• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Toward a Theoretical Framework for Researching Second Language Production

    2013-12-04 07:39:08
    當(dāng)代外語研究 2013年12期

    National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore

    Various theories have been proposed to account for second language production and the systematic variation characteristic of such production. Most of these theories, however, constitute only partial explanations because they often fail to handle mixed empirical findings about factors held to affect second language output and underlie systematic variation. This paper proposes a new theoretical framework for posing and addressing research questions about second language production in general and systematic variation in particular. Building on influential cognitive models of second language learning and use, the theoretical framework comprises three cognitive dimensions, viz. knowledge representation, attentional focus, and processing automaticity, which interact with each other. The theoretical framework is justified by drawing on the cognitive literature and cumulative findings from second language acquisition (SLA) research. The paper also outlines possible applications of the framework to issues of interest to SLA researchers working on a number of different fronts.

    Keywords: attention, automaticity, knowledge representation, models of second language learning and use, second language production, systematic variation of second language output

    Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Guangwei Hu, English Language & Literature Academic Group, National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University, 1 Nanyang Walk, Singapore 637616. Email: guangwei.hu@nie.edu.sg

    INTRODUCTION

    Since the advent of Swain’s (1985) Comprehensible Output Hypothesis, there has been a sustained interest in second language (L2) production as a learning mechanism (de Bot, 1996; Gass & Mackey, 2006; Izumi & Bigelow, 2000; Muranoi, 2007; Robinson, 2005; Shehadeh, 1999; Swain, 2005; Swain & Lapkin, 1995). To gain insight into oral and written L2 output as a learning mechanism, however, it is necessary to develop a fuller understanding of the psycholinguistic processes involved in L2 production. A useful point of departure for research aimed at unveiling these processes is an inquiry into the systematic variation that characterizes L2 output (i.e., synchronically patterned variability in the L2 learner’s output that obtains under different conditions and across different contexts of language use). Such systematic variation can manifest itself in the accuracy, complexity, fluency, and appropriacy of the L2 learner’ oral and written production (Ellis, 2009e; Gilabert, 2007; Housen & Kuiken, 2009; Larsen-Freeman, 2009; Ong & Zhang, 2013; Pallotti, 2009).

    Variable L2 production is an extensively documented phenomenon in second language acquisition (SLA) research. Although there is considerable evidence that systematic variation is not a unitary cognitive phenomenon (see Ellis, 2003; Skehan, 2009; Tarone, 1988), it is not entirely clear what cognitive factors are responsible for it. Empirical inconsistencies abound in this regard. Take variation in the linguistic accuracy of L2 output for example. Some studies (Hu, 2002a; Hulstijn & Hulstijn, 1984; Sorace, 1985) found distinct patterns of accuracy associated with differently represented knowledge (i.e., explicit vs. implicit knowledge), but others (Alderson, Clapham, & Steel, 1997; Elder, Warren, Hajek, Manwaring, & Davies, 1999; Grigg, 1986; Seliger, 1979) failed to find significant relationships between explicit knowledge and L2 performance or proficiency. Some studies witnessed more accurate performance on form-focused tasks (Green & Hecht, 1992) or more cognitively complex tasks (Kuiken & Vedder, 2007; Robinson, 1995; Robinson, Cadierno, & Shirai, 2009), whereas others reported greater accuracy on more communicative tasks (Leeman,etal., 1995; Liceras, 1987) or less complex tasks (Hu, 1999; Skehan & Foster, 1999). To further complicate the issue, there were also studies (e.g., Stokes, 1985; Tarone, 1988) which identified different, even opposite, patterns of variation for different target structures on the same production tasks. If one thing is clear from the research cited above, it is that research on the systematic variation of L2 output needs to move beyond hypotheses focusing on a single factor, be it monitoring, attentional focus, or cognitive complexity. Rather, such variation should be studied within a broader and more dynamic psycholinguistic framework of L2 production which can accommodate the interactions among various contributory factors.

    This paper proposes an integrated theoretical framework for posing and addressing research questions about L2 production in general and its systematic variation in particular. It first discusses two psycholinguistic models of language production—Bialystok’s Analysis and Control Model and McLaughlin’s Information Processing Model—which inform the theoretical framework presented herein. It then presents and justifies the theoretical framework, drawing on the relevant literature in cognitive psychology as well as findings from SLA research. Finally, it outlines how the theoretical framework may serve as a point of departure for integrating some hitherto isolated constructs in SLA research and for exploring issues of interest to SLA researchers working on a number of different fronts. For ease and clarity of exposition, the discussion of the framework focuses on systematic variation in the linguistic accuracy of L2 learner production in both the oral and the written mode, with the understanding that accuracy is not independent of, but interacts with, complexity, fluency, and appropriacy of L2 output (Ellis, 2009e; Larsen-Freeman, 2009; Norris & Ortega, 2009; Skehan, 2009). However, it can be argued that the cognitive constructs and their interrelationships proposed in the framework to account for systematic variation in accuracy can also adequately address other dimensions of systematic variation and their interplay.

    BIALYSTOK’S ANALYSIS AND CONTROL MODEL

    Since the early 1980s, Ellen Bialystok has been developing a psycholinguistic model of language learning and use represented in terms of two intersecting dimensions. These dimensions were first introduced as theanalyzedandautomaticfactors in Bialystok (1982), then reformulated as the continua ofknowledgeandcontrolin Bialystok and Ryan (1985), and more recently reconceptualized asanalysisandcontrol(Bialystok, 1990a, 1990b, 1994). The following discussion is based on Bialystok (1990a, 1993, 1994), as they contain detailed presentations of the latest formulation.

    The fundamental assumption of Bialystok’s model is that language proficiency is not a unitary cognitive construct (Bialystok, 1990a). Her dissatisfaction with Anderson’s (1983) conflation of knowledge and cognitive control of knowledge in a single distinction betweendeclarativeandproceduralknowledgehas led her to maintain that “an adequate description of language proficiency requires specification of at least two components of that process” (1990b, p. 48). These two basic components areanalysisof linguistic knowledge andcontrolof linguistic processing. As shown in Figure 1, they constitute the axes along which language learning and use vary. Analysis is “the process by which mental representations that were loosely organized around meanings... become rearranged into explicit representations that are organized around formal structures” (1994, p. 159). In other words, analysis has to do with the abstractness and generality of the knowledge in question. The benefit of increasing analysis is a gain in the accessibility of the knowledge that has been analyzed. According to Bialystok (1990b), language learning starts with unanalyzed (implicit) knowledge, which gradually evolves into analyzed (explicit) knowledge through continuous and progressive analysis. Moving away from her early conceptualization (see Bialystok, 1981) of the developmental relationship between implicit and explicit knowledge, Bialystok now maintains that implicit knowledge can become explicit but the reverse does not hold. Furthermore, Bialystok contends that although the explicitness of knowledge is an indicator of the level of its mental organization, explicit knowledge is not to be confused with conscious knowledge. “The highest level of analysis of knowledge is associated with... consciousness, but consciousness is not criterial to greater levels of analysis” because “a criterion of consciousness seriously underestimates the level of analysis with which linguistic knowledge is represented” (Bialystok, 1990a, p. 122).

    Figure 1 Language Use as a Function of

    Control is defined as “the process of selective attention that is carried out in real time” (Bialystok, 1994, p. 160). Different from her earlier definition of control as the relative ease and rapidity with which linguistic knowledge can be accessed in actual use, the construct in the latest version refers to “the processing choice about where attention should best be spent in the limited-capacity system” (Bialystok, 1990a, p. 122). Since in real-time language processing there are multiple sources of information competing for attention, effective processing requires “the ability to control attention to relevant and appropriate information and to integrate those forms in real time” (p. 125). As their control develops, learners are more capable of executing their intentions and directing their performance in response to task demands. Thus, a selective allocation of attention is criterial to higher levels of control. Fluency or automatic performance is “an emergent property of high levels of control” (p. 122). An important developmental consequence of increasing control isintentionalprocessing, which is held to be necessary for successful performance. Another point stressed by Bialystok is that the process of selective attention and the process of knowledge analysis develop independently. “Control processes, that is, are required to retrieve any knowledge, whether analyzed or nonanalyzed; and the development of this focusing and retrieval mechanism proceeds separately and in response to different experiences than does the development of analyzed representations of knowledge” (Bialystok & Ryan, 1985, p. 216).

    Although the model is intended to cover both language acquisition and use, it runs into difficulties as an account of L2 development (see Hulstijn, 1990; Schmidt, 1992). To begin with, although the postulated independence of analysis and control is justified at the level of product, in the sense that a high level of control does not necessarily imply a corresponding high level of analysis or vice versa (DeKeyser, 2003), it does not seem to be tenable in its strong form from a developmental point of view. Gombert (1990, p. 179) argues that “the analysis of knowledge must necessarily occur at an earlier level of development than the aspect of control, since the awareness of the structural characteristics of language is a prerequisite for their deliberate integration into the subject’s control of his or her activities of linguistic processing.” There are also other problems with the model as a developmental account of L2 acquisition. The claim that the acquisition of a language, be it an L1 or an L2, must start with unanalyzed (implicit) knowledge is certainly at variance with what happens in many an L2 classroom. Besides, the conceptualization of the development of control as an increasing ability to attend selectively, though plausible in the context of L1 acquisition, does not seem to fare well with L2 acquisition (Hu, 1999). Many L2 learners, especially adolescent and adult learners, are certainly able to, and have to, engage in intentional processing from the very beginning (Schmidt, 1990). It appears that these L2 learners’ problem is not so much a lack of ability to attend selectively in L2 learning. Rather, what they have to grapple with is an inadequate knowledge base of the target language as well as a lack of automaticity in accessing and applying their newly internalized knowledge (DeKeyser, 2007a).

