or during whole-class interactions. In these moments, students experimented with vocabulary and syntax, and would at times code-switch in order to further clarify a point. Through a close analysis of the language used in these safe houses, Canagarajah convincingly identified a rich, multilingual site where students are exposed to important communication strategies such as style-switching, which involves having the interlocutor switch between formal and more informal registers. On this basis, he argued that more research needs to be conducted in such spaces.

    Another prominent trend for researchers informed by CDA has been to closely analyze classroom discourse for the purpose of ascertaining how identities are negotiated. While identity negotiation has long been a point of interest for researchers from paradigms including the second and foreign language classrooms, it is somewhat surprising how few studies offer the close analysis of talk demonstrating how identity is actually constructed in the moment-by-moment of classroom interactions. Hruska (2004), in her year-long study of kindergarteners, examined ways in which linguistic realizations of gendered identities facilitated or hindered L2 learners’ classroom participation. In line with findings from language socialization research, she noticed that L2 proficiency played less of a role in determining access to interactions than identity constructions in unfolding talk (see also Hruska, 2007; Norton, 2001). For instance, boys tended to engage in what Hruska referred to ascompetitivediscourse, debating who had “won” or who was “bigger.” In contrast, girls often opted forromanticdiscourse, discussing issues such as “which classmates would marry each other.” The demarcations along gender lines constrained the types of interactions available for the class. Furthermore, when the teacher commenced a class-wide discussion on the youth soccer league—an activity that the students had verbally proclaimed to be “masculine,” girls retracted from speaking, despite the teacher’s prodding. Indeed, gender seemed to govern who could speak, and what kind of talk could ensue. As stressed by Hruska, these notions of gender did not direct the discourse in the form of overarching stereotypes, but rather, occurred at certain moments; they were also constantly in flux as children negotiated them in specific interactions. She concluded that moments in which gender ideologies emerged in the discourse evoked served to block certain students from their linguistic expression, and barred certain L2 learners from participating in the talk.

    Theorists have noted that terms such as “husband/wife,” “married/divorced” are reflective of heterosexuality being the dominant discourse on sexual identity. Nelson (1999), among other scholars, has argued that L2 researchers need to be more sensitive to alternate sexual identities, and that it is important to provide opportunities for language learners to use expressive language to talk about it. In Nelson’s study of a community-college ESL grammar course, the teacher presented a graphic of two women walking arm-in-arm during a lesson on modal verbs. This was followed by a content-based discussion of cultural assumptions about homosexuality. Though it remained unclear as to how this verbal exchange of sexual identity played a role in hindering or facilitating L2 development and learner participation in linguistic interactions, the importance of having L2 learners discuss cultural assumptions surrounding sexual identity was affirmed.

    All of the researchers reviewed in this section analyzed discourse with the expressed purpose of connecting the use of language to larger sociopolitical structures that operate in the target language culture. They located interactional moments when the language serves to dominate, resist, or negotiate the cultural ideologies in the community. The last framework to be reviewed below, conversation analysis (CA), converges in the sense that it also engages in close analysis of verbal interaction. Nevertheless, this approach favors working directly from the data, rather than from macrostructures such as race or gender, to form the research questions.

    CONVERSATION ANALYSIS (CA): UNDERSTANDING ACTION

    Founded by American sociologists in the 1960s and 1970s (Sacks, Schegloff, & Jefferson, 1974), CA as both a theory and a methodology comprises a set of assumptions that prioritize analytic induction and participant orientations: (1) social interaction is orderly at all points, such that no detail can be dismissedapriori; (2) participants orient to that order themselves, (i.e., order is not a result of the analyst’s conceptions or any pre-formulated theoretical categories); and (3) such order can be discovered and described by examining the details of interaction. The goal of CA is to uncover the methods and procedures participants utilize to understand each other and to be understood. Analysts work with audio or video recordings along with the transcripts of these recordings, which capture a full range of interactional details such as volume, pitch, pace, intonation, overlap, breaths, smiley voice, the length of silence as well as nonverbal conduct. It is in these minute details that evidence is located for how social actions such as requesting or criticizing are accomplished. The basic unit of interaction, for example, is an adjacency pair (APs) (Schegloff, 2007), where the first pair calls for the production of a second pair part. Through adjacency pairs, a wide range of actions may be accomplished: question-answer, invitation-acceptance, accusation-denial, complaint-apology, etc. When a question-answer adjacency pair is followed by a sequence-closing third such as “okay,” “good” or “thanks,” it begins to look, at least ostensibly, very much like the IRF structure in classroom discourse. The highly detailed nature of CA analyses has appealed to L2 and FL classroom researchers who wish to better understand the immediate interactional accomplishments or consequences of talk.

    Like the other frameworks examined in this paper, researchers working in the CA tradition have taken interest in the IRF structure. A specific focus has been placed on the local effects of the third turn. Seedhouse (2004) demonstrated that IRF cycles “...perform different interactional and pedagogical work according to the context in which they are operating” (p. 63). He supported his argument by juxtaposing two L2 classroom extracts: one text contained strings in which the ‘F’ slot displayed a teacher’s preoccupation with evaluating the grammar of a student; another showed a teacher handling grammar correction incidentally, while attending to the content of the student response, which triggered further teacher-student interaction. Seedhouse then concluded that the content of the third part can either narrow or widen the interactional space allotted to the learner.

    Waring (2008) uncovered one specific way in which the third turn can close down interaction. Her analysis of homework checks conducted in an adult ESL classroom showed that phrases such as “very good” in the third turn, termedexplicitpositiveassessments(EPA), are treated as a signal that the sequence had shut down; when students did have questions related to the answer that received the EPA, they did not pose the questions until long after the exchange related to the homework item had passed. Thus, positive assessments can actually hinder student participation through blocking a response from further elaboration or questioning by other students (see also Seedhouse, 1999).

