• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Analyzing Genre: Functional Parameters

    2010-12-06 10:19:12JamesMartin
    當代外語研究 2010年10期
    關鍵詞:續(xù)表

    James R. Martin

    (The University of Sydney, Sydney NSW, 2006)

    1. Resources

    This paper considers in a little more detail some recent developments that bear critically on the analyses undertaken. Essentially, these analyses are informed by the model of language known as systemic functional linguistics (hereafter SFL), especially the variety of that model which has evolved in Australia since 1975. The most accessible introduction to this variety of SFL is Eggins (1994).

    The functional grammar of English assumed here is that outlined in Halliday (1985), and as elaborated by Matthiessen (1995). Beyond this, the discourse analyses undertaken draw on Halliday and Hasan (1976) and Martin (1992a). The model of context in focus here has evolved out of earlier work by Martin (1985/1989) and Ventola (1987) and will be considered in more detail below.

    We should also clarify here that a substantial portion of the research impelling the development of the model since the early 80s has been carried out in the field of educational linguistics. The papers by Coffin, Iedema, Rose, Rothery and Stenglin, Veel and White in fact draw on research into secondary school and workplace literacy based in the Metropolitan East Region of the NSW Department of Education’s Disadvantaged Schools Program in the early 90s. This research and the materials deriving from it represented the second phase of literacy initiatives informed by SFL in the region (the first being the late 80s primary school focussed Language and Social Power Project). For a critical introduction to the issues contextualising this work see Cope and Kalantzis (1993). Martin was the chief academic consultant for both phases of this research, and was in addition supervisor of doctoral research by Christie, Iedema, Rose, Rothery and White. As a result it would be fair to say that the variety of SFL deployed here has been strongly influenced by University of Sydney SFL and its involvement in the development of “genre-based” literacy pedagogy and curriculum (for mid-term reviews see Christie 1992; Martin 1993a).

    2. SFL-modelling Language in Context

    Functional linguistics is centrally concerned with showing how the organisation of language is related to its use. In SFL this concern is pursued by modelling both language and social context as semiotic systems in a relationship of realisation with one another. In such a model social context and language meta-redound (Lemke 1995)—which is to say that social context comprises patterns of language patterns. Realisation also entails that language construes, is construed by, and (over time) reconstrues social context. One of the images commonly used to outline this relationship is presented in Figure 1, with language and social context as co-tangential circles.

    Figure 1 Language as the realisation of social context

    Adding to this model, SFL treats the organisation of language and of social context as functionally diversified along similar lines. With language, functional diversification is modelled through metafunctions—ideational, interpersonal, and textual. Ideational linguistic resources are concerned with representation, interpersonal resources with interaction and textual resources information flow. In SFL this intrinsic functional organisation is projected onto context, redounding with the variables of field, tenor and mode—where field focuses on institutional practices, tenor on social relations and mode on channel. Useful discussions of linguistic metafunctions in relation to register variables are found in Halliday (1978), Halliday and Hasan (1980/1985/1989), Martin (1991). The functional diversification reviewed here is laid over the language/social context relation in Figure 2; note the proportionality①of the intrinsic and extrinsic functionality—field is to ideational resources as tenor is to interpersonal resources as mode is to textual resources.

    To begin with, we look at the issue of realisation in relation to the relationship between language and social context. At this point we return to realisation, by way of unpacking the “internal” organisation of language and of social context. As far as language is concerned, SFL follows Hjelmslev in assuming a fundamental separation of content and expression planes. The content plane is concerned with the construal of meaning; the expression plane with the organisation of segmental and prosodic realisations of meaning in spoken or written language (phonology/graphology), or sign. Developing Hjelmslev, SFL takes the step of stratifying his content plane—as lexicogrammar and discourse semantics. Lexicogrammar focuses on resources for incorporating ideational, interpersonal and textual meaning as clauses and smaller units (groups/phrases, words and morphemes), as in Halliday (1985/1994), Matthiessen (1995), with lexis integrated as a more delicate perspective on grammar (Hasan 1987). Discourse semantics focuses on resources for integrating clauses with one another as cohesive text, as in Halliday and Hasan (1976), and Martin (1992a). If we unpack the inner circle of language in Figures 1 and 2, we arrive at the stratified model outlined in Figure 3—as discourse semantics metaredounding with lexicogrammar, metaredounding in its turn with phonology/graphology.

    Figure 2 Functional diversification of language & social context

    Figure 3 Stratification within language as metaredundancy

    Social context, in the model assumed here, is also treated as a stratified system②—comprising the levels of register and genre (as in Eggins and Martin in press). Register is used as a general composite term for the field, mode and tenor variables introduced earlier③. Set up as a level in this way, register is designed to interface the analysis of social context naturally with the metafunctionally diversified organisation of language resources. Genre on the other hand is set up above and beyond metafunctions (at a higher level of abstraction) to account for relations among social processes in more holistic terms, with a special focus on the stages through which most texts unfold. The relation of genre to register as complementary perspectives on the social “content” of language (i.e. context) is thus comparable in some respects to the relation of discourse semantics to lexicogrammar as complementary perspectives on language’s own content plane. Genre and register constitute a stratified perspective on what Hjelmslev referred to as connotative semiotics—semiotic systems that make use of another semiotic system as their expression plane (as opposed to denotative semiotics that have an expression plane of their own). The relation of connotative to denotative semiosis in the model is outlined in Hjelmslev’s terms in Figure 4 (for further discussion see Martin 1992a).

    Figure 4 Language’s stratified content form in relation to a stratified model of social context

    In earlier models of context (e.g. Martin 1992a), an additional layer of context was set up, referred to as ideology, to focus attention on the distribution of discursive resources in a culture, and the divergent ways in which social subjects construe social occasions. This modelling strategy does not appear to have fostered the dialogue among functional linguists and critical theorists that was intended (cf. Threadgold 1991, 1993, 1994). An alternative strategy for enhancing this dialogue will be suggested below. In the metaredundancy④model outlined in Figure 5, register (encompassing field, tenor and mode) contextualises language and is in turn contextualised by genre.

    Figure 5 Language metaredounding with register, metaredounding with genre

    3. Genesis (and Subjectivity)

    Halliday and Matthiessen were drawing increasing attention to language change (e.g. Halliday 1993b; Halliday & Matthiessen to appear). The central issue here has to do with instantiation—the manifestation of system in process, and the way in which manifestations rebound on and ultimately reconstrue the system from which they derive. Halliday (e.g. 1991, 1992a, b, c, 1993a) suggests a model in which language is conceived as a set of probabilistic systems, each instantiation of which in some way re-inflects probability—just as a cricketer’s batting average fluctuates slightly with every run scored. What we call “change” has to do with a drift of reverberations over time that in some way perturbs the system, making way for new resources. Nesbitt and Plum (1988) provide the classic insight into this process of semantic evolution in the context of clause complex relations. Note that a probabilistic model of this kind highlights the way in which connotative semiotics like register and genre are realised through language—namely, by reweighting the probabilities of certain linguistic choices being taken up (i.e. by putting different kinds of meaning at risk).

    Halliday and Matthiessen suggest a framework for modelling semiotic change according to the “time depth“ involved. For relatively short time frames such as that involved in the unfolding of a text, they suggest the term logogenesis⑤(Matthiessen mimeo); for the longer time frame of the development of language in the individual, they use the term ontogenesis (Painter 1984); and for maximum time depth, phylogenesis (as in Halliday’s reading of the history of scientific English; Halliday & Martin 1993). This framework is summarised in Table 1.

    Table 1 Framing Semiotic Change

    In a model of this kind, phylogenesis provides the environment for ontogenesis which in turn provides the environment for logogenesis; in other words, the stage a culture has reached in its evolution provides the social context for the linguistic development of the individual, and the stage this development has reached in the individual provides resources for the instantiation of unfolding texts. Conversely, logogenesis provides the material (i.e. semiotic goods) for ontogenesis, which in turn provides the material for phylogenesis; in other words, texts provide the means through which individuals interact to learn the system, and it is through the heteroglossic aggregation of individual (always already social) systems that the semiotic trajectory of a culture evolves. Language change in this model is read in terms of an expanding meaning potential, a key feature of semiotic systems as they adapt to new discursive and non-discursive (physical and biological) environments.

    Figure 6 Time frames and semogenesis

    At this point in the discussion we might resume the question of ideology, passed over above. The fundamental issue here has to do with the ways in which social subjects are positioned in a culture. To interpret this semiotically, we need to be able to model the meaning potential available to social subjects and, as well, the different ways in which they draw on this potential as they interact with each other. Traditionally, the variables which affect this positioning are generalised under headings such as gender, ethnicity, class and generation. SFL research in this area has been strongly influenced by Bernstein’s notions of coding orientation (e.g. Cloran 1989; Hasan 1990), with Martin (1992a) suggesting that ideology might be conceived as the system of coding orientations engendering subjectivity at a higher level of abstraction than genre.