    To claim that the model is not adequately equipped as an account of L2 learning, however, is not to deny its value as a plausible explanation of L2 performance. In fact, it can serve as a starting point for a viable psycholinguistic description of L2 learners’ proficiency and performance at a given point of development. The interactions of knowledge representation and selective attention with task demands can account for a large portion of the variation manifested in L2 learners’ performance on different tasks (Bialystok, 1982; Hu, 1999, 2002a). As Bialystok (1990a) argues, “the demands imposed upon language learners by various language uses can be described more specifically in terms of the demands placed upon each of these processing components, and the proficiency of learners can be described more specifically by reference to their mastery of each of the components” (p. 117). Useful work on the relationship between differently represented knowledge and language use can be found in Bialystok (1982, 1993) and Hu (1999, 2002a).

    The model, however, is incomplete in that an important dimension,automaticity, which figures prominently in most accounts of cognitive processes (Segalowitz, 2003), is notably missing. Bialystok’s (1990a) own justification for its exclusion is that it is “relatively inaccessible to investigation,” “impervious to development” ( p. 118), and “epiphenomenal” in the sense that it is only “an outcome of levels of attentional allocation” (Bialystok, 1990b, p. 50). More recent studies by DeKeyser (1997) and Robinson (1997) and research cited in Anderson (1995, Chapter 9) clearly show that automaticity is accessible to investigation and can be developed. To be sure, automaticity is developmentally related to analysis and attention in important ways, but it is also distinct from them synchronically and is certainly not epiphenomenal, inasmuch as automatic processing does not necessarily imply high levels of analysis of knowledge or absence/presence of selective attention (DeKeyser & Juffs, 2005; Ellis, 2009a). Automaticity refers to the relative ease and efficiency with which the knowledge that is needed can be accessed in real-time production (DeKeyser & Juffs, 2005; Jiang, 2012). It can be argued that it is not the reduction of attention that creates the impression of automaticity, but the automaticity of processing that makes the reduction possible (Hu, 1999). More importantly, as Faerch and Kasper (1986, pp. 53-54) point out, the conflation of processing modes and selective attention renders it impossible to investigate “how a procedure (e.g., as involved in establishing a syntactic plan) is used by different learners under different conditions.” In this light, automaticity, though interrelated with attention in complex ways, describes a different category of processing and should be kept separate from attention. Its interactions with knowledge and attention may hold the answer to much of systematic variation in L2 production that has a psycholinguistic origin (Hu, 1999).

    MCLAUGHLIN’S INFORMATION-PROCESSING MODEL

    McLaughlin (1987, 1990; McLaughlin, Rossman, & McLeod, 1983) has developed a cognitive model of L2 acquisition and use that draws on theories and research about human learning and information-processing in cognitive psychology (e.g., Cheng, 1985; Posner & Snyder, 1975; Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977). Fundamental to the model is the notion that “humans are limited-capacity information processors, both in terms of what they can attend to at a given point in time and in terms of what they can handle on the basis of knowledge and expectations” (McLaughlinetal., 1983, p. 137). The processing limitations that humans have to grapple with are perceived to exist along two independent dimensions that intersect:focusofattentionandautomaticityofprocessing.

    The first dimension, very much similar to thecontroldimension of Bialystok’s model, is a continuum of attentional allocation ranging from highly peripheral to highly focal attention. The focus of attention is characterized by selectivity and is largely a function of a particular task configuration and the limited human capacity to handle information. On this view, attention is a limited-capacity system of cognitive resources which is deployed to regulate the influx of information and select relevant information for further processing operations, depending on specific goals and plans. Thus, in accomplishing a complex task such as communication in an L2, a learner necessarily selects some task components to focus attention on but attends to the rest only peripherally in accordance with task demands and as a consequence of processing limitations. Similarly, in learning a complex skill, “subtasks need to be integrated by a ‘plan’ whereby the selection of subactivities is regulated according to overriding goals... [and]... attention is given to various subtasks on a time-sharing basis, with the learner attending first to one subtask and then to another” (McLeod & McLaughlin, 1986, p. 109).

    The second dimension of the model is a continuum of information-processing modes ranging from highly controlled to highly automatic processing with many gradations falling in between. Following Shiffrin and Schneider (1977), controlled processing is

    ... a temporary activation of nodes [i.e., sets of informational elements in human memory] in a sequence. This activation is under the attentional control of the subject and, since attention is required, only one such sequence can normally be controlled at a time without interference. Consequently, controlled processes are thought to intrude on the ability to perform simultaneously any other task that also requires a capacity investment. Controlled processes are therefore tightly capacity-limited, but have the advantage of being relatively easy to set up, alter, and apply to novel situations. (McLeod & McLaughlin, 1986, p. 110)

    By contrast,

    automaticprocessinginvolves the activation of certain nodes in memory every time the appropriate inputs are present. This activation is a learned response that has been built up through the consistent mapping of the same input to the same pattern of activation over many trials. Since an automatic process utilizes a relatively permanent set of associative connections in long-term storage, most automatic processes require an appreciable amount of training to develop fully. Once learned, an automatic process occurs rapidly and is difficult to suppress or alter. (McLaughlin, 1987, p. 134)

    On this view, controlled processing involves deliberate choices and combinations of informational elements, operates slowly and sequentially, and consumes much processing capacity. Automatic processing, on the other hand, is fast, effortless, and light on attentional resources, thus leaving more attention available for use by controlled processes being carried out simultaneously on other task components. Learning involves a progression from controlled to automatic processing. “Complex skills are learned and routinized (i.e., become automatic) only after the earlier use of controlled processes... that regulate the flow of information from working to long-term memory” (McLeod & McLaughlin, 1986, p. 111). Automaticity is achieved largely through practice. However, automaticity involves more than the frequency with which a certain procedure is activated: It also interacts with the restructuring of knowledge (McLaughlin, 1990). Successful performance depends on an optimal allocation of processing resources and a flexible balance between automatic and controlled processing. Figure 2 sums up the application of the two-dimensional model to L2 performance.

    Figure 2 Performance as a Function of

    The way the two dimensions intersect each other to yield four basic types of performance seems to run counter to the foregoing characterization of automatic and controlled processing. According to the model, controlled processing requires more than peripheral attention for its successful operation, and automatic processing consumes little attentional capacity. The existence of the focal/automatic and peripheral/controlled combinations in Figure 2 would prima facie contradict the tenets of the model. This contradiction, however, is not a real one. It is true that the model suggests that, under normal circumstances, automatic processing does not need focal attention and that the successful operation of controlled processing is predicated on the availability of sufficient attention. Nevertheless, it also posits that attentional allocation is under some level of cognitive control: Humans can choose to focus their attention deliberately on automatic processes or to divert it from controlled processes. In other words, attention is subject to voluntary control and can be directed by internal intentions (Schmidt, 2001). Therefore, theoretically speaking, “controlled and automatic processes can be either the focus of attention (which is usually, but not always, true of controlled processes) or on the periphery of attention (which is usually, but not always, true of automatic processes)” (McLaughlinetal., 1983, p. 142).

    The model, as outlined above, gives a plausible explanation for variable L2 performance under certain conditions of language use (see Hu, 2002a; Hulstijn & Hulstijn, 1984; Lehtonen, 1990; Segalowitz, 1986). However, the model is far from comprehensive and, consequently, its explanatory power suffers. In particular, it fails to include aknowledgedimension which has been found by Bialystok and others (e.g., DeKeyser, 2003; Ellis, 2004, 2005; Hu, 2002b) to play an important role in L2 use. McLaughlin himself came to see this weakness when he attempted to use the model to account for the findings of a reading study (McLeod & McLaughlin, 1986). Since then, he has introduced a new construct into his model:restructuring. Drawing on work done by cognitive psychologists (Cheng, 1985; Karmiloff-Smith, 1986b), McLaughlin (1990, p. 113) sees restructuring as the process whereby “l(fā)earners reorganize their internal representational framework.” Restructuring allows learning to occur in a discontinuous fashion, involves the replacement of old components with new ones, and brings about decrements as well as improvements in performance. Though potentially useful in explaining diachronic variation in L2 use, the notion of restructuring cannot easily be related to the two-dimensional model discussed above. As the following section shows, however, the inclusion of a knowledge dimension in a model of L2 performance is justifiable and presents important advantages.

    KNOWLEDGE, ATTENTION, AND AUTOMATICITY: A 3-DIMENSIONAL FRAEMWORK

    It was argued in the previous sections that both Bialystok’s Analysis and Control Model and McLaughlin’s Information-Processing Model constitute only partial explanations of L2 production. Given their respective strengths and because of their complementarity, however, an integration of the two models should yield a more comprehensive framework for researching L2 performance in general and systematic variation in particular. The theoretical framework to be outlined in this section represents an endeavor to this end. As will become clear, such an integration is not unwarranted but can produce a more powerful theoretical framework within which empirical work can be carried out. It not only overcomes the limitations of the original models but also addresses a theoretical impasse that Ellis (2009d) identifies in relation to current attempts to conflate knowledge and automaticity into a single dimension of controlled explicit knowledge vs. automatic implicit knowledge. As Ellis notes, one consequence of such a conflation is that researchers are often compelled to “invoke additional characteristics of the two types of knowledge (e.g., awareness and focus on meaning/form)” and/or further characteristics of processing such as “online or offline processing” (p. 342) to make unwieldy distinctions that detract from the usefulness of the construct.