    Rather than declaring the third turn a blanket “evaluation” or “feedback” move, Lee (2007) argues that attention must be paid to its local contingencies. In his analysis of college-level ESL classrooms, Lee uncovers five different functions for the third turn, each highly dependent upon the immediate context created by the second turn. For instance, a teacher may use the third turn to steer students into a particular interactional trajectory, leading them to evaluate the grammatical (in)correctness of a peer response; or a teacher may attend to classroom management issues, such as soliciting answers from the class after a second turn silence, or asking a student to repeat an answer that went unheard. Also focusing on the third turn, Fagan (2012a) demonstrates the multifunctionality of “okay” as a teacher resource for giving feedback.

    Aside from the F in IRF, conversation analytic interests have also been directed toward teacher questions. During what Markee (2004) callszonesofinteractionaltransition(ZIT), when a student summons a teacher to answer a question that a group of students has posed during peer work, the teacher often responds by inserting a counter-question, thereby regaining the initiation slot, and repositioning the student as the one whose answer is a candidate for evaluation or feedback. Teacher questions can also carry complex ramifications. Yes-no questions can convey a critical stance that does not encourage open and critical thinking (Waring, 2012a), and understanding-check questions launched at activity boundaries are often treated as a pre-closing rather than an invitation for questions (Waring, 2012b).

    Some recent conversation analytic work has looked into aspects of teacher talk beyond questions and feedback. Fagan (2012b) examines how a novice teacher deals with unexpected learner contributions by glossing over learner contributions and assuming the role of information provider. In a follow-up larger scale study, Fagan (2013) details how an expert teacher manages learner contributions by consistently prioritizing learner competence and positioning learners as partners in a joint inquiry that underscores exploration. Jacknick (2013) specifies the teacher’s use of laugher in managing moments where her epistemic authority becomes questionable. Waring (2013b) documents how the teachers in the adult ESL classroom manage competing voices through eitherselectiveattending(directing attention to one particular student voice that has in some way risen above the chorus of voices) orsequentialattending(directing attention to multiple voices in a serial manner).

    Aside from the focus on teacher talk, conversation analytic work has generated important insights into the active roles learners take in steering classroom interaction, such as moving out of the IRF (Waring, 2009), contesting activity shifts initiated by the teacher (Jacknick, 2011), challenging the teacher’s epistemic authority (Jacknick, 2013), and doing being playful (Waring, 2013). Waring (2011), for example, produces a typology of learner initiatives both within and outside the IRF and discusses the learning opportunities such initiatives may engender.

    Work on learner talk has also described how gestures are used to aid repair and to complete an action in small group project work in the ESL classroom (Olsher, 2005, 2008) and howinteractionalshiftsare accomplished as students begin and end tasks (Hellermann, 2007, 2008; Hellermann & Cole, 2008). Markee (2005) untangled an exchange during a university-level ESL class, in which the social practice of inviting a friend to a party co-existed with the teacher-assigned task of discussing Günter Grass. Between the teacher giving directions and the start of the task, an interactional space unrelated to the task opened up for talk. At that moment, two students began what Markee called the “skillful schizophrenia” (p. 210), assuming multiple identities of inviter/invitee and students. Although the off-task exchange was subject to frequent interruption, the students used eye gaze and hand motions to signal alignment in finishing the business of the invitational act. One of the participants was able to answer a teacher question before turning back to the invitation sequence, deftly attending to his dual role as a student and an inviter. By locating moments in which students balance several identities and manage two unfolding sequences, Markee argues that students display their growing L2 competence both during and at the boundaries of tasks. As a matter of fact, these two moments did occur simultaneously in the study. Overall, work on learner interaction has found that L2 learners of all proficiency levels engage in meaningful talk when given the opportunity to work with one another. In some cases, such interactional space allows students to aid one another in comprehending vocabulary words, or assist one another in repairing errors (Markee, 2000). However, as noted by Markee (2000) and Mori (2002), the teacher’s idea about whatshouldbe going on is not always what is going on. Students may be attempting to grasp the meaning of a lexical item in context, for instance, or students may be struggling with managing an interaction. On the other hand, students may achieve a naturally flowing conversation in their FL during the planning phase, when the teacher might not be expecting it. Finally, without any specific focus on either teacher or learner talk, Waring (2013c) shows how learners develop their competence in mastering the sociopragmatic aspect (i.e. the social expectations of how speakers in a community should communicate) and the pragmalinguistic aspect (i.e. the resources available to the speaker in the target language to convey a socially appropriate message) of certain interactional routines in English over a nine-week span. Taken together, the CA framework aids researchers in finding out exactly when and how these fruitful talks occur, thereby shedding light on ways to engage students in meaningful exchanges.

    CONCLUSION

    This brief review of frameworks informing classroom discourse analysis demonstrates that talk in the classroom has proven to be highly structured, and as such, can be described and explored in various ways. For a scholar working in language socialization or social cultural theory, for instance, the IRF format may afford teachers an opportunity to guide students through the process of using the target language in increasingly sophisticated ways. For a critical discourse analyst, the rigidity of the IRF may contribute to issues of teacher dominance in the classroom, resulting in the teacher’s control over the issue of who holds the floor. For a conversation analyst, each turn of the IRF sequence can be examined to shed light on how a turn-at-talk might play its role in constructing the next spate of interaction. All of these approaches allow researchers to understand the intricacies of how talk functions within the classroom walls. The recent resistance to unilaterally code an utterance as “initiation,” for example, or “feedback,” has opened the possibility to consider the multi-faceted nature of classroom talk. A teacher question, for example, can serve to gather information—or as a managerial strategy or a method to spark class debate. Thus, studies that privilege the contingency of both teacher and student talk may result in a more nuanced understanding of classroom talk, and may provide a more detailed analysis of both the advantages and drawbacks of certain kinds of classroom discourse.