    One of the dangers inherent in modelling subjectivity along these lines is that of locking subjects in and eliding agency, thereby effacing their potential for resistance and transformation. One way to defuse this danger might be to approach subjectivity dynamically, from the perspective of the genesis theory just outlined. This would enable us to foreground the ways in which subjects engage dynamically with texts as they unfold (logogenesis), the ways in which they are positioned and re-positioned socially throughout their life (ontogenesis) and the ways in which a culture reworks hegemony across generations (phylogenesis). In a model of this kind, it would be more natural to interpret language, register and genre as the projection of semohistory (across all three time frames) than as realising an abstract and reified ideology (as Martin’s model has at times been read to imply). In these terms, language, register and genre constitute the meaning potential that is immanent, from moment to moment as a text unfolds, for the social subjects involved, at the point in the evolution of the culture where meanings are being made. An outline of this perspective is presented as Figure 7, including some indication of the way in which links might be drawn with the work of central post-structuralist theorists in a model of this kind. In Figure 7 Halliday’s (1985/1994) notation for projecting clause complexes has been borrowed to represent one of the senses in which history (i.e. semogenesis) gives meaning to synchronic (albeit always changing) semiosis.

    Figure 7 Language, register and genre as the projection of their semohistory across time frames

    4. A Glance at Register

    Since the early 80s, researchers involved in developing the model of context outlined here have been working towards a semiotic construal of field, tenor and mode variables—inspired by Halliday’s (e.g. 1978) construal of language as a social semiotic. This has proved an ambitious undertaking, and has been shaped in various ways by the availability of research funding and consumer needs with educational concerns compelling a good deal of the research. The descriptive responsibilities of the field, tenor and mode analyses assumed is essentially that outlined in Martin (1992a).

    Field is concerned with systems of activity, including descriptions of the participants, process and circumstances these activities involve. From the perspective of field, the principles for relating activities to one another have to do with the institutional focus of the activities—the ways in which the activities co-articulate everyday and public institutions such as domestic life, bridge, rugby, information technology, sociology, science, bureaucracy and so on. The co-articulating activities of a field tend to share participants, which are organised taxonomically—via hyponymy (class/subclass) or meronymy (part/whole). Participants enter into activities by bonding with processes and circumstances, so that when we hear thatWaughhasjustnotchedupadoublecenturyatSabinaParkwe know the field of cricket is at stake⑥(or, if outsiders, we simply have no idea what is going on). For illustrative work on activity sequences and taxonomies in the field of science see Roseetal. (1992), Halliday and Martin (1993) and Martin and Veel (to appear).

    Tenor is concerned with social relations, as these are enacted through the dimensions of power and solidarity. Following Poynton (1985/1989) work on the “vertical” status dimension has focussed on reciprocity of semiotic choice—taking up her suggestion that interlocutors of equal status have access to the same kinds of meanings whereas interlocutors of unequal status take up semantic choices of different kinds. Work on the “horizontal” contact dimension has been slower to evolve, dependent as it was on the development of better descriptions of evaluative language and its bonding potential (see section 6 below). For important work on tenor in administrative contexts see Iedema (in press) and this paper.

    Mode is concerned with semiotic distance, as this is affected by the various channels of communication through which we undertake activity (field) and simultaneously enact social relations (tenor). From the perspective of field, semiotic distance has to do with the role played by language in the activity—part of what is going on, live commentary on what is going on, reconstruction of what was going on... and on to abstract and theoretical texts in which language reconstitutes activity (see section 7 below). From the perspective of tenor, semiotic distance has to do with the kinds of interaction various channels enable or disable—from the two-way aural and visual feedback of face to face conversation through telephone, radio and television to the no immediate feedback context of reading and writing. Recently, partly as a result of emergence of communication via electronic texts (e-mail; the world wide web), the role of image in relation to verbiage has received increasing attention (as inspired by Kress & van Leeuwen 1990 and O’Toole 1994); the study of multi-modal texts is currently a dynamic growth area as far as mode research is concerned (e.g. Lemke to appear).

    5. A Glance at Genre

    As with register (field, tenor and mode) the framework for analysing genre is essentially that outlined in Martin (1992a; for illustrative implementations see Ventola 1987, 1989, 1995). As such, genre is concerned with systems of social processes, where the principles for relating social processes to each other have to do with texture—the ways in which field, mode and tenor variables are phased together in a text. This means that the principles for relating texts to one another at the level of genre complement those at the level of register. For example, from the perspective of field (register), the instructions for doing a science experiment are immediately related to actually doing the experiment, the procedural recount of that experiment, the explanation the experiment is designed to illustrate (and so on; see Veel below). From the perspective of genre, on the other hand, the instructions are immediately related to a range of procedural texts (e.g directions, recipes, instruction manuals) with closely related texture (i.e. a sequence of commands, potentially prefaced by a list of tools, ingredients, or relevant apparatus, potentially headed by the purpose of the procedure and so on; see Rose below).

    In Australian educational linguistics, genres have been defined as staged, goal-oriented social processes (e.g. Martinetal1987), a definition which flags the way in which most genres take more than a single phase to unfold, the sense of frustration or incompletion that is felt when phases don’t unfold as expected or planned, and the fact that genres are addressed (i.e. formulated with readers and listeners in mind), whether or not the intended audience is immediately present to respond. In these terms, as a level of context, genre represents the system of staged goal-oriented social processes through which social subjects in a given culture live their lives.

    Recent work has foregrounded the question of modelling relations among genres and the nature of generic structure. The impetus for the concern with modelling genres relations stems from two sources—the availability of descriptions of an ever-widening range of genres, especially in educational contexts (families of stories, procedures, explanations etc.), and countering resistance to the notion of a distinct level of genre as part of a model of social context (as voiced by Hasan 1995).

    5.1 genre agnation

    As far as strategies for modelling genre as system are concerned, two approaches have been explored. One involves the traditional SFL approach to agnation, via typology. With this approach, categorical distinctions are set up as oppositions and used to factor out similarities and differences among genres. For simple sets of oppositions, a paradigm can be used to display the relevant valeur, as in Table 2⑦(from Martin in press).

    Table 2 Using a Paradigm to Express Genre Agnation

    Paradigms quickly become saturated as a display mechanism as additional oppositions are taken into account and more complicated typologies are better imaged as system networks. In Figure 8 (taken from Martin in press), procedural genres are added to the picture. The network shows procedures to be immediately agnate to historical recounts with which genres they share a generalised focus on activity; but whereas historical recounts make a statement about the past, procedures direct activity which has yet to be undertaken.

    Figure 8 Using a system network to model genre agnation

    Typological description of this kind and its formalisation in system networks is of course the cornerstone of SFL theory and description. The categorical precision with which it sets up its oppositions and renders meaning as valeur is at the same time its weakness. In grammar, for example, clauses are classified according to process type—and for purposes of analysis and description they have to be one type or another. But as the front cover of Halliday’s 2nd edition of hisIntroductiontoFunctionalGrammarillustrates, process types can also be interpreted as blending into one another as does one colour to another in a rainbow. Behaving (laughing,pondering,looking) is between acting (material processing) and sensing (mental processing) just as purple can be read as a cline between red and blue (for a discussion of typology and process type see Martin 1996a). As a result it is necessary to complement typological analysis with a topological perspective. Lemke introduces the notion of topology in relation to genre relations as follows (the significance of topology elsewhere in the model is taken up in Martin & Matthiessen 1991):

    A topology, in mathematical terms, is A SET OF CRITERIA FOR ESTABLISHING DEGREES OF NEARNESS OR PROXIMITY AMONG THE MEMBERS OF SOME CATEGORY. It turns a “collection” or set of objects into aspacedefined by the relations of those objects. Objects which are more alike by the criteria are represented in this space as being closer together; those which are less alike are further apart. There can be multiple criteria, which may be more or less independent of one another, so that two texts, for instance, may be closer together in one dimension (say horizontal distance), but further apart in another (vertical distance). What is essential, obviously, is our choice of the criteria, theparameters, that define similarity and difference on each dimension. These parameters can be represented as more or less alike. The same set of parameters allows us to describe both the similarities and the differences among texts, or text-types (genres). [from Lemke “The topology of genre...” unpublished earlier draft of Lemke in press]

    By way of brief illustration, consider the genre topology in Table 3, involving a set of secondary school history genres. The display arranges the history genres in a pedagogic developmental sequence, as a scaffolding for apprenticeship into the discipline. It begins on the left with genres that are likely to be more accessible to students because of their similarity to recounting genres in their oral cultures; it ends on the right with genres that involved a great deal of grammatical metaphor as far as text organisation is concerned, and thus depend on levels of literacy commonly associated with senior secondary school.

    Table 3 A Topological Perspective on Genre Agnation (for Secondary School History Genres)

    In Table 3 we have boxed in the genres—but the relation of one to another needs to be seen as a cline. For example, from the typological perspective, the categorical difference between historical recounts and accounts has to do with whether history unfolds sequentially or causally—whether one event is followed by another or leads to another. But many texts use a combination of sequential and causal relations. For purposes of typological classification, we have to define just what percentage of causal relations is required for a text to qualify as an account. The topological perspective on the other hand allows us to position texts on a cline, as more or less prototypical recounts or accounts according to the time/cause parameter.

    Generally, when articulating a pedagogic sequence like that in Table 3, there is more than one parameter to consider. Six parameters are in fact deployed above. And note that these parameters group the history genres in different ways. The “mood” parameter (proposition vs proposition/proposal) opposes most of the genres to the argumentative ones on the right since in general it is only the argumentative genres that include commands on one kind or another (e.g. exhortative appeals to redress past injustices). Conversely, the “identification” parameter opposes most of the history genres to the autobiographical and biographical recounts since in general it is only the auto/biographical recounts that focus on individual participants (as opposed to generic classes of participant). As with all topological parameters these distinctions are matters of degree—most mainstream history, for example, foregrounds male individuals who shape the course of history for generic classes of participant, right across the historical recount, account, explanation and argument spectrum.