    The theoretical framework proposed here, as schematically represented in Figure 3, comprises three dimensions:knowledgerepresentation,attentionalfocus, andprocessingautomaticity. These dimensions are continua rather than dichotomies, in keeping with their original treatment in the two models as well as with research in the fields of cognitive psychology and SLA research (Bialystok, 1990b, 1993; DeKeyser, 1997; Dienes & Perner, 1999; Faerch & Kasper, 1987; Hu, 2002b; Karmiloff-Smith, 1986a; McLaughlin, 1987; Segalowitz, 2003; Tarone, 1988). The three dimensions intersect one another and form, so to speak, the space within which L2 production and its systematic variation occur. Knowledge representation forms a continuum ranging from implicit to explicit knowledge, depending on the level of explicitness with which knowledge is represented. Since the highest level of explicitness is associated with consciousness (Bialystok, 1990a) and because metalinguistic knowledge is more or less consciously represented and verbalizable knowledge about the target grammar (Ellis, 2009a; Hu, 2011b; Roehr, 2007), such knowledge is located at the extreme explicit end of the continuum. Attentional focus refers to the allocation of attentional capacity in L2 performance, which varies from highly peripheral to highly focal attention, depending on task demands and communicative pressure (McLaughlin, 1987; Schmidt, 2001). Processing automaticity is a continuum of information-processing modes which ranges between highly controlled and highly automatic processing (DeKeyser, 1997; Segalowitz, 2003). Within the multidimensional theoretical framework, systematic variation in L2 production is a function of the interactions among knowledge representation, attentional focus, and processing automaticity required by specific production tasks.

    Figure 3 Cognitive Dimensions of L2 Production

    The theoretical framework can be justified on the grounds that each of its dimensions is a well established one in cognitive psychology and SLA research and that there are complex interactions among them that are increasingly understood. Although the very application of the implicit/explicit distinction to learning processes has been controversial, the distinction at the level of product (i.e., in terms of knowledge that has been internalized) is not problematic (Ellis, 2009a). Both DeKeyser (2003) and Ellis (2004) point out that the existence of implicit and explicit knowledge is widely recognized in cognitive psychology, though different terms have been used to refer to the distinction. In the cognitive literature, it is also generally accepted that how knowledge is represented can have important implications for how it is used (Anderson, 1995; Anderson & Fincham, 1994), and there is cumulative neurobiological evidence in support of the validity of the distinction (see Ellis, 2009a, 2009d). Many SLA theorists and researchers (e.g., DeKeyser & Juffs, 2005; Ellis, 2005, 2009b, c; Hu, 1999; Hulstijn, 1990; Johnson, 1996; Roehr, 2007; Sorace, 1985) have also clearly recognized the relevance and usefulness of distinguishing implicit and explicit knowledge. The distinction gives several advantages. It allows a more accurate description of what an L2 learner knows of the target language. It also allows SLA researchers to examine the relationship between what L2 learners know and what they can actually do. Furthermore, it makes it possible to investigate, in a more finely grained manner, the effects of those factors that moderate the use of L2 learners’ internalized knowledge in performance. As Odlin (1986, p. 124) argues,

    without distinctions in the kinds of knowledge underlying second language performance, there is little hope of explaining what it means to ‘know another language’. And without an adequate explanation of such knowledge, discussions of the comparative value of pattern practice, grammar instruction, and other techniques in foreign language pedagogy remain largely speculative.

    The importance of selective attention has also been widely recognized in cognitive sciences and SLA research. Psychologists such as Baddeley (2007), Kahneman and Treisman (1984), Logan (1988), Posner and Petersen (1990), and Wickens (1984) have all assigned an important role to attention in cognitive operations. In virtually all models of perception and fluency, crucial assumptions have been made about attention (Schmidt, 2001). In the field of language use, L1 researchers, notably Labov (1972), have identified attention as an important variable in their work on the style-shifting patterns of native speakers. Similarly, SLA theorists and researchers have emphasized and investigated the role of attention in learning (Leow, 1997; Robinson, 2003; Schmidt, 1990; Sharwood Smith, 1991; Williams, 1999) and in production (Ellis & Yuan, 2004; Ortega, 1999; Robinson, 2005; Schmidt, 1992; Skehan & Foster, 1999). Although attention is not the sole cause of systematic variation in L2 performance (see Tarone, 1988), there is growing evidence that it is one of the most important causes. Schmidt (2001) points out that “the concept of attention is necessary in order to understand virtually every aspect of second language acquisition” (p. 3) and that it is “essential to the control of action” (p. 16). Bialystok (1990b) clearly states that “information can be processed to various degrees of detail, and more fine-grained processing requires more attention” (p. 50). She also makes the point that “l(fā)earners could (and do) choose to increase attention and improve the resolution of the representation” (1990b, p. 50). In a similar vein, McLaughlinetal. (1983) point out that selective attention “is required by the limited capacity of the human mind to process information” (p. 136) and that “an adequate information-processing model of second language learning would include not only specification of how automatic and controlled processes are coordinated, but would also require an understanding of selective attention and the role and function of consciousness” (p. 155).

    The distinction between controlled and automatic processing is another well-established construct in contemporary cognitive research and SLA theorizing. Anderson (1995), Ericsson and Simon (1993), Fisk and Schneider (1984), LaBerge (1975), Logan (1988), Shiffrin and Dumais (1981), and Shiffrin and Schneider (1977) have all made extensive use of this distinction in accounting for how information is typically processed by the human mind. There is substantial empirical support for the distinction from research carried out in areas of visual search, category learning, lexical retrieval, syntactic processing, reading and verbal reporting. Brain imaging research has also pointed to separate neural circuits for controlled and automatic processing (see Segalowitz, 2003). Informed by the work of cognitive scientists, many SLA researchers have also made reference to the controlled/automatic distinction in their theoretical discussions of, and empirical investigations into, L2 acquisition and performance (e.g., DeKeyser, 1997, 2007a; Hu, 2002a; Jiang, 2012; Johnson, 1996; Skehan & Foster, 1999; Towell, Hawkins, & Bazergui, 1996). All this research recognizes the important role played by these two basic modes of information processing (Segalowitz, 2003). There is accumulating evidence that the acquisition of a cognitive skill starts with controlled processing and gradually moves toward more automatic processing which can overcome information-processing limitations by reducing the burden on cognitive resources (Hu, 2002b). It is certainly true to say that McLaughlinetal. (1983) and McLaughlin (1987) heralded this growing interest among SLA researchers in the role of processing automaticity in L2 learning and performance. Although Bialystok has excluded the notion of automaticity from the latest version of her model, the early versions (Bialystok, 1982; Bialystok & Ryan, 1985) did include automaticity as an independent dimension or a component of the control dimension. On one occasion, she argued that it is “reasonable to separate the learner’s representation of knowledge from access to that knowledge and that each of these variables contributes to the learner’s control over that knowledge” (Bialystok, 1988, p. 36).

    In line with findings from cognitive and SLA research as well as theorizing by Bialystok and McLaughlin, the following assumptions underlie the framework. First, explicit knowledge is acquired as analyzed knowledge and stored in long-term memory as a database which can be accessed through general interpretative procedures, whereas implicit knowledge is acquired as specialized procedures for action (Anderson, 1983; Ellis, 2009a). Explicit knowledge is generative, in that it can be used in both familiar and novel contexts (DeKeyser, 2003; Johnson, 1996). It can be easily modified or replaced with new information. However, it takes much time and practice to proceduralize and automatize access to such knowledge (Anderson, 1982; DeKeyser, 2007a, 2007b; McLaughlin, 1987). Implicit knowledge, on the other hand, can quickly become appreciably automatized because it is internalized in an already proceduralized form (Johnson, 1996). As a result, it is procedure-specific and cannot be easily modified or suppressed. Second, attention is selective, partially subject to voluntary control, and essential to action control (Bialystok, 1994; Leow, 1997; McLaughlinetal., 1983; Ortega, 1999; Schmidt, 2001). The most fundamental attribute of attention is its limited capacity. Although psychologists disagree about whether there is only a single attentional resource (Shiffrin, 1976) or there exist multiple attentional resources (Wickens, 1984), it is generally accepted that each resource pool has its capacity limitations (Schmidt, 2001). Finally, controlled processing is performed in a relatively slow, laborious, and serial manner that allows greater control of the relevant information by the performer and easier adaptation to new situations (McLaughlin, 1987). However, it is heavy on processing capacity. Automatic processing, on the other hand, is a more efficient, specialized, and routinized mode of processing that requires little processing capacity and does not interfere with the simultaneous execution of other capacity-taxing processes (Segalowitz, 2003; Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977). Automatic processing occurs only when the information in question has been activated some criterion number of times and has achieved some threshold of strength (Anderson, 1983; DeKeyser, 1997, 2007a). Furthermore, an automatic process is difficult to modify or replace, and may become the default option when attentional pressure is present (Faerch & Kasper, 1987; McLaughlin, 1987; Segalowitz, 2003).

    UTILITIY OF THE THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK

    To be sure, the three dimensions incorporated into the theoretical framework of L2 performance are not new ones, but the way they are perceived to interact with one another and to affect L2 performance systematically through their interactions cut the pie of L2 production and systematic variation in a different manner. Importantly, links and interactions that were missing from previous theorizing on L2 performance but that would seem to merit serious consideration have been proposed and justified. This reformulated account of L2 performance can serve as a useful framework for posing and addressing certain research questions about psycholinguistic processes involved in L2 performance, systematic variation in L2 production, and constraints on access to differently represented knowledge in real time.