    Research stemming from the three frameworks examined has also looked into the kinds of interactions that are not part of canonical classroom talk. Marginalized student-student exchanges, including off-task talk and unsanctioned interactions, have been shown to create spaces for students to experiment freely with the target language. These studies, while still small in number, contribute to a more complete understanding of how language is used in the classroom. Studies that seek to collect data of this kind of talk would help researchers and teachers alike to better understand what students grapple with or comment on when they believe the teacher is not listening. In addition, spontaneous teacher-student talk revealing emerging foci, which are not a part of a preset lesson plan, merits further research, as such studies could shed light on the kinds of language points that spark student questions, struggles, or debates. Through a continued, close exploration of both teacher-student and perhaps student-student interactions in particular, researchers can gain greater insight into the exact ways in which classroom discourse encourages or hinders student participation and language development in the L2 and FL classroom.

    Future research can also harvest great gains from some cross-fertilization between various approaches, The strength of CA in illuminating interactional practices, for example, when combined with SCT’s powerful lens for explaining teaching and learning, can yield findings that are both empirically defensible and theoretically illuminating. In addition, researchers and practitioners alike are frequently concerned about the learning “outcome” of specific teacher practices, and such outcomes are typically not obtainable from the analysis of discourse alone. Questions such as whether a particular teacher practice is more effective than some other practice are readily answerable with experimental designs. As such, collaboration with colleagues outside discourse analysis, though easier said than done, can be immensely productive.

    With greater insights into the nature of classroom discourse comes the possibility of linking theory to practice. Indeed, educating teachers about classroom language use might allow them to become more aware as to how students and teachers interact, and how such exchanges promote or hinder the possibility of the meaningful participation that purportedly leads to student learning. In particular, research on how experienced teachers handle the moment-by-moment contingencies of classroom talk would be of great use to teacher-educators and novice teachers alike. We envision teacher education programs that include analyzing actual classroom talk, in order for teachers-in-training to identify and maximize opportunities for their students to engage in the activities that are believed to lead to language development.

    REFERENCES

    Adger, C. T. (2001). Discourse in educational settings. In D. Schiffrin, D. Tannen, & H. Hamilton (Eds.),Thehandbookofdiscourseanalysis(pp. 503-517). Malden: Blackwell.

    Angelova, M., Gunawardena, D., & Volk, D. (2008). Peer teaching and learning: Co-constructing language in a dual-language first grade.LanguageandEducation, 20, 173-190.

    Antón. M. & DiCamila, F. (1998). Socio-cognitive functions of L1 collaborative interaction in the L2 classroom.CanadianModernLanguageReview, 54, 314-342.

    Bellack, A.A., Kliebard, H.M., Hyman, R.T., & Smith, F.L., Jr. (1966).Thelanguageoftheclassroom. New York: Teachers College Press.

    Canagarajah, A.S. (1993). Critical ethnography of a sri lankan classroom: Ambiguities in student opposition to reproduction through ESOL.TESOLQuarterly, 27, 601-626.

    Canagarajah, S. (2004). Subversive identities, pedagogical safehouses, and critical learning. In B. Norton & K. Toohey (Eds.),Criticalpedagogiesandlanguagelearning(pp. 116-137). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Drew, P. & Heritage, J. (Eds.) (1992).Talkatwork:Interactionininstitutionalsettings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Duff, P. A. (1995). An ethnography of communication in immersion classrooms in Hungary.TESOLQuarterly, 29, 505-537.

    Duff, P. A. (2002). The discursive construction of knowledge, identity, and difference: An ethnography of communication in the high school mainstream.AppliedLinguistics, 23, 289-322.

    Duff, P. A. (2007). Second language socialization as sociocultural theory: Insights and issues.LanguageTeaching, 40, 309-219.

    Fagan, D.S. (2012a). ‘Okay’ as a multifunctional resource for giving feedback in classrooms.Language&InformationSociety, 16, 9-41.

    Fagan, D.S. (2012b). ‘Dealing with’ unexpected learner contributions in whole-group activities: An examination of novice language teacher discursive practices.ClassroomDiscourse, 3, 107-128.

    Fagan, D.S. (2013).ManaginglearnercontributionsintheadultESLclassroom:Aconversationanalyticandethnographicexaminationofteacherpracticesandcognition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York.

    Fairclough, N. (2001). The discourse of new labor: Critical discourse analysis. In M. Wetherall, S. Taylor, & S. J. Yates (Eds.),Discourseasdata:Aguideforanalysis(pp. 229-266). London: Sage.

    Fairclough, N., Mulderrig, J., & Wodak, R. (2011). Critical discourse analysis. In T.van Dijk (Ed.),Discourseanalysis:Amultidisciplinaryapproach(pp. 357-375). London: Sage.

    Fanselow, J. F. (1977). Beyond Roshomon: Conceptualizing and describing the teaching act.TESOLQuarterly, 11, 17-39.

    Flanders, N. A. (1970).Analyzingteachingbehavior. Reading: Addison-Wesley.

    Gramsci, A. (1971).Selectionsfromtheprisonnotebooks,editedandtranslatedbyQuintonHoareandGeoffreyNowellSmith. New York: International.

    Gutiérrez, A.G. (2008). Microgenesis, method and object: A study of collaborative activity in a Spanish as a foreign language classroom.AppliedLinguistics, 29,120-148.

    Gutiérrez, K.D. & Larson, J. (1994). Language borders: Recitation as hegemonic discourse.InternationalJournalofEducationalReform, 3, 22-36.

    Gutiérrez, K.D., Larson, J., & Kreuter, B. (1995). Cultural tensions in the scripted classroom: The value of the subjugated perspective.UrbanEducation, 29, 410-442.

    Gutiérrez, K. D., Rymes, B., & Larson, J. (1995). Script, counterscript, and underlife in the classroom: James Brown versus Brown vs. Board of Education.HarvardEducationalReview, 65, 445-471.