    We have found the topological perspective on genre agnation to be a particularly significant one in educational contexts for a number of reasons. For one thing, it facilitates the development of learner pathways (as in Table 3)—outlines of what kinds of developmental sequence will help students move smoothly from control of one genre to another. For another, it helps teachers and students make sense of the real life instantiations of genres they come across in their reading and marking which are not always prototypical examples of canonical genres. And finally, the topological perspective provides principled tools for reasoning about genre mixing and semogenesis—as with the greening of secondary school science (Veel to appear); topologically, the new green genres can be read as involving additional ecological discourse parameters, above and beyond the traditional parameters associated with the discipline.

    5.2 genre structure

    Turning to the question of genre structure, the main developments have involved moving away from simple constituency representations of genre staging. This development first arose in the context of dealing with longer texts, such as those comprising so much of the reading and writing in secondary school subjects. Martin (1994, 1995a) proposes the term macro-genre for texts which combine familiar elemental genres such as recount, report, explanation, exposition and so on. He suggests that logico-semantic relations such as those outlined by Halliday for clause complexes (elaboration, extension, enhancement) can be deployed to reason about genre complexing in longer texts. In the course of this research Martin also noted periodic and prosodic patterns ranging over combinations of genre complexes for example, the introduction and conclusion of an essay, or the evaluative stance of a review. This raised the issue of whether the staging of elemental genres should be reconsidered in light of Halliday (1979) and Matthiessen’s work (1988) on types of structure in grammatical description.

    Martin (1996b) suggests that the structure of even elemental genres can indeed be factored out according to a range of structuring principles, and that constituency representation offers only a relatively compromised image of genre phasing. Martin’s rendering of Halliday and Matthiessen’s types of structure is outlined in Figure 9, which in addition associates the various structuring principles with metafunctions. The basic division is between particulate, prosodic and periodic structure. Particulate structure organises text segmentally, into either orbital or serial patterns. Orbital structure takes one segment as nuclear, and associates other segments with this nucleus as satellites (see White 1997); with serial structure, there is no nuclear segment on which others depend—the text unfolds step by step, with each step dependent on the immediately preceding. Prosodic structure is supra-segmental; it spreads itself across a text, more and less intensely as required, in a way akin to tone contours in phonology. Periodic structure is wave-like; it organises a text into a rhythm of peaks and troughs, as the demands of information flow prescribe.

    Figure 9 Types of structure in relation to modes of meaning

    It follows from this factoring of kinds of structure that genre structure is best interpreted simultaneously from the perspective of particulate, prosodic and periodic representations. By way of illustration, consider the analyses of an exemplum (one of the narrative genres discussed in Rothery & Stenglin 1997). Read from the perspective on constituency, an exemplum like Springsteen’s “Born in the USA” (Cranny-Francis and Martin 1991) consists of an Orientation, followed by an Incident, followed by an Interpretation, followed by a Coda. Read from the perspective of a multi-structural analysis on the other hand, the song has a nuclear stage (the Incident), with Orientation, Interpretation and Coda satellites; alongside this, the song has a chorus (“Born in the USA”) which interacts with the Interpretation in such a way that Springsteen’s scathing deconstruction of America’s military exploitation of its working class reverberates through the song; and punctuating this, the song begins with an Orientation and ends with a Coda that foreground, through relational clauses and low-key musical accompaniment, the powerless position of the veteran Springsteen has written the exemplum about. A crude synopsis of the complementarity of these readings is outlined in Figure 10, which includes some attempt to outline the sense in which constituency representation compromises the factored analysis.

    Figure 10 Types of structure in relation to constituency representation

    6. A Brief Outline of APPRAISAL

    In the course of the secondary school and workplace literacy research noted above, it became necessary to expand our analyses of interpersonal meaning to include more work on evaluative language (building on Martin 1992b, 1995b, 1996c). We tackled this first in the context of narrative analysis in English, moving on to the issue of objectivity in the media and finally focussing on responses to verbal and non-verbal artefacts in English and Creative Arts. Partly in line with this trajectory, we concentrated first on AFFECT (resources for construing emotion), then on JUDGEMENT (resources for judging behaviour in “ethical” terms) and finally on APPRECIATION (resources for valuing objects “aesthetically”). Collectively, we refer to these resources as APPRAISAL, including additional resources for AMPLIFICATION and ENGAGEMENT (a more detailed discussion of these resources is presented in Martin to appear).

    Before looking at the scaffolding we developed for categorising AFFECT, JUDGEMENT and APPRECIATION, let’s illustrate the domains of meaning at issue here. The examples are taken from the opening scene of the movieEducatingRita—the first meeting between Rita and her tutor, Frank. In the first example, Rita expresses her emotional response to the view from Frank’s window, drawing on mental processes of affection (relevant appraisal in bold face in these examples):

    AFFECT (emotions; reacting to behaviour, text/process, phenomena)

    Rita: I love this room. I love the view from this window. Do you like it?

    Frank: I don’t often consider it actually.

    In the following example, Frank makes judgements about the propriety of his appalling teaching as far as most of his students and Rita are concerned:

    JUDGEMENT (ethics; evaluating behaviour)

    Frank: You want a lot and I can’t give it. Between you, and me, and the walls, actually I am an appalling teacher. That’s all right most of the time. Appalling teaching is quite in order for most of my appalling students. But it is not good enough for you young woman.

    In the next example, Rita comments on the value of a book she’s been reading:

    APPRECIATION (aesthetics; evaluating text/process, phenomena)

    Rita: Rita Mae Brown, who wrote “Rubyfruit Jungle”. Haven’t...haven’t you read it?

    Frank: No.

    Rita: It’s a fantastic book, you know. Do you want to lend it?

    Frank: Ah yes.

    Rita: Here.

    Frank: Yes. Well, thank you very much.

    Rita: That’s okay.

    Alongside these evaluative resources, we also considered systems for adjusting a speaker’s commitment to what they are saying (ENGAGEMENT). In the following example Frank uses explicitly subjective modality metaphors (Halliday 1994: 358) to hedge his reply (exasperating Rita in the process).

    ENGAGEMENT (resources for adjusting modal responsibility)

    Rita: ...That’s a nice picture, isn’t it Frank?

    Frank: Uh yes, I suppose it is.

    Rita: It’s very erotic.

    Frank: Actually I don’t think I’ve looked at that picture in 10 years, but, uh, yes, it is, I suppose so.

    Rita: Well, there’s no suppose about it.

    Appraisal resources also include systems for grading evaluations—turning up the volume as in the first example, or playing things down as in the second.

    AMPLIFICATION (resources for grading)

    Rita: ...You know like when I’m in the hair-dressers where I work, I’ll say something like um “Oh, I’m really fucked.” dead loud. I mean, it doesn’t half cause a fuss...

    Frank: [laughs]

    Frank: ...What is your name?

    Rita: Me first name?

    Frank: Well, that would at least constitute some sort of start, wouldn’t it?

    Rita: Rita.

    Frank: Rita. Uh, here we are. Rita. It says here “Mrs S White.”

    Grading is an essential feature of APPRAISAL, which means that a number of related resources for negotiating solidarity are treated separately, under the heading INVOLVEMENT. These resources include the use of names, slang and taboo lexis, specialised and technical terms, standard and non-standard features and the like to orchestrate group membership.

    INVOLVEMENT (lexical in/exclusion; not graded)

    Rita: It’s a fantastic book, you know. Do you want to lend it?

    ...Rita: Yes, but with educated people, they don’t worry, do they? It’s the aristocracy that swears more than anyone. It’s all “Pass me the fucking pheasant.” with them...

    Rita: ...And oh what do they call you round here?

    Frank: Sir. But you may call me Frank.

    Rita: Okay, Frank. That’s a nice picture, isn’t it Frank?

    A rough outline of interpersonal resources is presented as Table 4, organised by strata. In the table, NEGOTIATION refers to MOOD-based resources for exchanging information and goods/services, as outlined in Martin (1992). The APPRAISAL and INVOLVEMENT systems are designed to complement the traditional focus on dialogue into terms of turn-taking and exchange. They are of special relevance to work on the discourse dynamics of solidarity.

    Table 4 A Survey of Interpersonal Resources (Across Strata)

    (續(xù)表)

    Unfortunately, space does not permit a detailed presentation of APPRAISAL resources. Our framework for AFFECT stabilised around the variables outlined in Table 5. Note that at this stage it does not include resources for desire (wanting, wishing, longing etc.), which is elsewhere grouped with modality (modulations of inclination); nor does it include resources for surprise (being shocked, startled, taken aback etc.). AFFECT variables consider:

    1. Are the feelings popularly construed by the culture as positive (good vibes that are enjoyable to experience) or negative ones (bad vibes that are better avoided)?

    —(positive affect) The boy was HAPPY

    —(negative affect) The boy was SAD

    2. Are the feelings realised as a surge of emotion involving some kind of embodied paralinguistic or extra-linguistic manifestation, or more prosodically experienced as a kind of predisposition or ongoing mental state?

    —(behavioural surge) The boy LAUGHED

    —(mental disposition) The boy LIKED the present

    3. Are the feelings construed as directed at or reacting to some specific external agency or as a general ongoing mood for which one might pose the question “Why are you feeling that way?” and get the answer “I’m not sure.”