    The theoretical framework and its underlying assumptions can generate specific hypotheses for empirical testing. One hypothesis is that there are distinct patterns of formal accuracy associated with differently represented knowledge (i.e., explicit and implicit knowledge) of an L2 on specific production tasks. For if explicit and implicit knowledge are internalized and represented in different ways, it is reasonable to predict that they differ in terms of real-time accessibility at a given point of L2 development, especially at lower stages. Such differences in accessibility can be expected to give rise to differences in the extent to which these two types of knowledge are mobilized on a specific production task (Hu, 1999). A second hypothesis is that the patterns of formal accuracy associated with explicit and implicit knowledge may vary to different extents across production tasks that differ in attentional configurations. In other words, access to different types of knowledge in real-time production differs in their susceptibility to the influence of differences in attentional focus between various production tasks (Hu, 2002a). If explicit and implicit knowledge are accessed in different processing modes at a given point in time, they can be expected to depend, to different extents, on the availability of attentional resources for successful processing. Given the assumption underlying controlled and automatic processing, a third hypothesis predicts that regardless of the explicitness of the knowledge in question, formal accuracy associated with more automatized knowledge will be less affected by attentional pressure than formal accuracy associated with less automatized knowledge (Hu, 2002b). That is, the extent to which attentional configurations on specific tasks constrain the involvement of a knowledge source may depend on the level of automaticity reached in processing the knowledge in question. Other specific hypotheses can also be derived from the theoretical framework. For example, time pressure built into a production task can have a differential effect on the involvement of knowledge sources that differ in terms of processing automaticity, and cognitively demanding tasks may inhibit access to unautomatized explicit knowledge by leaving little attentional capacity available for the operation of controlled processing. Although the above hypotheses are concerned with formal accuracy, specific hypotheses can also be framed with regard to other performance measures such as fluency and linguistic complexity of production (see Ellis, 2009e; Skehan, 2009).

    The theoretical framework is not only aimed at uncovering the primary cognitive factors involved in L2 performance and the cardinal dimensions of its systematic variation but it is also intended to explain how other factors may affect L2 production. Figure 4 presents an expanded view of how various contextual, pedagogical, linguistic, and individual factors may influence L2 production via the three fundamental dimensions of L2 production. Some of these factors are task structure (Skehan & Foster, 1999), discourse contexts (Robinson, 1995), modality of language use (Elder & Ellis, 2009), time pressure (Hu, 2002b), opportunity for planning (Ellis, 2009e; Ellis & Yuan, 2004), type of instruction (Erlam, Loewen, & Philp, 2009; Hu, 2011a, 2011b), transfer of training (DeKeyser, 2007a; Hu, 2010), learning environments (Philp, 2009), linguistic prototypicality (Hu, 2002b), linguistic features (Ellis, 2009c), age (DeKeyser, 2003; Ellis, 2005; Philp, 2009), working memory (DeKeyser & Koeth, 2011; Erlam, 2009), current proficiency/developmental stage (Elder & Ellis, 2009), learning/cognitive style (Zietek & Roehr, 2011), strategies for language learning/use (Hu, 1999), cross-linguistic influences (Elder & Manwaring, 2004; Hu, Skuja-Steele, & Hvitfeldt, 2000), and motivation (Ellis, 2009d). The overarching claim is that these factors come to shape L2 performance and contribute to systematic variation through the interplay among knowledge representation, attentional focus, and processing automaticity in the process of production. They can also have their effect felt by mediating the contributions of other factors to L2 learning and use.

    By way of illustration, I shall examine how findings from research in two areas can be interpreted within this theoretical framework. The first area is L1 transfer. One type of L1 transfer much discussed in the SLA literature is automatic transfer (Faerch & Kasper, 1987; Sharwood Smith, 1986). It is posited that this type of transfer involves the activation of highly automatized L1 procedures in situations where linguistic processing is on the periphery of attention and where there is competition between L1 and L2 subplans of production. The study by Huetal. (2000) produced some evidence of automatic transfer. The researchers examined the accuracy with which Chinese learners of English used the English articles on a battery of production tasks which differed in terms of attention to form. They found that although the learners had correct explicit knowledge of the target uses, they frequently failed to supply the definite or indefinite article in obligatory contexts. Many of these omissions could be attributed to the influence of Mandarin Chinese, in which a noun phrase can occur without any determiner regardless of definite or indefinite reference. More importantly, an inverse relationship was found between the tendency to leave out the definite or indefinite article and the amount of attention to form allowed by the production tasks. One plausible interpretation of these results is that the attentional configurations required by the production tasks constrained the learners’ access to their explicit knowledge to different extents. On those tasks where they were under a heavy cognitive load and were preoccupied with conceptual processing, the learners tended to fall back on highly automatized procedures (L1 procedures) as a way of circumventing the attention-consuming process of activating and applying their largely unautomatized explicit knowledge. This would result in a notable decline in their accuracy in using the target structures. However, as Kohn (1986, p. 26) argues, “errors which occur as the result of a transfer on the retrieval level not backed up by the learner’s knowledge are usually not very stable” and “as soon as the output conditions are more favourable the learner will be able to use his knowledge to control for these errors with the result that they disappear.” Thus, on the production tasks that allowed a greater focus of attention on form, the learners’ suppliance of the target structures attained higher levels of accuracy most likely because of their greater opportunity to draw on their explicit knowledge.

    Figure 4 An Expanded View of How Various Contextual, Linguistic,

    The second area of research which can be explored within the theoretical framework concerns the effect of task structure on L2 performance. Several studies (Ellis, 2009e; Ellis & Yuan, 2004; Foster & Skehan, 1996; Huetal., 2000; Ong & Zhang, 2013; Skehan & Foster, 1999) found that different features of tasks had an impact on performance in terms of fluency, linguistic complexity, and/or accuracy. For example, Skehan and Foster (1999) explored the effect of inherent task structure on L2 performance by using structured and unstructured narrative retelling tasks administered under different processing conditions. The researchers were able to demonstrate that tasks with clear inherent structure elicited more fluent performance than those without such clear structure and that greater linguistic complexity was achieved under less demanding processing conditions. There was also a significant interaction effect between task structure and processing conditions on linguistic accuracy. Skehan and Foster interpreted their results as suggesting that those tasks with clear inherent structure eased the processing burden and allowed the participants to allocate greater attention to linguistic processing in a more sustained manner than those tasks with less clear macrostructure. Presumably, the greater allocation of attention to language form enabled access to knowledge that otherwise would have been inaccessible and facilitated processes that were carried out in a relatively controlled mode. Huetal. (2000) also attested to an effect of task structure on performance. The L2 learners in their study completed four spontaneous writing tasks: two narratives and two arguments. They were found to be significantly more accurate in using a number of target structures when they had explicit knowledge than when they had only implicit knowledge. Furthermore, they were markedly more accurate on the narrative tasks than on the argumentative ones when explicit knowledge was available. The researchers attributed the higher accuracy evidenced on the narrative tasks to the participants’ more experience with and greater expertise in narrative structure, which in turn allowed them to allocate more attention to linguistic processing and hence enabled them to access their largely unautomatized explicit knowledge.

    Space constraints prevent a detailed explication of how the other factors presented in Figure 4 may be explored within the integrated theoretical framework. As with L1 transfer and task structure, however, it is not difficult to envisage links between these factors and the dimensions of the theoretical framework. For instance, L2 learners’ online choice of strategies for use may be determined by the attentional configuration required by a specific task and by their automaticity in applying the strategies in question. Similarly, differences in the mode of language use (e.g., speaking vs. writing) and time pressure may impinge on the allocation of attention to various components of the production process and consequently call for different knowledge sources and processing modes. A particular learning style or learning environment may have its impact on L2 performance by pushing the L2 learner toward one, rather than the other, end of the knowledge dimension and/or automaticity dimension. Linguistic factors such as the relative prototypicality or markedness of target uses may affect, among other things, L2 learners’ automaticity in processing their knowledge of these uses. It is noteworthy that when a factor affects any of the three dimensions, viz. knowledge representation, attentional focus and processing automaticity, there can be important repercussions for L2 learners’ development along the other dimensions and for the real-time interplay among all the dimensions in performance. In other words, there are dynamic interrelationships among these dimensions.

    CONCLUSION

    This paper has presented a theoretical framework for researching L2 production that integrates previous theoretical and empirical work on attentional resources, knowledge representation, and processing automaticity in cognitive psychology and SLA. The framework is based on the assumption that a comprehensive account of L2 use and its inherent systematic variability is based on an understanding of the cognitive processes that L2 learners engage in during performance. It is falsifiable in that it can generate specific and empirically testable hypotheses about how the cognitive processes may impact upon L2 production. The theoretical framework represents an attempt to integrate into a coherent conceptualization the many factors known to affect L2 production and contribute to its systematic variation. In other words, this conceptualization makes it possible to examine the impact on L2 use of many hitherto isolated factors (e.g., language learning/use strategies, cross-linguistic influences, cognitive style, and type of instruction) in an integrated framework. How these and other factors come to affect and shape the interactions of knowledge, attention, and control in the very process of L2 use are issues that need to be addressed and worked out in more detail and with greater clarity in future empirical research. Insights into L2 production yielded by such empirical research can feed back into theorizing on second language acquisition.

    REFERENCES

    Alderson, J. C., Clapham, C., & Steel, D. (1997). Metalinguistic knowledge, language aptitude and language proficiency.LanguageTeachingResearch, 1, 93-121.