    Hall, J. K. (1995). “Aw, man, where you goin??” Classroom interaction and the development of L2 interactional competence.IssuesinAppliedLinguistics, 6, 37-62.

    Hall, J. K. (1998). Differential teacher attention to student utterances: The construction of different opportunities for learning in the IRF.LinguisticsandEducation, 9, 287-311.

    Hall, J. K. & Walsh, M. (2002). Teacher-student interaction and learning.AnnualReviewofAppliedLinguistics, 22, 186-203.

    He, A. W. (2000). The grammatical and interactional organization of teacher’s directives: Implications for socialization of Chinese American children.LinguisticsandEducation, 11, 119-140.

    Hellermann, J. (2007). The development of practices for action in classroom dyadic interaction: Focus on task openings.TheModernLanguageJournal, 91, 83-96.

    Hellermann, J. (2008).Socialactionsforclassroomlanguagelearning. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

    Hellermann, J. & Cole, E. (2008). Practices for social interaction in the language-learning classroom: Disengagements from dyadic task interaction.AppliedLinguistics, 30, 186-215.

    Hruska, B. L. (2004). Constructing gender in an English dominant kindergarten: Implications for Second Language learners.TESOLQuarterly, 38, 459-485.

    Hruska, B. L. (2007). “She My Friend”: Implications of friend ideologies, identities, and relationships for bilingual kindergarteners.MulticulturalPerspectives, 9, 3-12.

    Jacknick, C. (2011). Breaking in is hard to do: Negotiating classroom activity shifts.ClassroomDiscourse, 2, 20-38.

    Jacknick, C. (2013). “’Cause the textbook says...”: Laughter and student challenges in the ESL classroom. In P. Glenn & L. Holt (Eds.),Onlaughing:Studiesoflaughterininteraction. London: Continuum.

    Kahn, G. (2012). Open-ended tasks and the qualitative investigation of second language classroom discourse.JournalofEthnographicandQualitativeResearch, 6, 90-107.

    Kanagy, R. (1999). Interactional routines as a mechanism for L2 acquisition and socialization in an immersion context.JournalofPragmatics, 31, 1467-1492.

    Kulik, D. & Shieffelin, B. B. (2004). Language socialization. In A. Duranti (Ed.),Acompaniontolinguisticanthropology. (pp. 349-368). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Kumaravadivelu, B. (1999). Critical classroom discourse analysis.TESOLQuarterly, 33, 453-484.

    Lantolf, J. P. & Thorne, S. L. (2006).Socioculturaltheoryandthegenesisofsecondlanguagedevelopment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Lave, J. & Wenger, E. (1991).Situatedlearning:Legitimateperipheralparticipation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Lee, Y. (2007). Third turn position in teacher talk: Contingency and the work of teaching.JournalofPragmatics, 39, 180-206.

    Lemke, J. L. (1990).Talkingscience:Language,learning,andvalues. New York: Ablex.

    Markee, N. (2000).Conversationanalysis. New York: Routledge.

    Markee, N. (2004). Zones of interactional transition in ESL classes.TheModernLanguageJournal, 88, 583-596.

    Markee, N. (2005). The organization of off-task talk in second language classrooms. In K. Richards & P. Seedhouse (Eds.),Applyingconversationanalysis(pp. 197-213). New York: Palgrave.

    Markee, N. & Kasper, G. (2004). Classroom talks: An introduction.ModernLanguageJournal, 88, 491-500.

    McHoul, A. W. (1978). The organization of turns at formal talk in the classroom.LanguageinSociety, 7, 183-213.

    McHoul, A. W. (1990). The organization of repair in classroom talk.LanguageinSociety, 19, 349-377.

    Mehan, H. (1979a).Leaninglessons:Socialorganizationintheclassroom. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Mehan, H. (1979b). “What time is it, Denise?”: Asking known information questions in classroom discourse.TheoryintoPractice, 28, 285-294.

    Mori, J. (2002). Task design: Plan, and development of talk-in-interaction: An analysis of a small group activity in a Japanese language classroom.AppliedLinguistics, 23, 323-347.

    Morita, N. (2004). Negotiating participation and identity in second language academic communities.TESOLQuarterly, 38, 573-603.

    Nassaji, H. & Wells, G. (2000). What’s the use of the “triadic dialogue?”: An investigation of teacher-student interaction.AppliedLinguistics, 21, 376-406.

    Nelson, C. (1999). Sexual Identities in ESL: Queer theory and classroom inquiry.TESOLQuarterly, 33, 371-391.

    Norton, B. & Toohey, K. (Eds.). (2004).Criticalpedagogiesandlanguagelearning. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Norton, B. (2001). Non-participation, imagined communities and the language classrooms. In M. P. Breen (Ed.),Learnercontributionstolanguagelearning(pp. 159-171). London: Longman.

    Nystrand, M. (1997).Openingdialogue:UnderstandingthedynamicsoflanguageandlearningintheEnglishclassroom. New York: Teachers College Press.

    Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (1984). Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories and their implications. In R. Schweder & R. LeVine (Eds.),Culturetheory:Essaysonmind,selfandemotion(pp. 276-320). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Ohta, A.S. (1999). Interactional routines and the socialization of interactional style in adult learners of Japanese.JournalofPragmatics, 31, 1493-1512.

    Ohta, A. S. (2000). Rethinking interaction in SLA: Developmentally appropriate assistance in the zone of proximal development and the acquisition of L2 grammar. In James P. Lantolf (Ed.),Socioculturaltheoryandsecondlanguagelearning(pp. 51-78). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Olsher, D. (2005). Talk and gesture: The embodied completions of sequential actions in spoken interaction. In R. Gardner & J. Wagner (Eds.),Secondlanguageconversations(pp. 221-245). London: Continuum.