    —(reaction to other) The boy LIKED the present/the present PLEASED the boy

    —(undirected mood) The boy was HAPPY

    4. How are the feelings graded-towards the lower valued end of a scale of intensity or towards the higher end; or between?

    —(low) The boy LIKED the present

    —(“median”) The boy LOVED the present

    —(high) The boy ADORED the present

    5. The final variable in our typology of affect groups emotions into three major sets. The in/security variable covers emotions concerned with eco-social well-being-anxiety, fear, confidence and trust; the dis/satisfaction variable covers emotions concerned with telos (including frustration)-ennui, anger, curiosity, respect; the un/happiness variable covers emotions concerned with sadness, antipathy, happiness and love.

    —(in/security) The boy was ANXIOUS/CONFIDENT

    —(dis/satisfaction) The boy was FED UP/ABSORBED

    —(un/happiness) The boy was SAD/HAPPY

    Our framework for JUDGEMENT is outlined in Table 6 (for a partial exemplification see Martin 1995c). Judgement can perhaps be thought of as the institutionalisation of feeling, in the context of proposals (norms about how people should and shouldn’t behave). Like AFFECT, it has a positive and negative dimension—corresponding to positive and negative judgements about behaviour. Our media research led us to divide judgements into two major groups, social esteem and social sanction (Iedemaetal. 1994). Judgements of esteem have to do with normality (how unusual someone is), capacity (how capable they are) and tenacity (how resolute they are); judgements of sanction have to do with veracity (how truthful someone is) and propriety (how ethical someone is).⑧

    Table 5 A Framework for Analyzing English AFFECT (with Examples)

    (續(xù)表)

    Table 6 A Framework for Analyzing JUDGEMENT in English

    SOCIAL SANCTION‘mortal’POSITIVE[praise]NEGATIVE[condemn]veracity [truth] truthful, honest,credible...;dishonest,deceitful...;real, authentic,genuine...;glitzy, bogus,fake...;“is s/he honest?” frank, direct... deceptive,manipulative...propriety [ethics] good, moral,ethical...;bad, immoral,evil...;law abiding, fair,just...;corrupt, unfair,unjust...;“is s/he beyondreproach?”sensitive, kind,caring...insensitive, mean,cruel...

    Social esteem involves admiration and criticism, typically without legal implications; if you have difficulties in this area you may need a therapist. Social sanction on the other hand involves praise, and condemnation, often with legal implications; if you have problems in this area you may need a lawyer. The kinds of judgement speakers take up is very sensitive to their institutional position. For example, only journalists with responsibility for writing editorials and other comment have a full range of judgemental resources at their disposal; reporters writing hard news that is meant to sound objective have to avoid explicit judgements completely.

    Our framework for APPRECIATION is outlined in Table 7. Appreciation can perhaps be thought of as the institutionalisation of feeling, in the context of propositions (norms about how products and performances are valued). Like AFFECT and JUDGEMENT it has a positive and negative dimension—corresponding to positive and negative evaluations of texts and processes (and natural phenomena). The system is organised around three variables-reaction, composition and valuation.⑨Reaction has to do with the degree to which the text/process in question captures our attention (reaction:impact) and the emotional impact it has on us. Composition has to do with our perceptions of proportionality (composition:balance) and detail (composition:complexity) in a text/process. Valuation has to do with our assessment of the social significance of the text/process.

    Table 7 A Framework for Analysing APPRECIATION in English

    Of these dimensions, valuation is especially tied up with field, since the criteria for valuing a text/process are for the most part institutionally specific. But beyond this, since both JUDGEMENT and APPRECIATION are in a sense institutionalisations of feeling, all of the dimensions involved will prove sensitive to field. An example of this coupling of ideational and interpersonal meaning is presented for appreciations of research in the field of linguistics in Table 8.

    Table 8 Field Specific Appreciation (for Linguistics)

    Further complicating this issue is the implicit coupling of field with appraisal. This means that ideational meanings can be used to appraise, even though explicitly evaluative lexis is avoided. For example, when Rita mentions Rita Mae Brown’sRubyfruitJunglein the example noted above, it is clearly the case that her tutor Frank and anyone else naively apprenticed into mainstream literary sensibilities are being positioned by Willie Russell to value the book as insignificant, judge Rita as ignorant and feel dislike for the book, its author and Rita as well. Mere mention of the book evokes these feelings, without any explicit appraisal having to be construed at all. Every institution is loaded with couplings of this kind, and socialisation into a discipline involves both an alignment with the institutional practices involved and an affinity with the attitudes one is expected to have towards those practices.

    In practical terms this means that when analysing APPRAISAL in a text, one has to take into account the APPRAISAL that is evoked by ideational tokens, alongside that which is explicitly inscribed; and beyond this it means that analysts need to declare their reading position—since the evaluation one makes of evocations depends on the institutional position one is reading from. There are many readers, for example, who would have aligned with Rita rather than Frank with respect to a popular culture text likeRubyfruitJungle. Similarly, according to reading position, formal and functional linguists will evaluate terms in the following sets of oppositions in complementary ways—with firm convictions about what the good guys and the bad guys should celebrate⑩:

    rule/resource∷cognitive/social∷acquisition/development∷syntagmatic/paradigmatic∷form/function∷language/parole∷system/process∷psychology & philosophy/sociology & anthropology∷cognitive/social∷theory/description∷intuition/corpus∷knowledge/meaning∷syntax/discourse∷pragmatics/context∷parsimony/extravagance∷cognitive/critical∷technicist/humanist∷truth/social action∷performance/instantiation∷categorical/probabilistic∷contradictory/complementary∷proof/exemplification∷reductive/comprehensive∷arbitrary/natural∷modular/fractal∷syntax & lexicon/lexicogrammar...

    By way of summary, a topological perspective on APPRAISAL resources is offered in Figure 11. It tries to align types of AFFECT, JUDGEMENT and APPRECIATION in terms of their affinities across appraisal variables. For example, capacity is aligned with valuation, because of the close relation between judging someone’s behaviour as capable and appreciating the text/process arising from the behaviour (e.g. a skilful cricketer/a skilful innings; a gifted painter/an innovative painting etc.). Similarly, reaction is aligned with the relevant types of AFFECT. ENGAGEMENT (modality, projection, mitigation etc.) and AMPLIFICATION (grading, intensity, etc.) have been included as attendant resources for hedging how committed we are to what we feel and how strongly we feel about it.

    Figure 11 A topological perspective on APPRAISAL resources

    7. Grammatical and Contextual Metaphor

    At this point in the discussion I would like to return to the stratification parameter introduced in section 2 above, and elaborate a little on the way a stratified content plane (lexicogrammar and discourse semantics) is used by Halliday to interpret abstraction, particularly in written English. Then, in closing, I will briefly consider the way in which analogous reasoning in relation to a stratified context plane (register and genre) might be used to interpret one of the kinds of texts that emerge around the issue of “mixed genres”.

    First, stratification and abstraction. Halliday has described the realisation relationship between lexicogrammar and discourse semantics (modelled in images like Figure 3 above) as “natural”. This reading suggests that there is an unmarked correlation between meanings and wordings—that all things being equal we expect, interpersonally, that statements will be realised as declaratives, questions as interrogatives and commands as imperatives, or ideationally, that participants will be realised as nouns, processes as verbs, properties as adjectives and logical relations as conjunctions. Things not being equal, the model allows for tensions to arise between semantics and grammar, opening up the possibility of indirect speech acts (one mood acting as another) or nominalisations (e.g. processes dressed up as nouns). Halliday refers to the realisation and instantiation processes whereby meaning and wording are denaturalised along these lines as grammatical metaphor (see especially Halliday 1985/1994, Chapter 10).

    Interpersonal grammatical metaphors of the indirect speech act variety are well known in the discipline. In the following example from a bureaucratic context, a declarative clause (Iaskthat...) is used in an “imperative” context, in place of the direct imperativeGivethemyourfullsupport:

    (The guidelines have been developed as a resource for staff and students and have my personal endorsement.) I ask that you give them your full support.

    Halliday refers to these indirect speech acts as grammatical metaphors because firstly, they must be read on two levels, with the literal declarative mood taken as standing for the underlying command (it is the interaction of the two levels that makes the meaning); and secondly, the literal level in some sense symbolises the underlying reading (in the example just considered the clause complex, with its first person, present tense projecting verbal process (Iask) and irrealis projection (thatyougive) deconstructs the meaning of imperative in ideational terms).

    Whereas speech act theory focuses on interpersonal metaphors of mood, Halliday’s theory generalises the phenomenon to other interpersonal resources, including modality. Grammatical metaphor in relation to modality was illustrated in the context of the discussion of ENGAGEMENT above, with Frank using first person present tense mental processes of cognition (Isuppose) instead of modal verbs to realise low probability. Borrowing ideational resources to construe interpersonal meanings in this way opens up, among other things, opportunities for verbal play. Russel takes up one of these in the opening scene of the playEducatingRita, with Frank facetiously reconstruing his partner’s modality metaphor as a misplaced query about his ideational desires.

    Frank: ...What do you mean am I determined to go to the pub? —I don’t need determination to get me into a pub... (Russel 1985:2)

    For detailed analyses of interpersonal metaphor across a range of texts see Halliday 1982, Martin 1992a, 1995b.