    Anderson, J. R. (1983).Thearchitectureofcognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Anderson, J. R. (1995).Learningandmemory:Anintegratedapproach. New York: Wiley.

    Anderson, J. R. & Fincham, J. M. (1994). Acquisition of procedural skills from examples.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:Learning,Memory,andCognition, 20, 1322-1340.

    Baddeley, A. D. (2007).Workingmemory,thought,andaction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Bialystok, E. (1981). The role of linguistic knowledge in second language use.StudiesinSecondLanguageAcquisition, 4, 31-45.

    Bialystok, E. (1982). On the relationship between knowing and using forms.AppliedLinguistics, 3, 181-206.

    Bialystok, E. (1988). Psycholinguistic dimensions of second language proficiency. In W. Rutherford & M. Sharwood Smith (Eds.),Grammarandsecondlanguageteaching:Abookofreadings(pp. 31-50). New York: Newbury House.

    Bialystok, E. (1990a).Communicationstrategies:Apsychologicalanalysisofsecondlanguageuse. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Bialystok, E. (1990b). The dangers of dichotomy: A reply to Hulstijn.AppliedLinguistics, 11, 46-51.

    Bialystok, E. (1993). Metalinguistic dimensions of bilingual language proficiency. In E. Bialystok (Ed.),Languageprocessinginbilingualchildren(pp. 113-140). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Bialystok, E. (1994). Analysis and control in the development of second language proficiency.StudiesinSecondLanguageAcquisition, 16, 157-168.

    Bialystok, E. & Ryan, E. B. (1985). A metacognitive framework for the development of first and second language skills. In D. L. Forrest-Pressley, G. E. Mackinnon, & T. G. Waller (Eds.),Metacognition,cognition,andhumanperformance:Vol. 1.Theoreticalperspectives(pp. 207-252). Orlando: Academic Press.

    Cheng, P. W. (1985). Restructuring versus automaticity: Alternative accounts of skill acquisition.PsychologicalReview, 92, 414-423.

    de Bot, K. (1996). The psycholinguistics of the output hypothesis.LanguageLearning, 46, 529-555.

    DeKeyser, R. M. (1997). Beyond explicit rule learning: Automatizing second language morphosyntax.StudiesinSecondLanguageAcquisition, 19, 195-221.

    DeKeyser, R. M. (2003). Implicit and explicit learning. In C. J. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.),Thehandbookofsecondlanguageacquisition(pp. 313-348). Malden: Blackwell.

    DeKeyser, R. M. (2007a). Introduction: Situating the concept of practice. In R. M. DeKeyser (Ed.),Practiceinasecondlanguage:Perspectivesfromappliedlinguisticsandcognitivepsychology(pp. 1-18). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    DeKeyser, R. M. (2007b). Conclusion: The future of practice. In R. M. DeKeyser (Ed.),Practiceinasecondlanguage:Perspectivesfromappliedlinguisticsandcognitivepsychology(pp. 287-304). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    DeKeyser, R. M. & Juffs, A. (2005). Cognitive considerations in L2 learning. In E. Hinkel (Ed.),Handbookofresearchinsecondlanguageteachingandlearning(pp. 437-454). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    DeKeyser, R. M. & Koeth, J. (2011). Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning. In E. Hinkel (Ed.),Handbookofresearchinsecondlanguageteachingandlearning(Vol. 2, pp. 395-406). New York: Routledge.

    Dienes, Z. & Perner, J. (1999). A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge.BehavioralandBrainSciences, 22, 735-808.

    Elder, C. & Ellis, R. (2009). Implicit and explicit knowledge of an L2 and language proficiency. In R. Ellis, S. Loewen, C. Elder, R. Erlam, J. Philp, & H. Reiders (Eds.),Implicitandexplicitknowledgeinsecondlanguagelearning,testingandteaching(pp. 167-193). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

    Elder, C. & Manwaring, D. (2004). The relationship between metalinguistic knowledge and learning outcomes among undergraduate students of Chinese.LanguageAwareness, 13, 145-162.

    Elder, C., Warren, J., Hajek, J., Manwaring, D., & Davies, A. (1999). Metalinguistic knowledge: How important is it in studying a language at university?AustralianReviewofAppliedLinguistics, 22, 81-95.

    Ellis, R. (2003).Task-basedlanguagelearningandteaching. Oxford: Oxford University press.

    Ellis, R. (2004). The definition and measurement of L2 explicit knowledge.LanguageLearning, 54, 227-275.

    Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study.StudiesinSecondLanguageAcquisition, 27, 141-172

    Ellis, R. (2009a). Implicit and explicit learning, knowledge and instruction. In R. Ellis, S. Loewen, C. Elder, R. Erlam, J. Philp, & H. Reiders (Eds.),Implicitandexplicitknowledgeinsecondlanguagelearning,testingandteaching(pp. 3-25). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

    Ellis, R. (2009b). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language. In R. Ellis, S. Loewen, C. Elder, R. Erlam, J. Philp, & H. Reiders (Eds.),Implicitandexplicitknowledgeinsecondlanguagelearning,testingandteaching(pp. 31-64). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

    Ellis, R. (2009c). Investigating learning difficulty in terms of implicit and explicit knowledge. In R. Ellis, S. Loewen, C. Elder, R. Erlam, J. Philp, & H. Reiders (Eds.),Implicitandexplicitknowledgeinsecondlanguagelearning,testingandteaching(pp. 143-166). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

    Ellis, R. (2009d). Retrospect and prospect. In R. Ellis, S. Loewen, C. Elder, R. Erlam, J. Philp, & H. Reiders (Eds.),Implicitandexplicitknowledgeinsecondlanguagelearning,testingandteaching(pp. 335-353). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

    Ellis, R. (2009e). The differential effects of three types of task planning on the fluency, complexity, and accuracy in L2 oral production.AppliedLinguistics, 30, 474-509.

    Ellis, R. & Yuan, F. (2004). The effects of planning on fluency, complexity, and accuracy in second language narrative writing.StudiesinSecondLanguageAcquisition, 26, 59-84.

    Ericsson, K. A. & Simon, H. A. (1993).Protocolanalysis:Verbalreportsasdata(Rev. ed.). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Erlam, R. (2009). The elicited oral imitation test as a measure of implicit knowledge. In R. Ellis, S. Loewen, C. Elder, R. Erlam, J. Philp, & H. Reiders (Eds.),Implicitandexplicitknowledgeinsecondlanguagelearning,testingandteaching(pp. 65-93). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

    Erlam, R., Loewen, S., & Philp, J. (2009). The roles of output-based and input-based instruction in the acquisition of L2 implicit and explicit knowledge. In R. Ellis, S. Loewen, C. Elder, R. Erlam, J. Philp, & H. Reiders (Eds.),Implicitandexplicitknowledgeinsecondlanguagelearning,testingandteaching(pp. 241-261). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

    Faerch, C. & Kasper, G. (1986). Cognitive dimensions of language transfer. In E. Kellerman & M. Sharwood Smith (Eds.),Crosslinguisticinfluenceinsecondlanguageacquisition(pp. 49-65). New York: Pergamon.

    Faerch, C. & Kasper, G. (1987). Perspectives on language transfer.AppliedLinguistics, 8, 111-136.

    Fisk, A. D. & Schneider, W. (1984). Memory as a function of attention, level of processing, and automatization.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:Learning,MemoryandCognition, 10, 181-197.

    Foster, P. & Skehan, P. (1996). The influence of planning and task type on second language performance.StudiesinSecondLanguageAcquisition, 18, 299-323.

    Gass, S. M., & Mackey, A. (2006). Input, interaction and output: An overview.AILAReview, 19, 3-17.

    Gilabert, R. (2007). Effects of manipulating task complexity on self-repairs during L2 oral production.InternationalJournalofAppliedLinguistics, 45, 215-240.

    Gombert, J. E. (1990).Metalinguisticdevelopment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Green, P. S. & Hecht, K. (1992). Implicit and explicit grammar: An empirical study.AppliedLinguistics, 13, 168-184.

    Grigg, T. (1986). The effects of task, time and rule knowledge on grammar performance for three English structures.UniversityofHawaiiWorkingPapersinESL, 5, 37-60.

    Housen, A. & Kuiken, F. (2009). Complexity, accuracy, and fluency in second language acquisition.AppliedLinguistics, 30, 461-473.

    Hu, G. (1999).Explicitmetalinguisticknowledgeatwork:ThecaseofspontaneouswrittenproductionbyformaladultChineselearnersofEnglish. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Nanyang Technological University, Republic of Singapore.

    Hu, G. (2002a). Metalinguistic knowledge at work: The case of written production by Chinese learners of English.AsianJournalofEnglishLanguageTeaching, 12, 5-44.

    Hu, G. (2002b). Psychological constraints on the utility of metalinguistic knowledge in second language production.StudiesinSecondLanguageAcquisition, 24, 347-386.

    Hu, G. (2010). Revisiting the role of metalanguage in L2 teaching and learning.EnglishAustraliaJournal, 26, 61-70.

    Hu, G. (2011a). A place for metalanguage in the L2 classroom.ELTJournal, 65, 180-182.

    Hu, G. (2011b). Metalinguistic knowledge, metalanguage, and their relationship in L2 learners.System, 39, 63-77.

    Hu, G., Skuja-Steele, R., & Hvitfeldt, R. (2000).IsexplicitmetalinguisticknowledgeusefulinL2production? Paper presented at the International Conference on Language in the Mind? Implications for Research and Education, Fort Canning Lodge, Singapore.

    Hulstijn, J. H. (1990). A comparison between the information-processing and the analysis/control approaches to language learning.AppliedLinguistics, 11, 30-45.