    Olsher, D. (2008). Gesturally-enhanced repeats in the repair turn: Communication strategy or cognitive language-learning tool? In S. G. McCafferty & G. Stam (Eds.),Gesture:Secondlanguageacquisitionandclassroomresearch(pp. 109-130). New York & London: Routledge.

    Poole, D. (1992). Language socialization in the second language classroom.LanguageLearning, 42, 593-616.

    Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation.Language, 50, 696-735.

    Schegloff, E. A. (2007).Sequenceorganizationininteraction:Aprimerinconversationanalysis,Volume1. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Schieffelin, B. B. & Ochs, E. (1986). Language socialization.AnnualReviewofAnthropology, 15, 163-191.

    Seedhouse, P. (1999). Task-based interaction.ELTJournal, 53, 149-156.

    Seedhouse, P. (2004).Theinteractionalarchitectureofthelanguageclassroom:Aconversationanalysisperspective. Malden: Blackwell.

    Simon, R. I. & Dippo, D. (1986). On critical ethnography work.AnthropologyandEducationQuarterly, 17, 195-202.

    Sinclair, J. M. & Coulthard, R. M. (1975).Towardsananalysisofdiscourse:TheEnglishusedbyteachersandpupils. London: Oxford University Press.

    Swain, M. (2000). The output hypothesis and beyond: Mediating acquisition through collaborative dialogue. In James P. Lantolf (Ed.),Socioculturaltheoryandsecondlanguageearning(pp. 97-114). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Thornborrow, J. (2002).Powertalk:Languageandinteractionininstitutionaldiscourse. London: Virago.

    Toohey, K. (1996). Learning English as a second language in kindergarten: A community of practice perspective.TheCanadianModernLanguageReview, 52, 549-576.

    Vygotsky, L. S. (1978).Mindinsociety:Thedevelopmentofhigherpsychologicalprocesses. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Walsh, S. (2002). Construction or obstruction: Teacher talk and learner involvement in the EFL classroom.LanguageTeachingResearch, 6, 3-23.

    Walsh, S. (2006).Investigatingclassroomdiscourse. New York: Routledge.

    Waring, H. Z. (2008). Using explicit positive assessment in the language classroom: IRF, feedback, and learning opportunities.TheModernLanguageJournal, 92, 577-594.

    Waring, H. Z. (2009). Moving out of IRF (initiation-response-feedback): A single case analysis.LanguageLearning, 59, 796-824.

    Waring, H. Z. (2011). Learner initiatives and learning opportunities.ClassroomDiscourse, 2, 201-218.

    Waring, H. Z. (2012a). Yes-no questions that convey a critical stance in the language classroom.LanguageandEducation, 26, 451-469.

    Waring, H. Z. (2012b). “Any questions?:” Investigating understanding-checks in the language classroom.TESOLQuarterly, 46, 722-752.

    Waring, H. Z. (2013a). Doing being playful in the language classroom.AppliedLinguistics, 34, 191-210.

    Waring, H. Z. (2013b). Managing the competing voices in the language classroom.DiscourseProcesses, 50, 316-338.

    Waring, H. Z. (2013c). “How was your weekend?”: Developing the interactional competence in managing routine inquiries.LanguageAwareness, 22, 1-16.

    Wood, D., Bruner, J., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem-solving.JournalofChildPsychologyandChildPsychiatry, 17, 89-100.

    Willet, J. (1995). Becoming first graders in an L2: An ethnographic study of L2 socialization.TESOLQuarterly, 29, 473-503.

    Zuengler, J. & Mori, J. (2002). Microanalyses of classroom discourse: A critical consideration of methods.AppliedLinguistics, 23, 283-288.