    Halliday further generalises the notion of grammatical metaphor across metafunctions, to cover tensions between ideational wordings and meanings. These have been extensively reviewed in Martin (1992a, 1993b), and Halliday and Martin (1993), especially in the context of their role in constructing the uncommon sense discourses of science and humanities. Ideational metaphors also abound in bureaucratic contexts, as illustrated in the following example:

    The Passive Restraint Guide is designed to prevent intentional abuse by children. Excessive towel loop length could make intentional abuse more likely. Failure to follow loading instructions could result in serious injury or death. [notice on towel dispenser in the men’s loo in The 99er Diner, north of Vancouver in Canada]

    If we paraphrase the second sentence as “If you don’t act as you’ve been instructed to when loading, you could injure yourself seriously or die”, then various features of the metaphorical version are foregrounded—for example, the realisation of cause as a verb (couldresultin), and processes as nouns (failure,loadinginstructions,seriousinjuryanddeath). Note that grammatical metaphor facilitates metaphor in its more traditional literary usage—how else can playing cricket catch an ear?

    On a mid-week evening the sound of willow and leather in violent collision caught my ear and I hurried along. (Colin Luckhurst “I do like to be beside the seaside”,GuardianWeeklyAugust 21 1994.)

    Grammatical metaphor also facilitates the organisation of APPRAISAL, across institutions. In the following example two judgements (of tenacity and capacity) are nominalised (courageandtoughness) and placed in a causal relationship (contributedto) with the success of the Long March. The advantage of nominalising the judgements for the historian is that s/he can make them Agents in a range of causative processes whose meaning potential is subtle and differentiated enough to support the interpretation of why things happened as they did.

    This question has often been raised by historians, and a number of factors have been suggested to explain the success of the Long March...The courage and toughness of the young members of the Red Army, many of whom were teenagers, also contributed to its success.

    The educational implications of grammatical metaphor are immense, especially in light of Halliday’s suggestion (e.g. 1993c) that grammatical metaphor develops out of non-metaphorical text across all three of the time frames discussed abovetending to unfold later in text (logogenesis), tending to develop later in the maturation of the individual (ontogenesis) and tending to evolve later in the history of a culture (phylogenesis). In other words, if we take the fracturing of glass as an example, Halliday is claiming that a series of realisations which unfolds as follows is in some sense a natural one as far as semogenesis is concerned.

    (the question of how) glass cracks, (the stress needed to) crack glass, (the mechanism by which) glass cracks, as a crack grows, the crack has advanced, will make slow cracks grow, speed up the rate at which cracks grow, the rate of crack growth, we can increase the crack growth rate 1,000 times... (Halliday & Martin 1993: 56)

    If we add to this Halliday’s association of the emergence ideational metaphor with the development of writing systems (Halliday 1985/1989), and the association of ideational metaphor with discipline specific secondary school literacy (Martin 1993b)—then we arrive at a foundational sequencing principle as far as institutionalised learning is concerned, namely, grammatically metaphorical text after non-metaphorical. The principle has certainly shaped genre-based literacy programs in Australia (as reflected in the genre topology in Table 3 above). Its implications and implementations are likely to reverberate through language in education initiatives for years to come.

    The move from non-metaphorical to metaphorical text is in some sense symbolised across literate cultures by the separation of primary and secondary schooling and the drift from thematically organised multi-disciplinary units of work in primary school to strongly classified discipline specific work in secondary school. Halliday (1993c) also suggests a semiotic interpretation of the beginning of school, which has been explored in detail by Painter (1993). What seems to be emerging from this research is that the move to primary schooling symbolises the ability to deal with abstractions. The example I often draw on to illustrate this point comes from my daughter, then aged 4. At Manly pier in Sydney, there is a famous sign which greets visitors: 7milesfromSydneyand1000milesfromcare. Upon my reciting this on arrival, my daughter asked “Where’s care?” Over the next year or two, she learned not to ask questions like this, and then to laugh at her younger brother making mistakes of just that kind.

    A rough frameworkfor classifying types of thing is outlined in Figure 12. Note that the network distinguishes specialised and technical language. Specialised things, like everyday things, can be ostensively defined; you can point them out to apprentices. Technical things on the other hand are abstractions, that have to be linguistically defined (and even if you can point to them, as with the gene in a microscope, just pointing doesn’t fully explain what they mean; Halliday has referred in passing to technical terms as “semiotic ratchets”). Alongside the technical abstraction of science, we find institutional abstractions (bureaucratic ratchets if you will) that organise our lives. Abstractions also include terms for semiosis (e.g. fact, idea, concept, notion) and terms for generic dimensions of meaning (such as size, shape, colour, means, manner, way, etc.).

    Figure 12 Kinds of thing-covering abstractions and grammatical metaphors

    Analysis of the abstract and metaphorical things is not just a matter of derivation. The nounregulationfor example may refer to an institutional abstract, as intheregulationsdon’tpermitthatactivity, or it may be a metaphorical process, as inexcessiveregulationofstudents’behaviourmaynotalwaysbeintheschool’sbestinterests. Analysis is further complicated by the fact that technical and institutional terms are engendered through grammatical metaphor-in other words, grammatical metaphor is used to define and explain these meanings, which are then intended to transcend the metaphors and carry on as abstractions in their own right. The following text from Halliday and Martin (1993: 223) illustrates this process for the termscompression,rarefaction, andcompressionwave, leading to the definition of the once concrete, but now abstract meaningsound.

    (a) If we look at how a tuning fork produces sound (b) we can learn just what sound is. (c) By looking closely at one of the prongs (d) you can see that it is moving to and fro (vibrating). (e) As the prong moves outwards (f) it squashes, or compresses, the surrounding air. (g) The particles of air are pushed outwards (h) crowding against and bashing into their neighbours (i) before they bounce back. (j) The neighbouring air particles are then pushed out (k) to hit the next air particles and so on. (l) This region of slightly “squashed” together air moving out from the prong is called a compression. (m) When the prong of the tuning fork moves back again (n) the rebounding air particles move back into the space that is left. (o) This region where the air goes “thinner“ is called a rarefaction (p) and also moves outwards. (q) The particles of air move to and fro in the same direction in which the wave moves. (r) Thus sound is a compression wave that can be heard.

    For purposes of analysis, these terms are probably best treated as metaphorical at their point of genesis, and as technical abstractions thereafter. In order to help clarify the boundary between metaphorical and abstract things, Halliday’s inventory of types of grammatical metaphor has been included here as Table 9.

    Table 9 Types of Grammatical Metaphor

    (續(xù)表)

    This brief discussion of the educational significance of grammatical metaphor puts us in a position to raise the issue of contextual metaphor and ask whether similar kinds of tension across strata can be found at the level of context. The following text, discussed in Cranny-Francis and Martin (1995), lends itself to an interpretation of this kind. On the surface, the text is organised as a kind of story—a psychological quest narrative; the story, however, was intended in its context to stand for an information report about dolphins as part of an attack on genre-based literacy pedagogy, along the lines of “Why can’t students hand in a range of genres in science? Why are you trying to limit them to boring old information reports?” Informally, then, we might describe the text as a story standing for a report (analogous to informally describing an indirect speech act as a declarative standing for an imperative).

    Rephrasing this in terms of stratal tension at the level of context, we could argue that the register of the texts (its field, mode and tenor selections) invoke narrative, but that certain features of the story (the Socratic dialogue, the technical information about dolphins, the fact that the object of the quest is information, etc.) indicate that at a deeper level that text is intended to instantiate a report genre. As with grammatical metaphor, it is the tension between levels (between genre and register in this instance) that construes the meaning. Contextual metaphors of this kind represent an excursion of English discipline discourse across the curriculum which has been promoted by radical progressive pedagogies around the English speaking world.

    Is this a report or a recount or a discussion?

    Yesterday I went to the library and found a book about dolphins. I had seen dolphins on TV and I was interested in them. I wanted to find the answer to the question, why are dolphins so interesting to humans?

    The book said that dolphins were sea mammals. I bet you didn’t know that dolphins have to breathe air! If they don’t breathe air, they will die.

    I have often wondered what dolphins like to eat, so I looked in the book for information about this. Do they eat other fish, I wondered? I found out that they do.

    I suppose you know what dolphins look like, of course. I found out some interesting things, such as what that dorsal fin is for and how they keep warm.

    Why do we humans like dolphins so much, I often wonder. I searched in the book for the answer to this question, but could not get down to the real reason. The book talked about their tricks and stunts and their general friendliness. As I thought about it, I came to the conclusion that it had something to do with the fact that they, like us, are mammals.

    It may well be that a notion of contextual metaphor, interpreted along these lines, will turn out to be as educationally significant as Halliday’s work on grammatical metaphor-particularly with respect to success in secondary school. In progressive history classrooms, for example, students are often encouraged to write stories from the point of view of participants in historical processes. To succeed in this task, they have to realise that the story they tell must not only be accurate in historical detail, but ought as well to symbolise and focus attention on issues the historian considers significant. The task, in other words, has to be read not as an opportunity to depart from history discourse, but as an opportunity to use a story to stand for historical interpretation.

    Looking beyond education, the notion of grammatical metaphor (interpreted as stratal tension within language) has been developed by systemic linguists as an important insight into the way in which a culture increases its linguistic meaning potential (e.g. Halliday 1992a,c, 1993b; Martin 1993b; Halliday & Martin 1993). By refocussing attention on the issue of contextual metaphor (interpreted as stratal tension at the level of context) we should be able to gain some valuable insights into one of the trajectories along which the social processes of a culture expand-by deploying register variables metaphorically (as figure), to symbolise a complementary genre (as ground).