    Hulstijn, J. H. & Hulstijn, W. (1984). Grammatical errors as a function of processing constraints and explicit knowledge.LanguageLearning, 34, 23-43.

    Izumi, S. & Bigelow, M. (2000). Does output promote noticing and second language acquisition?.TESOLQuarterly, 34, 239-278.

    Jiang. N. (2012). Automaticity in a second language: Definition, importance, and assessment.ContemporaryForeignLanguages, 384, 34-48.

    Johnson, K. (1996).Languageteachingandskilllearning. Cambridge: Blackwell.

    Kahneman, D. & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.),Varietiesofattention(pp. 29-61). Orlando: Academic Press.

    Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986a). From meta-processes to conscious access: Evidence from children’s metalinguistic and repair data.Cognition, 23, 95-147.

    Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986b). Stage/structure versus phase/process in modelling linguistic and cognitive development. In I. Levin (Ed.),Stageandstructure:Reopeningthedebate(pp. 164-190). Norwood: Ablex.

    Kohn, K. (1986). The analysis of transfer. In E. Kellerman & M. Sharwood Smith (Eds.),Crosslinguisticinfluenceinsecondlanguageacquisition(pp. 21-34). New York: Pergamon.

    Kuiken, F. & Vedder, I. (2007). Cognitive task complexity and linguistic performance in French L2 writing. In M. P. García Mayo (Ed.),Investigatingtasksinformallanguagelearning(pp. 117-135). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

    LaBerge, D. (1975). Acquisition of automatic processing in perceptual and associative learning. In P. M. A. Rabbit & S. Dornic (Eds.),AttentionandperformanceⅤ (pp. 50-64). New York: Academic Press.

    Labov, W. (1972).Sociolinguisticpatterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Larsen-Freeman, D. (2009). Adjusting expectations: The study of complexity, accuracy, and fluency in second language acquisition.AppliedLinguistics, 30, 579-589.

    Leeman, J., Arteagoitia, I., Fridman, B., & Doughty, C. (1995). Integrating attention to form with meaning: Focus on form in content-based Spanish instruction. In R. Schmidt (Ed.),Attentionandawarenessinforeignlanguagelearning(pp. 1-63). Honolulu: University of Hawaii, Second Language Teaching & Curriculum Center.

    Lehtonen, J. (1990). Foreign language acquisition and the development of automaticity. In H. W. Dechert (Ed.),CurrenttrendsinEuropeansecondlanguageacquisitionresearch(pp. 37-50). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

    Leow, R. P. (1997). Attention, awareness and foreign language behaviour.LanguageLearning, 47, 467-505.

    Liceras, J. (1987). The role of intake in the determination of learners’ competence. In S. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.),Inputinsecondlanguageacquisition(pp. 354-373). Rowley: Newbury House.

    Logan, G. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization.PsychologicalReview, 95, 492-527.

    McLaughlin, B. (1987).Theoriesofsecond-languagelearning. London: Edward Arnold.

    McLaughlin, B. (1990). Restructuring.AppliedLinguistics, 11, 113-128.

    McLaughlin, B., Rossman, T., & McLeod, B. (1983). Second language learning: An information-processing perspective.LanguageLearning, 33, 135-158.

    McLeod, B. & McLaughlin, B. (1986). Restructuring or automaticity? Reading in a second language.LanguageLearning, 36, 109-123.

    Muranoi, H. (2007). Output practice in the L2 classroom. In R. M. DeKeyser (Ed.),Practiceinasecondlanguage:Perspectivesfromappliedlinguisticsandcognitivepsychology(pp. 51-84). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Norris, J. M. & Ortega, L. (2009). Towards an organic approach to investigating CAF in instructed SLA: The case of complexity.AppliedLinguistics, 30, 555-578.

    Odlin, T. (1986). On the nature and use of explicit knowledge.InternationalReviewofAppliedLinguisticsinLanguageTeaching, 24, 123-144.

    Ong, J. & Zhang, L.J. (2013). Effects of the manipulation of cognitive processes on EFL writers’ text quality.TESOLQuarterly, 47, 375-398.

    Ortega, L. (1999). Planning and focus on form in L2 oral performance.StudiesinSecondLanguageAcquisition, 21, 109-148.

    Pallotti, G. (2009). CAF: Defining, refining, and differentiating constructs.AppliedLinguistics, 30, 590-601.

    Philp, J. (2009). Pathways to proficiency: Learning experiences and attainment in implicit and explicit knowledge of English as a second language. In R. Ellis, S. Loewen, C. Elder, R. Erlam, J. Philp, & H. Reiders (Eds.),Implicitandexplicitknowledgeinsecondlanguagelearning,testingandteaching(pp. 194-215). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

    Posner, M. I. & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain.AnnualReviewofNeuroscience, 13, 25-42.

    Posner, M. I. & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.),Informationprocessingandcognition:TheLoyolaSymposium(pp. 55-85). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Robinson, P. (1995). Task complexity and second language narrative discourse.LanguageLearning, 45, 99-140.

    Robinson, P. (1997). Generalizability and automaticity of second language learning under implicit, incidental, enhanced, and instructed conditions.StudiesinSecondLanguageAcquisition, 19, 223-247.

    Robinson, P. (2003). Attention and memory during SLA. In C. J. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.),Thehandbookofsecondlanguageacquisition(pp. 631-378). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Robinson, P. (2005). Cognitive complexity and task sequencing: Studies in a componential framework for second language task design.InternationalReviewofAppliedLinguistics, 45, 1-32.

    Robinson, P., Cadierno, T., & Shirai, Y. (2009). Time and motion: Measuring the effects of the conceptual demands of tasks on second language speech production.AppliedLinguistics, 30, 533-554.

    Roehr, K. (2007). Metalinguistic knowledge and language ability in university-level L2 learners.AppliedLinguistics, 29, 173-199.

    Schmidt, R. (1990). The role of consciousness in second language learning.AppliedLinguistics, 11, 129-158.

    Schmidt, R. (1992). Psychological mechanisms underlying second language fluency.StudiesinSecondLanguageAcquisition, 14, 357-385.

    Schmidt, R. (2001). Attention. In P. Robinson (Ed.),Cognitionandsecondlanguageinstruction(pp. 3-32). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Segalowitz, N. S. (1986). Skilled reading in the second language. In J. Vaid (Ed.),Languageprocessinginbilinguals:Psycholinguisticandneuropsychologicalperspectives(pp. 3-19). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Segalowitz, N. S. (2003). Automaticity and second languages. In C. J. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.),Thehandbookofsecondlanguageacquisition(pp. 382-408). Malden: Blackwell.

    Seliger, H. W. (1979). On the nature and function of language rules in language teaching.TESOLQuarterly, 13, 359-369.

    Sharwood Smith, M. (1986). The competence/control model, crosslinguistic influence and the creation of new grammars. In E. Kellerman & M. Sharwood Smith (Eds.),Crosslinguisticinfluenceinsecondlanguageacquisition(pp. 10-20). New York: Pergamon.

    Sharwood Smith, M. (1991). Speaking to many minds: On the relevance of different types of language information for the L2 learner.SecondLanguageResearch, 7, 118-132.

    Shehadeh, A. (1999). Non-native speakers’ production of modified comprehensible output and second language learning.LanguageLearning, 49, 627-675.

    Shiffrin, R. M. (1976). Capacity limitations in information processing, attention, and memory. In W. K. Estes (Ed.),Handbookoflearningandcognitiveprocesses:Vol. 4.Attentionandmemory(pp. 177-236). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Shiffrin, R. M. & Dumais, S. T. (1981). The development of automatism. In J. R. Anderson (Ed.),Cognitiveskillsandtheiracquisition(pp. 111-140). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Shiffrin, R. M. & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory.PsychologicalReview, 84, 127-190.

    Skehan, P. (2009). Modelling second language performance: Integrating complexity, accuracy, fluency, and lexis.AppliedLinguistics, 30, 510-532.

    Skehan, P. & Foster, P. (1999). The influence of task structure and processing conditions on narrative retellings.LanguageLearning, 49, 93-120.

    Sorace, A. (1985). Metalinguistic knowledge and language use in acquisition-poor environments.AppliedLinguistics, 6, 239-254.

    Stokes, J. D. (1985). Effects of student monitoring of verb inflection in Spanish.TheModernLanguageJournal, 69, 377-384.

    Swain, M. (1985). Communicative competence: Some roles of comprehensive input and comprehensible output in its development. In S. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.),Inputinsecondlanguageacquisition(pp. 235-253). Rowley: Newbury House.

    Swain, M. (2005). The output hypothesis: Theory and research. In E. Hinkel (Ed.),Handbookofresearchinsecondlanguageteachingandlearning(pp. 471-483). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Swain, M. & Lapkin, S. (1995). Problems in output and the cognitive processes they generate: A step towards second language learning.AppliedLinguistics, 16, 371-391.

    Tarone, E. (1988).Variationininterlanguage. London: Edward Arnold.

    Towell, R., Hawkins, R., & Bazergui, N. (1996). The development of fluency in advanced learners of French.AppliedLinguistics, 17, 84-119.

    Wickens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.),Varietiesofattention(pp. 63-102). Orlando: Academic Press.

    Williams, J. N. (1999). Memory, attention, and inductive learning.StudiesinSecondLanguageAcquisition, 21, 1-48.

    Zietek, A. A. & Roehr, K. (2011). Metalinguistic knowledge and cognitive style in Polish classroom learners of English.System, 39, 417-426.