    国产99白浆流出| 在线观看午夜福利视频| 国产亚洲欧美精品永久| 久久久久久久久中文| 色哟哟哟哟哟哟| 日韩欧美一区视频在线观看| 日韩av在线大香蕉| 精品久久久久久成人av| 久久精品亚洲熟妇少妇任你| 国产成人系列免费观看| 极品教师在线免费播放| 在线av久久热| 欧美精品啪啪一区二区三区| 国产精品香港三级国产av潘金莲| 日韩av在线大香蕉| 亚洲第一电影网av| 欧美国产精品va在线观看不卡| 久久久久亚洲av毛片大全| 亚洲精品美女久久av网站| 国产精华一区二区三区| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看 | 久久人妻av系列| 亚洲一区中文字幕在线| 成人三级黄色视频| 日日干狠狠操夜夜爽| 亚洲免费av在线视频| 亚洲中文日韩欧美视频| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av在线| 啦啦啦 在线观看视频| 制服诱惑二区| 久久 成人 亚洲| 免费在线观看影片大全网站| 亚洲男人天堂网一区| 最近最新免费中文字幕在线| 最新美女视频免费是黄的| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 国产在线观看jvid| 色播亚洲综合网| 精品午夜福利视频在线观看一区| 九色亚洲精品在线播放| 午夜免费观看网址| 国产色视频综合| 91大片在线观看| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 国产精品香港三级国产av潘金莲| 在线观看午夜福利视频| 级片在线观看| 999精品在线视频| 99热只有精品国产| 久9热在线精品视频| 国产在线精品亚洲第一网站| 老司机靠b影院| www.999成人在线观看| tocl精华| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 老熟妇仑乱视频hdxx| 亚洲精品美女久久久久99蜜臀| 国产色视频综合| 黄片播放在线免费| 日韩免费av在线播放| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 波多野结衣一区麻豆| 亚洲在线自拍视频| 999精品在线视频| 国产欧美日韩精品亚洲av| 欧美成狂野欧美在线观看| 人人澡人人妻人| 久久精品91蜜桃| 亚洲男人天堂网一区| 午夜精品在线福利| 成人精品一区二区免费| 色在线成人网| 一夜夜www| 国产精品久久视频播放| www.熟女人妻精品国产| 亚洲av成人一区二区三| 天天躁夜夜躁狠狠躁躁| 丝袜美腿诱惑在线| 国产成人啪精品午夜网站| 老熟妇乱子伦视频在线观看| 精品乱码久久久久久99久播| 欧美日韩福利视频一区二区| 韩国av一区二区三区四区| 久久久国产欧美日韩av| 亚洲精华国产精华精| 人妻久久中文字幕网| 久久久久国产精品人妻aⅴ院| 搡老岳熟女国产| а√天堂www在线а√下载| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 九色亚洲精品在线播放| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| 精品熟女少妇八av免费久了| 中文字幕精品免费在线观看视频| 国产精品久久久人人做人人爽| 麻豆av在线久日| 国产高清videossex| 在线观看免费视频日本深夜| 在线免费观看的www视频| 国产色视频综合| 99精品在免费线老司机午夜| 村上凉子中文字幕在线| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 天堂影院成人在线观看| 久久久久久免费高清国产稀缺| 天堂√8在线中文| 国产成人av教育| 韩国av一区二区三区四区| 在线视频色国产色| 啪啪无遮挡十八禁网站| 黄色a级毛片大全视频| 老司机靠b影院| 色综合站精品国产| 国产亚洲欧美精品永久| 在线观看免费视频日本深夜| 亚洲狠狠婷婷综合久久图片| 欧洲精品卡2卡3卡4卡5卡区| 18禁观看日本| 亚洲黑人精品在线| 免费在线观看日本一区| 91字幕亚洲| 99国产综合亚洲精品| 老司机午夜福利在线观看视频| 老熟妇乱子伦视频在线观看| 亚洲一区二区三区色噜噜| 国产黄a三级三级三级人| 免费在线观看亚洲国产| 国产乱人伦免费视频| 久久精品国产清高在天天线| 黄色片一级片一级黄色片| 韩国av一区二区三区四区| 欧美在线黄色| 男人操女人黄网站| 又黄又粗又硬又大视频| 岛国视频午夜一区免费看| 午夜精品久久久久久毛片777| 一级毛片高清免费大全| 午夜a级毛片| 一区二区三区精品91| 国产成人免费无遮挡视频| 免费看a级黄色片| 狠狠狠狠99中文字幕| 国产成人啪精品午夜网站| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区| 亚洲欧美精品综合久久99| 久热爱精品视频在线9| 中文字幕人妻丝袜一区二区| 成年女人毛片免费观看观看9| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 深夜精品福利| 久久精品国产99精品国产亚洲性色 | 纯流量卡能插随身wifi吗| 丝袜美腿诱惑在线| 国产片内射在线| 久久婷婷成人综合色麻豆| 黄色视频,在线免费观看| 成熟少妇高潮喷水视频| 日韩免费av在线播放| 黄频高清免费视频| 成人18禁在线播放| 免费看a级黄色片| 母亲3免费完整高清在线观看| 9色porny在线观看| 免费少妇av软件| 1024视频免费在线观看| 黑人操中国人逼视频| 久久狼人影院| 久久这里只有精品19| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 久久久久久久午夜电影| 色综合欧美亚洲国产小说| 在线观看66精品国产| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| √禁漫天堂资源中文www| 色哟哟哟哟哟哟| 免费看美女性在线毛片视频| 黄片播放在线免费| 一a级毛片在线观看| 久久性视频一级片| 女性被躁到高潮视频| 免费在线观看完整版高清| 18禁黄网站禁片午夜丰满| 色老头精品视频在线观看| 日本精品一区二区三区蜜桃| 美女扒开内裤让男人捅视频| 91字幕亚洲| 日韩av在线大香蕉| 99久久99久久久精品蜜桃| 精品久久久久久,| 十八禁人妻一区二区| 99国产精品99久久久久| 日韩欧美三级三区| 久久久国产成人精品二区| 一级黄色大片毛片| 欧美日韩乱码在线| 日日夜夜操网爽| 中文字幕人成人乱码亚洲影| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 天天一区二区日本电影三级 | 国产亚洲精品综合一区在线观看 | 久久久国产欧美日韩av| 中文字幕人妻熟女乱码| 亚洲欧美日韩另类电影网站| 国产伦人伦偷精品视频| 免费一级毛片在线播放高清视频 | 国内精品久久久久精免费| 91成人精品电影| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片 | 免费无遮挡裸体视频| 国产黄a三级三级三级人| 色av中文字幕| 免费在线观看影片大全网站| 久久精品成人免费网站| 欧美性长视频在线观看| 国产精品日韩av在线免费观看 | 亚洲av成人av| 欧美国产日韩亚洲一区| 天天躁狠狠躁夜夜躁狠狠躁| 女警被强在线播放| 精品少妇一区二区三区视频日本电影| 亚洲七黄色美女视频| 怎么达到女性高潮| 国产精品久久久久久精品电影 | 午夜日韩欧美国产| 精品人妻在线不人妻| 国产麻豆成人av免费视频| 国产乱人伦免费视频| 久久精品人人爽人人爽视色| 日本三级黄在线观看| 日韩精品青青久久久久久| 一级,二级,三级黄色视频| 男男h啪啪无遮挡| 久久精品国产亚洲av香蕉五月| 无人区码免费观看不卡| 国产视频一区二区在线看| 欧美日韩亚洲综合一区二区三区_| 国产日韩一区二区三区精品不卡| 久久狼人影院| 国产区一区二久久| 亚洲 国产 在线| 国产精品 国内视频| 亚洲国产欧美网| 一进一出好大好爽视频| 亚洲精品国产一区二区精华液| 老熟妇仑乱视频hdxx| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 搡老熟女国产l中国老女人| 88av欧美| 色尼玛亚洲综合影院| 99国产极品粉嫩在线观看| 午夜福利视频1000在线观看 | 国产精华一区二区三区| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 亚洲性夜色夜夜综合| 91老司机精品| 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 91麻豆av在线| 午夜影院日韩av| 后天国语完整版免费观看| 一边摸一边抽搐一进一出视频| 亚洲中文av在线| 国产精品永久免费网站| 日本欧美视频一区| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类 | aaaaa片日本免费| 亚洲三区欧美一区| 18美女黄网站色大片免费观看| 美国免费a级毛片| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| 久久人人爽av亚洲精品天堂| 一进一出好大好爽视频| 午夜成年电影在线免费观看| 国产真人三级小视频在线观看| 女警被强在线播放| 精品熟女少妇八av免费久了| 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 最近最新免费中文字幕在线| 亚洲av五月六月丁香网| 叶爱在线成人免费视频播放| 男男h啪啪无遮挡| 黄片小视频在线播放| 色综合欧美亚洲国产小说| 此物有八面人人有两片| 日韩有码中文字幕| 国产高清视频在线播放一区| 好男人在线观看高清免费视频 | av视频免费观看在线观看| 国产97色在线日韩免费| 亚洲少妇的诱惑av| 久久久精品国产亚洲av高清涩受| 日韩免费av在线播放| 久久久久九九精品影院| 一级a爱视频在线免费观看| 午夜亚洲福利在线播放| 国产成+人综合+亚洲专区| 亚洲精品国产精品久久久不卡| 国产精品永久免费网站| 亚洲第一av免费看| www国产在线视频色| 韩国精品一区二区三区| 午夜老司机福利片| 亚洲全国av大片| 亚洲五月婷婷丁香| 国产亚洲精品久久久久久毛片| 两个人视频免费观看高清| 高潮久久久久久久久久久不卡| 在线永久观看黄色视频| 在线播放国产精品三级| 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 国产精品久久久av美女十八| 成年女人毛片免费观看观看9| 国产精品影院久久| 极品人妻少妇av视频| 悠悠久久av| 大香蕉久久成人网| 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 这个男人来自地球电影免费观看| 又紧又爽又黄一区二区| 国产精品久久视频播放| 成年版毛片免费区| 日韩av在线大香蕉| 欧美激情久久久久久爽电影 | 久热爱精品视频在线9| 搡老妇女老女人老熟妇| 亚洲一区二区三区不卡视频| 黄色视频,在线免费观看| 久久国产亚洲av麻豆专区| 亚洲第一青青草原| 久久热在线av| 在线免费观看的www视频| 亚洲第一电影网av| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 精品久久久久久成人av| 男女做爰动态图高潮gif福利片 | 亚洲 国产 在线| 国产欧美日韩一区二区精品| 激情视频va一区二区三区| 亚洲精华国产精华精| 欧美精品啪啪一区二区三区| 亚洲av熟女| 又黄又粗又硬又大视频| 亚洲精品中文字幕一二三四区| 99国产精品99久久久久| 一个人免费在线观看的高清视频| 看片在线看免费视频| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| 国产精品野战在线观看| 在线天堂中文资源库| 国产人伦9x9x在线观看| 久久中文看片网| 久久久久精品国产欧美久久久| 国产真人三级小视频在线观看| 久久精品国产99精品国产亚洲性色 | 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| av天堂久久9| 一进一出好大好爽视频| 天堂影院成人在线观看| 美女高潮喷水抽搐中文字幕| 一级毛片女人18水好多| 亚洲中文av在线| 欧美最黄视频在线播放免费| 久久久久久久午夜电影| 中文字幕高清在线视频| 久久久精品欧美日韩精品| 91精品国产国语对白视频| 国产三级黄色录像| 精品国产国语对白av| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 日日夜夜操网爽| 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看| 91精品国产国语对白视频| 国产午夜精品久久久久久| 日韩大尺度精品在线看网址 | 久久精品国产综合久久久| videosex国产| 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 男女之事视频高清在线观看| 国产精品国产高清国产av| 国产精品av久久久久免费| 亚洲中文日韩欧美视频| 久久中文字幕一级| 又黄又爽又免费观看的视频| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 精品少妇一区二区三区视频日本电影| 久久天躁狠狠躁夜夜2o2o| 老司机在亚洲福利影院| 国产成人影院久久av| 一边摸一边做爽爽视频免费| 91成年电影在线观看| 桃色一区二区三区在线观看| 免费少妇av软件| 色av中文字幕| 亚洲激情在线av| 正在播放国产对白刺激| 99精品欧美一区二区三区四区| 黄片播放在线免费| 美女高潮到喷水免费观看| 精品久久久久久久人妻蜜臀av | 欧美大码av| 黄色女人牲交| 欧美av亚洲av综合av国产av| 久久精品影院6| 免费观看精品视频网站| 美女高潮到喷水免费观看| 亚洲中文日韩欧美视频| 黄频高清免费视频| 九色亚洲精品在线播放| 免费看十八禁软件| 麻豆久久精品国产亚洲av| 