    附注:

    ① One problem with this image is that it implies that field/ideational resources are in some sense more extensive than mode/textual ot tenor/interpersonal ones; theoretically, this is not the case.

    ② For critique of this stratified model see Hasan (1995); Martin (in press) offers a partial reply.

    ③ In this sense of the term, register is thus comparable to what Halliday and Hasan (e.g. 1985/1989) refer to as context of situation; see Matthiessen (1993) for discussion.

    ④ In Figure 5, a single slash is used to represent metaredundancy, and a double slash to represent meta-metaredundancy—thus language/register∥genre is read as language metaredounding with register, meta-metaredounding with genre.

    ⑤ Along this dimension Halliday and Matthiessen are refocusing a long standing SFL interest in the dynamics of text as process (Martin 1985a; Bateman 1989).

    ⑥ For the record, Steve Waugh is an Australian cricketer, who performed the remarkable feat of scoring more than 200 runs in a single innings against the West Indies in 1995; in this field, 100 runs is considered an outstanding contribution.

    ⑦ Procedural recounts give an after the fact account of a procedure that has been enacted; historical recounts record two or more generations of human endeavour; descriptions characterise a specific participant; descriptive reports characterises a generic class of participants.

    ⑧ At this level of delicacy the types of JUDGEMENT are related to MODALITY (Halliday 1994), in the following proportions-normality is to usuality, as capacity is to ability, as tenacity is to inclination, as veracity is to probability, as propriety is to obligation.

    ⑨ These variables are relatable to the kind of mental processing (Halliday 1994) involved in the appreciation, in following proportions-reaction is to affection, as composition is to perception as valuation is to cognition.

    ⑩ For these complementarities, “/” stands for “is to” and “∷” stands for “as”; they can thus be read asruleistoresourceascognitiveistosocial... etc.

    Bateman, J. 1989. Dynamic systemic-functional grammar: a new frontier [J].Word(40): 1-2. (Systems, structures and discourse: selected papers from the Fifteenth International Systemic Congress): 263-286.

    Christie, F. 1992. Literacy in Australia [J].ARAL(12): 142-155.

    Cloran, C. 1989. Learning through language: the social construction of gender [A]. In R. Hasan & J. R. Martin (eds.).LanguageDevelopment:LearningLanguage,LearningCulture[C]. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex (Meaning and Choice in Language: studies for Michael Halliday): 111-151.

    Cope, W. & M. Kalantzis (eds.). 1993.ThePowersofLiteracy:AGenreApproachtoTeachingLiteracy[M]. London: Falmer (Critical Perspectives on Literacy and Education) & Pittsburg: University of Pittsburg Press (Pittsburg Series in Composition, Literacy, and Culture).

    Cranny-Francis, A. & J. R. Martin. 1991. Contratextuality: the poetics of subversion [A]. In F. Christie (ed.).LiteracyinSocialProcesses:PapersfromtheFirstAustralianSystemicLingusiticsConference,HeldatDeakinUniversity,January1990 [C]. Darwin: Centre for Studies of Language in Education, Northern Territory University: 286-344.

    Cranny-Francis, A. & J. R. Martin. 1995. Writings/readings: how to know a genre [J].Interpretations:JournaloftheEnglishTeachers’AssociationofWesternAustralia28(3): 1-32.

    Eggins, S. 1994.AnIntroductiontoSystemicFunctionalLinguistics[M]. London: Pinter.

    Eggins, S. & J. R. Martin. in press. Genres and registers of discourse [A]. In T. A. van Dijk (ed.).Discourse:AMultidisciplinaryIntroduction[C]. London: Sage.

    Halliday, M. A. K. 1978.LanguageasaSocialSemiotic:thesocialinterpretationoflanguageandmeaning[M]. London: Edward Arnold.

    Halliday, M. A. K. 1979. Modes of meaning and modes of expression: types of grammatical structure, and their determination by different semantic functions [A]. In D. J. Allerton, E. Carney & D. Holdcroft (eds.).FunctionandContextinLinguisticAnalysis:EssaysOfferstoWilliamHaas[C]. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press: 57-79.

    Halliday, M. A. K. 1982. The de-automatization of grammar: from Priestley’s ‘An Inspector Calls’ [A]. In J. M. Anderson (ed.).LanguageFormandLinguisticVariation:PapersDedicatedtoAngusMacIntosh[C]. Amsterdam: Benjamins: 129-159.

    Halliday, M. A. K. 1985a/1994AnIntroductiontoFunctionalGrammar[M]. London: Edward Arnold.

    Halliday, M. A. K. 1985b.SpokenandWrittenLanguage[M]. Geelong, Vic.: Deakin University Press [republished by Oxford University Press 1989].

    Halliday, M. A. K .1991. Towards probabilistic interpretations [A]. In E. Ventola (ed.).FunctionalandSystemicLinguistics:ApproahcesandUses[C]. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter (Trends in Linguistics Studies and Monographs 55): 39-61.

    Halliday, M. A. K. 1992a. How do you mean? [A]. In M. Davies & L. Ravelli (eds.).RecentAdvancesinSystemicLinguistics[C]. London: Pinter: 20-35.

    Halliday, M. A. K. 1992b. Language as system and language as instance: the corpus as a theoretical construct [A]. In J. Svartvik (ed.).DirectionsinCorpusLinguistics:ProceedingsofNobelSymposium82,Stockholm, 4-8August1991 [C]. Berlin: De Gruyter.(Trends in Linguistics Studies and Monographs 65): 61-77.

    Halliday, M. A. K. 1992c. The act of meaning [A]. In J. E. Alatis (ed.).GeorgetownUniversityRoundTableonLanguagesandLinguistics1992:Language,CommunicationandSocialmeaning[C]. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press.

    Halliday, M. A. K. 1993a. Quantitative studies and probabilities in grammar [A]. In M. Hoey (ed.).Data,Description,Discourse:PapersonEnglishLanguageinHonourofJohnMcH.Sinclair[onhissixtiethbirthday] [C]. London: Harper Collins: 1-25

    Halliday, M. A. K. 1993b.LanguageinaChangingWorld[M]. Canberra, ACT: Applied Linguistics Association of Australia (Occasional paper 13).

    Halliday, M. A. K. 1993c. Towards a language-based theory of learning [J].LinguisticsandEducation5.2: 93-116.

    Halliday, M.A.K. 1994.AnIntroductiontoFunctionalGrammar[M]. London: Edward Arnold.

    Halliday, M.A.K. 1994.AnIntroductiontoFunctionalGrammar[M]. London: Edward Arnold.

    Halliday, M. A. K. to appear. On language in relation to the evolution of human consciousness [R]. (paper prepared for Nobel Symposium 92 “The relation between languae and mind”. Stockholm, 8-12 August 1994).

    Halliday, M. A. K. to appear. Things and relations: regrammaticizing experience as technical knowledge [A]. In J. R. Martin & R. Veel (eds.).Readingscience:criticalandfunctionalperspectivesondiscoursesofscience[C].

    Halliday M. A. K. & R. Hasan. 1976.CohesioninEnglish[M]. London: Longman (English Language Series 9).

    Halliday, M. A. K. & R. Hasan. 1980. Text and Context: aspects of language in a social-semiotic perspective [J].SophiaLinguisticaVI. Tokyo: The Graduate School of Languages and Linguistics & the Linguistic Institute for International Communication, Sophia University. [new edition published as M. A. K. Halliday & R. Hasan. 1985.Language,context,andtext:aspectsoflanguageinasocial-semioticperspective. Geelong, Vic.: Deakin University Press [republished by Oxford University Press 1989]]

    Halliday, M. A. K. & J. R. Martin. 1993.WritingScience:LiteracyandDiscursivePower[M]. London: Falmer (Critical Perspectives on Literacy and Education).

    Halliday, M. A. K. & C. M. I. M. Matthiessen. to appear.ConstruingExperiencethroughLanguage:ALanguage-basedApproachtoCognition[M]. Berlin: De Gruyter (Foundations of Communication and Cognition).

    Hasan, R. 1987. The grammarian’s dream: lexis as most delicate grammar [A]. In M. A. K. Halliday & R. P. Fawcett (eds.).NewDevelopmentsinSystemicLinguisticsVol. 1:TheoryandDescription[C]. London: Pinter: 184-211.

    Hasan, R. 1990. Semantic variation and sociolinguistics [J].AustralianJournalofLinguistics(9)2: 221-276.

    Hasan, R. 1995. The conception of context in text [A]. In P. Fries & M. Gregory (eds.).DiscourseinSociety:SystemicFunctionalPerspectives[C]. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex (Meaning and Choice in Language: studies for Michael Halliday; Advances in Discourse Processes Volume L): 183-283.

    Iedema, R. in press for 1996.AdministrationLiteracy(WriteitRightLiteracyinIndustryResearchProject-Stage3) [M]. Sydney: Metropolitan East Disadvantaged Schools Program.

    Iedema, R., S. Feez & P. White. 1994. Media Literacy (Write it Right Literacy in Industry Research Project—Stage 2) [M]. Sydney: Metropolitan East Disadvantaged Schools Program.

    Kress, G. & T. van Leeuwen. 1990.ReadingImages[M]. Geelong: Vic.: Deakin University Press (Sociocultural Aspects of Language and Education).