    √禁漫天堂资源中文www| 如何舔出高潮| 人人妻人人看人人澡| av.在线天堂| 日本与韩国留学比较| 自拍欧美九色日韩亚洲蝌蚪91 | 黑丝袜美女国产一区| 国产真实伦视频高清在线观看| 国产一区亚洲一区在线观看| 精品国产国语对白av| 中文字幕亚洲精品专区| 看免费成人av毛片| freevideosex欧美| av天堂久久9| 丝袜脚勾引网站| 精品久久久噜噜| 成人免费观看视频高清| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 人人妻人人添人人爽欧美一区卜| 一级av片app| 欧美高清成人免费视频www| 亚洲av日韩在线播放| 日韩三级伦理在线观看| 国产在视频线精品| av在线老鸭窝| 黑丝袜美女国产一区| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线| 亚洲成人手机| av黄色大香蕉| 夜夜看夜夜爽夜夜摸| 久久久久精品久久久久真实原创| 久久 成人 亚洲| 欧美3d第一页| 岛国毛片在线播放| 国产精品国产三级专区第一集| 一边亲一边摸免费视频| 国产黄色视频一区二区在线观看| 熟女av电影| 2022亚洲国产成人精品| 成人影院久久| 麻豆乱淫一区二区| 国产探花极品一区二区| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 狂野欧美激情性bbbbbb| 日韩欧美一区视频在线观看 | 丰满饥渴人妻一区二区三| 国产精品人妻久久久久久| 亚洲av欧美aⅴ国产| 日韩三级伦理在线观看| 水蜜桃什么品种好| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验| 一区二区三区精品91| av一本久久久久| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 国产成人一区二区在线| 欧美日韩在线观看h| 亚洲av电影在线观看一区二区三区| 水蜜桃什么品种好| 高清视频免费观看一区二区| av免费观看日本| 在线观看美女被高潮喷水网站| 亚洲成人一二三区av| 欧美人与善性xxx| 国产精品福利在线免费观看| 免费观看性生交大片5| 91精品国产九色| 内射极品少妇av片p| 69精品国产乱码久久久| 王馨瑶露胸无遮挡在线观看| 亚洲国产毛片av蜜桃av| 在线观看av片永久免费下载| 成人18禁高潮啪啪吃奶动态图 | 狠狠精品人妻久久久久久综合| 日本黄色日本黄色录像| 少妇人妻 视频| 在线观看免费高清a一片| 精品99又大又爽又粗少妇毛片| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| av播播在线观看一区| 丰满迷人的少妇在线观看| 人妻系列 视频| 久久亚洲国产成人精品v| 国产精品成人在线| 黄色毛片三级朝国网站 | 国产男人的电影天堂91| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 午夜视频国产福利| 国产极品天堂在线| 久久久久久久精品精品| av在线app专区| 欧美区成人在线视频| 亚洲av.av天堂| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 免费少妇av软件| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 一级片'在线观看视频| 中文乱码字字幕精品一区二区三区| 日本欧美视频一区| 精品熟女少妇av免费看| 中国国产av一级| 热re99久久精品国产66热6| 97超视频在线观看视频| 国产 精品1| 少妇被粗大的猛进出69影院 | 国产伦理片在线播放av一区| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 亚洲国产最新在线播放| 成人午夜精彩视频在线观看| 26uuu在线亚洲综合色| 精品卡一卡二卡四卡免费| 精品亚洲乱码少妇综合久久| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在| 啦啦啦在线观看免费高清www| 国产片特级美女逼逼视频| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| 国产精品偷伦视频观看了| 在线观看美女被高潮喷水网站| 国产欧美日韩一区二区三区在线 | 国产成人91sexporn| 国产在视频线精品| 久久这里有精品视频免费| 日日啪夜夜撸| 一二三四中文在线观看免费高清| 中文字幕人妻熟人妻熟丝袜美| 精品国产一区二区三区久久久樱花| av又黄又爽大尺度在线免费看| 男人爽女人下面视频在线观看| h视频一区二区三区| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 久久久久久久精品精品| 高清在线视频一区二区三区| 亚洲av日韩在线播放| 亚洲精品国产色婷婷电影| 韩国高清视频一区二区三区| av天堂中文字幕网| 国产精品三级大全| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 精品人妻熟女毛片av久久网站| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 精品卡一卡二卡四卡免费| 免费少妇av软件| 久久久午夜欧美精品| 嫩草影院新地址| 亚洲国产精品国产精品| 亚洲精品视频女| 黄色怎么调成土黄色| 国产深夜福利视频在线观看| 日韩一本色道免费dvd| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图| 纯流量卡能插随身wifi吗| 久久久久久久久久人人人人人人| 欧美另类一区| 久久精品国产鲁丝片午夜精品| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在| 男女国产视频网站| 极品少妇高潮喷水抽搐| freevideosex欧美| 久久韩国三级中文字幕| kizo精华| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av| 精品人妻熟女av久视频| 久久女婷五月综合色啪小说| 日韩强制内射视频| 亚洲不卡免费看| 亚洲av日韩在线播放| 一级二级三级毛片免费看| 在线亚洲精品国产二区图片欧美 | 国产成人精品婷婷| 国产亚洲一区二区精品| 寂寞人妻少妇视频99o| 伦理电影免费视频| 熟妇人妻不卡中文字幕| 大话2 男鬼变身卡| 国产黄片视频在线免费观看| 国产淫语在线视频| 九九久久精品国产亚洲av麻豆| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 欧美高清成人免费视频www| 日韩中字成人| av女优亚洲男人天堂| 99re6热这里在线精品视频| 亚洲国产av新网站| av国产精品久久久久影院| 晚上一个人看的免费电影| 日本免费在线观看一区| 午夜免费鲁丝| 97在线人人人人妻| 婷婷色麻豆天堂久久| 黄色毛片三级朝国网站 | 亚洲av电影在线观看一区二区三区| 亚洲不卡免费看| 中文天堂在线官网| 大片免费播放器 马上看| 欧美97在线视频| 国产精品无大码| 丰满少妇做爰视频| 在线精品无人区一区二区三| 久久人人爽人人爽人人片va| 内地一区二区视频在线| freevideosex欧美| 日韩亚洲欧美综合| 18+在线观看网站| 视频区图区小说| 男人爽女人下面视频在线观看| 免费看光身美女| xxx大片免费视频| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 亚洲成人av在线免费| 亚洲欧洲日产国产| 亚洲精品国产av蜜桃| 三级国产精品欧美在线观看| 免费观看无遮挡的男女| 国产又色又爽无遮挡免| 又大又黄又爽视频免费| 97精品久久久久久久久久精品| 看免费成人av毛片| 青春草亚洲视频在线观看| 内地一区二区视频在线| 街头女战士在线观看网站| 欧美日韩国产mv在线观看视频| 日韩在线高清观看一区二区三区| 中国三级夫妇交换| 国产精品蜜桃在线观看| 亚洲精品乱码久久久久久按摩| 18禁在线无遮挡免费观看视频| 国产在线男女| 免费不卡的大黄色大毛片视频在线观看| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| .国产精品久久| 久久久久人妻精品一区果冻| 丝袜喷水一区| 亚洲怡红院男人天堂| 99热全是精品| 美女大奶头黄色视频| 国产免费视频播放在线视频| 日韩在线高清观看一区二区三区| 九色成人免费人妻av| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 男女边摸边吃奶| 嫩草影院入口| 精品少妇黑人巨大在线播放| 黑人猛操日本美女一级片| 伊人久久国产一区二区| 日韩伦理黄色片| 国产美女午夜福利| 精品熟女少妇av免费看| 欧美日韩国产mv在线观看视频| 国产毛片在线视频| 久久热精品热| 国产在视频线精品| av天堂中文字幕网| 卡戴珊不雅视频在线播放| 18+在线观看网站| 亚洲精品乱久久久久久| 久热久热在线精品观看| 亚洲精品,欧美精品| 一本大道久久a久久精品| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 国产欧美另类精品又又久久亚洲欧美| 99热6这里只有精品| 中文资源天堂在线| a级毛片免费高清观看在线播放| 麻豆乱淫一区二区| 黄色日韩在线| 午夜福利视频精品| 欧美日本中文国产一区发布| 在线观看美女被高潮喷水网站| 大又大粗又爽又黄少妇毛片口| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 国产精品蜜桃在线观看| 国产一区二区三区av在线| 五月天丁香电影| 女性生殖器流出的白浆| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图| 亚洲三级黄色毛片| 亚洲欧洲国产日韩| 久久精品夜色国产| 美女大奶头黄色视频| 亚洲综合精品二区| 色网站视频免费| 啦啦啦中文免费视频观看日本| 国产午夜精品一二区理论片| 国产欧美另类精品又又久久亚洲欧美| 91成人精品电影| 看非洲黑人一级黄片| 久久人人爽人人片av| 寂寞人妻少妇视频99o| 嫩草影院入口| 国产黄片美女视频| 在线观看免费高清a一片| 国产毛片在线视频| 国产日韩一区二区三区精品不卡 | 高清视频免费观看一区二区| 亚洲av电影在线观看一区二区三区| 男人添女人高潮全过程视频| 亚洲自偷自拍三级| 国产日韩欧美亚洲二区| 日本vs欧美在线观看视频 | 大码成人一级视频| 欧美精品高潮呻吟av久久| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 精品99又大又爽又粗少妇毛片| 久久99蜜桃精品久久| 