别揉我奶头~嗯~啊~动态视频| 国产精品综合久久久久久久免费 | 国产精品98久久久久久宅男小说| 午夜福利视频1000在线观看 | 久久久国产成人精品二区| 国产又爽黄色视频| 在线播放国产精品三级| 国产欧美日韩精品亚洲av| 国产成人免费无遮挡视频| 精品久久久久久久毛片微露脸| 女人爽到高潮嗷嗷叫在线视频| 亚洲成国产人片在线观看| 久久久久亚洲av毛片大全| av片东京热男人的天堂| 欧美在线黄色| 制服人妻中文乱码| 国产一区二区激情短视频| 色播在线永久视频| 狠狠狠狠99中文字幕| 国产精品98久久久久久宅男小说| 午夜精品在线福利| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 桃色一区二区三区在线观看| 一区福利在线观看| 两个人看的免费小视频| 色综合站精品国产| 欧美国产精品va在线观看不卡| 亚洲国产欧美一区二区综合| 久久久久久免费高清国产稀缺| 欧美日韩福利视频一区二区| 国产男靠女视频免费网站| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 国产亚洲精品综合一区在线观看 | 国产精品九九99| 看黄色毛片网站| 中文字幕另类日韩欧美亚洲嫩草| 老熟妇乱子伦视频在线观看| 一个人观看的视频www高清免费观看 | 国产男靠女视频免费网站| 精品久久蜜臀av无| 免费av毛片视频| 中文字幕久久专区| 免费看美女性在线毛片视频| 老司机在亚洲福利影院| 国产av又大| 日韩精品青青久久久久久| 可以免费在线观看a视频的电影网站| 在线观看免费视频网站a站| 久久热在线av| 一个人免费在线观看的高清视频| 美女国产高潮福利片在线看| 国产亚洲av嫩草精品影院| 久久人人97超碰香蕉20202| 国产成人欧美在线观看| 日韩精品免费视频一区二区三区| 最近最新中文字幕大全电影3 | 久久人妻福利社区极品人妻图片| 国产精品一区二区精品视频观看| 香蕉国产在线看| 国产精品久久久久久亚洲av鲁大| 99久久综合精品五月天人人| 宅男免费午夜| 热re99久久国产66热| 51午夜福利影视在线观看| 超碰成人久久| 高清毛片免费观看视频网站| 男人舔女人的私密视频| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 午夜免费观看网址| 久久 成人 亚洲| 免费女性裸体啪啪无遮挡网站| 成年版毛片免费区| 别揉我奶头~嗯~啊~动态视频| 日本五十路高清| 国产成人啪精品午夜网站| 日本精品一区二区三区蜜桃| 亚洲av成人一区二区三| 日本 欧美在线| 国产一区二区三区视频了| 免费不卡黄色视频| 免费av毛片视频| 国产1区2区3区精品| 久久人妻熟女aⅴ| 亚洲欧美激情在线| 精品一区二区三区四区五区乱码| 久久国产精品男人的天堂亚洲| 最近最新中文字幕大全电影3 | 色播亚洲综合网| 色哟哟哟哟哟哟| 久久久久久亚洲精品国产蜜桃av| 亚洲视频免费观看视频| 真人做人爱边吃奶动态| 色播亚洲综合网| 悠悠久久av| 久久草成人影院| 亚洲成人国产一区在线观看| 亚洲国产日韩欧美精品在线观看 | 日本在线视频免费播放| 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区| 又黄又爽又免费观看的视频| 69精品国产乱码久久久| 啦啦啦免费观看视频1| 日本三级黄在线观看| 久久香蕉国产精品| 亚洲av五月六月丁香网| 国产精品美女特级片免费视频播放器 | 亚洲色图 男人天堂 中文字幕| 男女午夜视频在线观看| 国产aⅴ精品一区二区三区波| 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看| 天天一区二区日本电影三级 | 91成人精品电影| 丰满人妻熟妇乱又伦精品不卡| 色综合站精品国产| av免费在线观看网站| 国产熟女午夜一区二区三区| 国产又爽黄色视频| 亚洲情色 制服丝袜| 国产av精品麻豆| 在线观看免费视频日本深夜| 99久久精品国产亚洲精品| 日韩欧美一区视频在线观看| 国产成人免费无遮挡视频| 人成视频在线观看免费观看| 纯流量卡能插随身wifi吗| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看| 黄色视频不卡| 又黄又爽又免费观看的视频| av福利片在线| 久热这里只有精品99| 一区二区三区国产精品乱码| 夜夜爽天天搞| 亚洲av成人不卡在线观看播放网| 午夜成年电影在线免费观看| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 岛国在线观看网站| 久久午夜亚洲精品久久| 巨乳人妻的诱惑在线观看| 人妻丰满熟妇av一区二区三区| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区黑人| 亚洲一区二区三区色噜噜| 欧美一级毛片孕妇| 欧美日本中文国产一区发布| 又黄又爽又免费观看的视频| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 久久精品aⅴ一区二区三区四区| 一级毛片高清免费大全| 中国美女看黄片| 亚洲少妇的诱惑av| 久久人妻av系列| 亚洲人成77777在线视频| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 精品国产一区二区久久| 一进一出好大好爽视频| netflix在线观看网站| 黄片大片在线免费观看| 午夜影院日韩av| 50天的宝宝边吃奶边哭怎么回事| 满18在线观看网站| 成人av一区二区三区在线看| 亚洲国产精品久久男人天堂| 亚洲性夜色夜夜综合| 久久精品91蜜桃| aaaaa片日本免费| 欧美黑人精品巨大| 人妻丰满熟妇av一区二区三区| cao死你这个sao货| 乱人伦中国视频| 久久久久精品国产欧美久久久| 叶爱在线成人免费视频播放| 色哟哟哟哟哟哟| 国产激情久久老熟女| 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 久热爱精品视频在线9| 亚洲成a人片在线一区二区| 99精品欧美一区二区三区四区| 岛国在线观看网站| 热99re8久久精品国产| 国产精品免费一区二区三区在线| 老司机深夜福利视频在线观看| 99精品久久久久人妻精品| 国产蜜桃级精品一区二区三区| 国产亚洲精品av在线| 两性夫妻黄色片| av欧美777| 亚洲国产日韩欧美精品在线观看 | 亚洲欧美日韩高清在线视频| 啪啪无遮挡十八禁网站| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av在线| 亚洲在线自拍视频| 最近最新中文字幕大全免费视频| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| 国产高清videossex| 男女之事视频高清在线观看| 18禁观看日本| 精品第一国产精品| cao死你这个sao货| 日韩免费av在线播放| www.熟女人妻精品国产| 免费女性裸体啪啪无遮挡网站| 精品电影一区二区在线| 国产精品一区二区在线不卡| 日本a在线网址| 天天躁夜夜躁狠狠躁躁|