    Lemke, J. L. 1995.TextualPolitics:DiscourseandSocialDynamics[M]. London: Taylor & Francis (Critical Perspectives on Literacy and Education).

    Lemke, J. to appear. Multiplying meaning: visual and verbal semiotics in scientific text [A]. In J. R. Martin & R. Veel (eds.).ReadingScience:CriticalandFunctionalPerspectivesonDiscoursesofScience[C].

    Martin, J. R. 1985a. Process and Text: two aspects of human semiosis [A]. In J. D. Benson & W. S. Greaves (eds.).SystemicPerspectivesonDiscourse,Vol. 1:SelectedTheoreticalPapersfromthe9thInternationalSystemicCongress[C]. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex: 248-274.

    Martin, J. R. 1985b.FactualWriting:ExploringandChallengingSocialReality[M]. Geelong, Vic.: Deakin University Press [republished by Oxford University Press 1989]

    Martin, J. R. 1991. Intrinsic functionality: implications for contextual theory [J].SocialSemiotics①(1)1: 99-162.

    Martin, J. R. 1992a.EnglishText:SystemandStructure[M]. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Martin, J. R. 1992b. Macroproposals: meaning by degree [A]. In W. A. Mann & S. A. Thompson (eds.).DiscourseDescription:DiverseAnalysesofaFundRaisingText[C]. Amsterdam: Benjamins: 359-395.

    Martin, J. R. 1993a. Genre and literacy-modelling context in educational linguistics [J].AnnualReviewofAppliedLinguistics(13): 141-172.

    Martin, J. R. 1993b. Technology, bureaucracy and schooling: discursive resources and control [J].CulturalDynamics(6)1: 84-130.

    Martin, J. R. 1994. Macro-genres: the ecology of the page [J].Network(21): 29-52.

    Martin, J. R. 1995a. Text and clause: fractal resonance [J].Text(15)1: 5-42.

    Martin, J. R. 1995b. Interpersonal meaning, persuasion and public discourse: packing semiotic punch [J].AustralianJournalofLinguistics(15)1: 33-67.

    Martin, J. R. 1995c. Reading positions/positioning readers: JUDGEMENT in English [J].Prospect:ajournalofAustralianTESOL(10)2: 27-37.

    Martin, J. R. 1996a. Metalinguistic diversity: the case from case [A]. In R. Hasan, C. Cloran & D. Butt (eds.).FunctionalDescriptions:TheoryinPractice[C]. Amsterdam: Benjamins (Current Issues in Linguistic Theory): 323-372.

    Martin, J. R. 1996b. Types of structure: deconstructing notions of constituency in clause and text [A]. In E. Hovy & D. Scott (eds.).BurningIssuesinDiscourse:AMultidisciplinaryPerspective[C]. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Martin, J. R. 1996c. Evaluating disruption: symbolising theme in junior secondary narrative [A]. In R. Hasan & G. Williams (eds.).LiteracyinSociety[C]. London: Longman.

    Martin, J. R. in press. A context for genre: modelling social processes in functional linguistics [A]. In R. Stainton & J. Devilliers (eds.).CommunicationinLinguistics[C]. Toronto: GREF (Collection Theoria).

    Martin, J. R. to appear. Beyond exchange: APPRAISAL resources in English [A]. In S. Hunston & G. Thompson (eds.).EvaluationinText[C]. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Martin, J. R., F. Christie & J. Rothery. 1987. Social processes in education: a reply to Sawyer and Watson (and others) [A]. In I. Reid (ed.).ThePlaceofGenreinLearning:CurrentDebates[C]. Geelong, Vic.: Centre for Studies in Literary Education (Typereader Publications 1): 46-57. [more fully published in The Teaching of English:JournaloftheEnglishTeachers’AssociationofNewSouthWales53. 1987: 3-22]

    Martin, J. R. & R. Veel (eds.). to appear.Readingscience:criticalandfunctionalperspectivesondiscoursesofscience[C]. under review.

    Martin, J. R. & C. M. I. M. Matthiessen. 1991. Systemic typology and topology [A]. In F. Christie (ed.).LiteracyinSocialProcesses:papersfromtheinauguralAustralianSystemicLinguisticsConference,heldatDeakinUniversity,January1990 [C]. Darwin: Centre for Studies in Language in Education, Northern Territory University. 1991: 345-383.

    Matthiessen, C. M. I. M. 1988. Representational issues in systemic functional grammar [A]. In J. D. Benson & W. S. Greaves (eds.).SystemicFunctionalApproachestoDiscourse[C]. Norwood, N J: Ablex: 136-175.

    Matthiessen, C. M. I. M. 1991. Language on language: the grammar of semiosis [J].SocialSemiotics(1)2.: 69-111.

    Matthiessen, C. M. I. M. 1995.Lexicogrammaticalcartography:Englishsystems[M]. Tokyo: International Language Sciences Publishers.

    Matthiessen, C. M. I. M. mimeo. Instantial systems and logogenesis [R]. (Paper presented at the Third Chinese Systemic-functional linguistics symposium, Hangzhou, June 17-20: 1993).

    Matthiessen, C. M. I. M. & M. A. K. Halliday. in press. Systemic Functional Grammar: a first step into theory [A]. In J. Ney (eds.).CurrentApproachestoSyntax[C]. Tokyo: International Language Studies.

    Nesbitt, C. & G. Plum. 1988. Probabilities in a systemic-functional grammar: the clause complex in English [A]. In R. P. Fawcett & D. Young (eds.).NewDevelopmentsinSystemicLinguistics:TheoryandApplication[C]. London: Pinter: 6-38.

    O’Donnell, M. 1990. A dynamic model of exchange [J].Word(41)3.

    O’Toole, M. 1994.TheLanguageofDisplayedArt[M]. London: Leicester University Press (a division of Pinter).

    Painter, C. 1984.IntotheMotherTongue:ACaseStudyofearlyLanguageDevelopment[M]. London: Pinter.

    Painter, C. 1993.LearningthroughLanguage:ACaseStudyintheDevelopmentofLanguageasaResourceforLearningfrom2 1/2to5years[D]. PhD Thesis: Department of Linguistics, University of Sydney.

    Poynton, C. 1985.LanguageandGender:MakingtheDifference[M]. Geelong, Vic.: Deakin University Press. [republished by Oxford University Press 1989]

    Rose, D., D. McInnes & H. Korner. 1992.ScientificLiteracy(WriteitRightLiteracyinIndustryResearchProject-Stage1) [M]. Sydney: Metropolitan East Disadvantaged Schools Program.

    Rothery, J. & M. Stenglin. 1994a.Spine-ChillingStories:AUnitofWorkforJuniorSecondaryEnglish(WriteitRightResourcesforLiteracyandLearning) [M]. Sydney: Metropolitan East Disadvantaged Schools Program.

    Rothery, J. & M. Stenglin. 1994b.ExploringNarrativeinVideo:AUnitofWorkforJuniorSecondaryEnglish(WriteitRightResourcesforLiteracyandLearning) [M]. Sydney: Metropolitan East Disadvantaged Schools Program.

    Rothery, J. & M. Stenglin. 1994 c.WritingaBookReviewAUnitofWorkforJuniorSecondaryEnglish(WriteitRightResourcesforLiteracyandLearning) [M]. Sydney: Metropolitan East Disadvantaged Schools Program.

    Rothery, J. & M. Stenglin. 1997. Entertaining and instructing: exploring experience through story [A]. In F. Chrstine & J. R. Martin (eds.).GenreandInstititutions:SocialProcessintheWorkplaceandSchool[C]. London & New York: 231-263.

    Russel, W. 1985.EducatingRita[M]. London: Longman (Longman Study Texts).

    Threadgold, T. 1991. Postmoderism, systemic-functional linguistics as metalanguage and the practice of cultural critique [A]. In F. Christie (ed.).LiteracyinSocialProcesses:PapersfromtheInauguralAustralianSystemicLinguisticsConference,heldatDeakinUniversity,January1990 [C]. Darwin: Centre for Studies in Language in Education, Northern Territory University. 1991: 60-82.

    Threadgold, T. 1993. Performing genre: violence, the making of protected subjects, and the discourses of critical literacy and radical pedagogy [J].DomainsofLiteracy,ChangingEnglish(1)1: 2-31.

    Threadgold, T. 1994. Grammar, genre and the ownership of literacy [J].IdiomXXIX.2: 20-28. [reprinted inTheTeachingofEnglish3: 62-70.]

    Veel, R. to appear. Being green and going grey: the language of environmentalism in the classroom [A]. In J. R. Martin & R. Veel (eds.).ReadingScience:CriticalandFunctionalPerspectivesondiscoursesofScience[C].

    Ventola, E. 1987.TheStructureofSocialInteraction:ASystemicApproachtotheSemioticsofServiceEncounters[M]. London: Pinter (Open Linguistics Series).

    Ventola, E. 1989. Text analysis in operation: a multilevel approach [A]. R. P. Fawcett & D. Young (eds.).NewDevelopmentsinSystemicLinguisticsvol. 2:theoryandapplication[C]. London: Pinter: 52-77.

    Ventola, E. 1995. Generic and register qualities of texts and their realization [A]. In P. Fries & M. Gregory (eds.).DiscourseinSociety:SystemicFunctionalPerspectives[C]. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex (Meaning and Choice in Language: studies for Michael Halliday; Advances in Discourse Processes Volume L) 3-28.