亚洲人与动物交配视频| 久久免费观看电影| 女性被躁到高潮视频| av福利片在线观看| 欧美性感艳星| 人妻 亚洲 视频| 黑人高潮一二区| 97在线人人人人妻| 七月丁香在线播放| 日本91视频免费播放| 看非洲黑人一级黄片| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 黄片无遮挡物在线观看| 在线观看www视频免费| 日韩av不卡免费在线播放| 色婷婷久久久亚洲欧美| 成年美女黄网站色视频大全免费 | 久久久a久久爽久久v久久| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 欧美 日韩 精品 国产| 这个男人来自地球电影免费观看 | av视频免费观看在线观看| 亚洲一级一片aⅴ在线观看| 黑人巨大精品欧美一区二区蜜桃 | 蜜臀久久99精品久久宅男| 色婷婷久久久亚洲欧美| 国产成人精品无人区| 免费观看的影片在线观看| 国产黄片美女视频| 18禁在线无遮挡免费观看视频| 黄色配什么色好看| 亚洲,一卡二卡三卡| 久久亚洲国产成人精品v| 精品一区在线观看国产| 国产在线男女| 久久久久人妻精品一区果冻| 午夜福利,免费看| 国产亚洲午夜精品一区二区久久| 人妻 亚洲 视频| 日韩av免费高清视频| 久久国产精品男人的天堂亚洲 | 少妇人妻 视频| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 秋霞在线观看毛片| 日产精品乱码卡一卡2卡三| a 毛片基地| 国产免费一级a男人的天堂| 日本欧美国产在线视频| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 欧美 亚洲 国产 日韩一| 在线天堂最新版资源| 99久久综合免费| 岛国毛片在线播放| 大片免费播放器 马上看| 久久久久久久久久久免费av| 午夜91福利影院| 久久女婷五月综合色啪小说| 精品久久久精品久久久| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看| 国产免费一区二区三区四区乱码| 国产高清不卡午夜福利| 一本大道久久a久久精品| 高清午夜精品一区二区三区| 春色校园在线视频观看| 三级国产精品欧美在线观看| 欧美精品亚洲一区二区| 91午夜精品亚洲一区二区三区| 国产成人aa在线观看| 亚洲精品aⅴ在线观看| av女优亚洲男人天堂| 91久久精品电影网| 成年人午夜在线观看视频| 日本-黄色视频高清免费观看| 一本久久精品| 国产免费一级a男人的天堂| 男人爽女人下面视频在线观看| 国产女主播在线喷水免费视频网站| 精品一区二区三区视频在线| 男人舔奶头视频| 国产精品国产三级国产专区5o| av在线观看视频网站免费| 在线观看www视频免费| 欧美日韩在线观看h| 亚洲精品456在线播放app| 观看av在线不卡| 中文字幕亚洲精品专区| 99热网站在线观看| 免费黄频网站在线观看国产| 五月玫瑰六月丁香| 精品一品国产午夜福利视频| 久久久精品94久久精品| 亚洲无线观看免费| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜爱| 色94色欧美一区二区| 王馨瑶露胸无遮挡在线观看| 制服丝袜香蕉在线| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 亚洲精品久久久久久婷婷小说| 久久99热6这里只有精品| 蜜臀久久99精品久久宅男| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 免费播放大片免费观看视频在线观看| 丁香六月天网| 国产精品免费大片| 80岁老熟妇乱子伦牲交| 综合色丁香网| 国产精品一区二区三区四区免费观看| 精品卡一卡二卡四卡免费| 熟女电影av网| 丝袜脚勾引网站| 亚洲av国产av综合av卡| 丝袜在线中文字幕| 成人国产麻豆网| 新久久久久国产一级毛片| 爱豆传媒免费全集在线观看| 夜夜爽夜夜爽视频| 久久久久人妻精品一区果冻| 国产欧美亚洲国产| 一本久久精品| 国产永久视频网站| 丰满饥渴人妻一区二区三| 婷婷色综合大香蕉| av在线app专区| 亚洲av福利一区| av视频免费观看在线观看| 9色porny在线观看| h日本视频在线播放| 日韩免费高清中文字幕av| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在| 啦啦啦视频在线资源免费观看| 黄色配什么色好看| 婷婷色综合www| 天天操日日干夜夜撸| 99热全是精品| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 日韩av在线免费看完整版不卡| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区国产| 成人黄色视频免费在线看| 亚洲不卡免费看| av免费在线看不卡| 久热这里只有精品99| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 不卡视频在线观看欧美| 视频中文字幕在线观看| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 在线观看免费高清a一片| 成人亚洲精品一区在线观看| 一区二区三区乱码不卡18| 亚洲精品aⅴ在线观看| 建设人人有责人人尽责人人享有的| 精品久久久久久久久亚洲| 亚洲国产精品国产精品| 一边亲一边摸免费视频| 在线精品无人区一区二区三| 亚洲国产最新在线播放| 精品人妻偷拍中文字幕| 国产精品久久久久久精品古装| 少妇人妻 视频| 在线观看一区二区三区激情| a级毛片免费高清观看在线播放| 精品少妇黑人巨大在线播放| 人妻人人澡人人爽人人| 22中文网久久字幕| 成人二区视频| 中国三级夫妇交换| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验| 男男h啪啪无遮挡| 日本免费在线观看一区| 日韩一区二区三区影片| 日韩精品有码人妻一区| 美女中出高潮动态图| 国产视频首页在线观看| 久久久久久久久久久免费av| 亚洲国产色片| 亚洲天堂av无毛| 伊人久久国产一区二区| 欧美另类一区| 伦理电影大哥的女人| 2021少妇久久久久久久久久久| 男人舔奶头视频| 亚洲精品国产av蜜桃| 五月玫瑰六月丁香| 欧美激情国产日韩精品一区| 欧美日韩国产mv在线观看视频| 欧美精品人与动牲交sv欧美| 国产精品伦人一区二区| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 成年女人在线观看亚洲视频| 99九九线精品视频在线观看视频| 亚洲成人av在线免费| 最近2019中文字幕mv第一页| 哪个播放器可以免费观看大片| 老女人水多毛片| 噜噜噜噜噜久久久久久91| 国产精品人妻久久久久久| av卡一久久| 欧美精品人与动牲交sv欧美| 久久久久久久国产电影| 免费久久久久久久精品成人欧美视频 | 99久久精品热视频| 人妻 亚洲 视频| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜爱| 三级国产精品欧美在线观看| 免费看日本二区| 日韩三级伦理在线观看| 亚洲久久久国产精品| 一区二区三区免费毛片| 国产黄色免费在线视频| 伊人久久国产一区二区| 日韩强制内射视频| 久久久久久久久久人人人人人人| 亚洲精品第二区| 亚洲国产av新网站| 久久毛片免费看一区二区三区| 久久韩国三级中文字幕| 亚洲成人av在线免费| 欧美 亚洲 国产 日韩一| 免费观看在线日韩| 国产男人的电影天堂91| 国产成人aa在线观看| 日韩人妻高清精品专区| 欧美精品亚洲一区二区| 永久免费av网站大全| 曰老女人黄片| 欧美xxⅹ黑人| 精品卡一卡二卡四卡免费| 三级经典国产精品| 一级二级三级毛片免费看| 日韩成人伦理影院| 观看美女的网站| www.av在线官网国产| 中文字幕av电影在线播放| 三级经典国产精品| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 国产熟女欧美一区二区| 亚洲av免费高清在线观看| 高清黄色对白视频在线免费看 | 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区 | 汤姆久久久久久久影院中文字幕| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 欧美日韩视频精品一区| 国产欧美日韩一区二区三区在线 | 一级a做视频免费观看| 欧美高清成人免费视频www| 欧美日韩视频高清一区二区三区二| 欧美+日韩+精品| 国精品久久久久久国模美| 国产男女超爽视频在线观看| 高清欧美精品videossex| 交换朋友夫妻互换小说| 麻豆乱淫一区二区| 日韩大片免费观看网站| 亚洲久久久国产精品| 五月天丁香电影| 国产欧美日韩综合在线一区二区 | 日韩中文字幕视频在线看片| 人妻制服诱惑在线中文字幕| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 亚洲伊人久久精品综合| 人妻一区二区av| 亚洲欧美清纯卡通| 国产黄频视频在线观看| 欧美97在线视频| 久久韩国三级中文字幕| 日日啪夜夜撸| 一个人免费看片子| 啦啦啦中文免费视频观看日本| av网站免费在线观看视频| 精品99又大又爽又粗少妇毛片| 欧美少妇被猛烈插入视频| 99精国产麻豆久久婷婷| 国产亚洲一区二区精品| 午夜激情久久久久久久| 亚洲成色77777| 韩国高清视频一区二区三区| 亚洲国产欧美在线一区| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 99热全是精品| 黑人高潮一二区| 精品酒店卫生间| 99热全是精品| av又黄又爽大尺度在线免费看| 岛国毛片在线播放| 一级黄片播放器| 亚洲精品色激情综合| 亚洲中文av在线| 麻豆精品久久久久久蜜桃| 一本—道久久a久久精品蜜桃钙片| 99re6热这里在线精品视频| 亚洲欧美中文字幕日韩二区| 亚洲国产av新网站| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 曰老女人黄片| 亚洲av男天堂| 看免费成人av毛片| 亚洲精品第二区| av又黄又爽大尺度在线免费看| 五月开心婷婷网| 人妻系列 视频| 又粗又硬又长又爽又黄的视频| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 韩国av在线不卡| 夜夜爽夜夜爽视频| av国产精品久久久久影院| 亚洲精品亚洲一区二区| 少妇精品久久久久久久| 久久精品久久精品一区二区三区| 久久免费观看电影| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 久久97久久精品|