    White, P. 1997. Death, disruption and the moral order: the narrative impulse in mass-media ‘hard news’ reporting [A]. In F. Chrstine & J. R. Martin (eds.).GenreandInstititutions:SocialProcessintheWorkplaceandSchool[C]. London & New York: 101-133.

    ① This journal in functional linguistics, semiotics and critical theory can be ordered from: Prof David Birch, Communication & Media Studies, Central Queensland University, Rokchampton, Queensland 4702, Australia.

    猜你喜歡
    續(xù)表
    2023 年第一季度航天器發(fā)射統(tǒng)計
    國際太空(2023年4期)2023-05-20 02:41:38
    Analysis of hub genes in small-cell lung carcinoma by weighted gene co-expression network※
    Brief Introduction of Lathyrus Cultivation and Using in Turkey
    2021年第1季度航天器發(fā)射統(tǒng)計
    國際太空(2021年4期)2021-06-18 14:08:50
    Landslide displacement prediction based on the Genetic Simulated Annealing algorithm
    Novel analgesic targets and corresponding analgesic leading compounds
    2019年第三季度航天器發(fā)射統(tǒng)計
    國際太空(2019年12期)2019-02-07 06:57:22
    其他統(tǒng)計資料
    2016年第二季度航天器發(fā)射統(tǒng)計
    國際太空(2016年9期)2016-10-21 07:45:50
    其他統(tǒng)計資料
    国产又色又爽无遮挡免| 尾随美女入室| 色综合色国产| 中文欧美无线码| 免费电影在线观看免费观看| 亚洲国产精品成人综合色| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 国产免费又黄又爽又色| 欧美日韩一区二区视频在线观看视频在线 | 国产极品精品免费视频能看的| 日本与韩国留学比较| 亚州av有码| 久久综合国产亚洲精品| 最近2019中文字幕mv第一页| 久久久精品大字幕| 天堂中文最新版在线下载 | 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 午夜福利在线在线| 亚洲国产欧美在线一区| 99久国产av精品国产电影| 亚洲不卡免费看| 91狼人影院| 中文字幕av成人在线电影| 久久久久久伊人网av| 两个人视频免费观看高清| 国产精品国产三级国产专区5o | 激情 狠狠 欧美| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 尤物成人国产欧美一区二区三区| 国产精品国产高清国产av| av播播在线观看一区| 日本猛色少妇xxxxx猛交久久| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 久久久a久久爽久久v久久| 欧美最新免费一区二区三区| 亚洲av.av天堂| 狠狠狠狠99中文字幕| 99热6这里只有精品| 亚洲av成人精品一区久久| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 免费观看a级毛片全部| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 插阴视频在线观看视频| 亚洲人成网站高清观看| 国产成人精品久久久久久| 国产成人91sexporn| 最近中文字幕高清免费大全6| 三级国产精品片| 国产探花在线观看一区二区| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 精品午夜福利在线看| 六月丁香七月| 免费黄网站久久成人精品| 特级一级黄色大片| 国产一区二区三区av在线| 亚洲精品国产成人久久av| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 久久久久久久国产电影| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 国产一区二区在线观看日韩| 岛国在线免费视频观看| 蜜桃久久精品国产亚洲av| 国产免费男女视频| www日本黄色视频网| 舔av片在线| 国产麻豆成人av免费视频| 亚洲国产精品国产精品| 国产白丝娇喘喷水9色精品| 成人毛片a级毛片在线播放| 少妇丰满av| 国产精品,欧美在线| 国产精品,欧美在线| 欧美性猛交黑人性爽| 观看美女的网站| 免费黄色在线免费观看| 国产色爽女视频免费观看| av在线亚洲专区| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看 | 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 特大巨黑吊av在线直播| 最近最新中文字幕大全电影3| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 一级黄色大片毛片| 色噜噜av男人的天堂激情| 一区二区三区四区激情视频| 国产麻豆成人av免费视频| 天堂影院成人在线观看| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 国产不卡一卡二| 日韩欧美精品v在线| 美女黄网站色视频| 一区二区三区四区激情视频| 国产精品美女特级片免费视频播放器| 亚洲av成人精品一区久久| 内地一区二区视频在线| 内地一区二区视频在线| 国产在线一区二区三区精 | 久久久色成人| 国产精品久久久久久av不卡| 99久久精品热视频| 国产一区二区在线观看日韩| 国产精品无大码| 日本五十路高清| 欧美最新免费一区二区三区| 小蜜桃在线观看免费完整版高清| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 国产精品久久久久久久久免| 久久久色成人| 欧美成人一区二区免费高清观看| 久久久久九九精品影院| 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 床上黄色一级片| 日韩一本色道免费dvd| 久久久久国产网址| 国产探花在线观看一区二区| 最近最新中文字幕大全电影3| 亚洲不卡免费看| a级毛色黄片| 91久久精品国产一区二区三区| 最近最新中文字幕免费大全7| 3wmmmm亚洲av在线观看| 亚洲国产精品sss在线观看| 婷婷色麻豆天堂久久 | 国产麻豆成人av免费视频| 久久精品国产鲁丝片午夜精品| 欧美高清成人免费视频www| 插逼视频在线观看| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 欧美激情在线99| 国产精品美女特级片免费视频播放器| 美女高潮的动态| 久久久久久久午夜电影| 国产真实乱freesex| 久久久久九九精品影院| 日本三级黄在线观看| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 欧美日韩在线观看h| 国产一区二区亚洲精品在线观看| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 国产精品久久久久久久久免| 久久久久久九九精品二区国产| 久久热精品热| 国产 一区精品| 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频| 久久6这里有精品| 中文字幕精品亚洲无线码一区| 国产亚洲最大av| 国语对白做爰xxxⅹ性视频网站| 一级毛片我不卡| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜爱| 中文字幕精品亚洲无线码一区| 超碰97精品在线观看| 男女国产视频网站| 成人无遮挡网站| 激情 狠狠 欧美| 日本爱情动作片www.在线观看| 亚洲成人中文字幕在线播放| 精品久久久久久久久av| 精品一区二区三区视频在线| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| 亚洲乱码一区二区免费版| 99在线人妻在线中文字幕| 午夜免费激情av| 国产69精品久久久久777片| 免费一级毛片在线播放高清视频| 亚洲欧美精品自产自拍| 97超视频在线观看视频| 一边摸一边抽搐一进一小说| 国产综合懂色| 欧美+日韩+精品| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 九九爱精品视频在线观看| 国产精品乱码一区二三区的特点| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类| 51国产日韩欧美| 高清毛片免费看| 亚洲国产精品久久男人天堂| 亚洲三级黄色毛片| 久久久午夜欧美精品| 亚洲伊人久久精品综合 | 亚洲精品影视一区二区三区av| 久久精品影院6| 美女cb高潮喷水在线观看| 国产亚洲午夜精品一区二区久久 | 精品久久久噜噜| 18+在线观看网站| 亚洲av日韩在线播放| 久久婷婷人人爽人人干人人爱| av在线播放精品| 中文乱码字字幕精品一区二区三区 | 国产精品无大码| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 99视频精品全部免费 在线| 亚洲中文字幕一区二区三区有码在线看| 人人妻人人澡欧美一区二区| 色综合站精品国产| 中国国产av一级| 国产精品三级大全| 欧美色视频一区免费| 国产大屁股一区二区在线视频| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 亚洲精品亚洲一区二区| 亚州av有码| 亚洲欧美日韩高清专用| 亚洲欧美精品综合久久99| 国产探花极品一区二区| 久久久久精品久久久久真实原创| 综合色丁香网| 久久久色成人| 青春草视频在线免费观看| 欧美极品一区二区三区四区| 日韩成人伦理影院| 少妇人妻一区二区三区视频| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 99热这里只有精品一区| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| ponron亚洲| 国产男人的电影天堂91| 久99久视频精品免费| 国产淫语在线视频| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 男人舔奶头视频| 久久精品综合一区二区三区| 大香蕉97超碰在线| 国产黄片美女视频| 91av网一区二区| 国产一区亚洲一区在线观看| 国内精品宾馆在线| 欧美极品一区二区三区四区| 国产单亲对白刺激| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 国产成人91sexporn| 亚洲国产精品成人综合色| 午夜福利成人在线免费观看| 国产精品乱码一区二三区的特点| 美女大奶头视频| 九九久久精品国产亚洲av麻豆| av又黄又爽大尺度在线免费看 | 久久久久久久亚洲中文字幕| ponron亚洲| or卡值多少钱| 日韩成人av中文字幕在线观看| 久久综合国产亚洲精品| 欧美激情在线99| 精品欧美国产一区二区三| 秋霞伦理黄片| 国产精品国产三级专区第一集| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 寂寞人妻少妇视频99o| 亚洲精品乱久久久久久| 亚洲欧美中文字幕日韩二区| 十八禁国产超污无遮挡网站| ponron亚洲| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 最近中文字幕2019免费版| 亚洲中文字幕一区二区三区有码在线看| 久久久久久伊人网av| 国模一区二区三区四区视频| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品| 中文字幕av成人在线电影| 国产成人freesex在线| 日韩,欧美,国产一区二区三区 | 国产在视频线在精品| 国产综合懂色| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 精品人妻熟女av久视频| 精品久久久久久久久久久久久| 国产av不卡久久| 哪个播放器可以免费观看大片| 国内少妇人妻偷人精品xxx网站| 成人国产麻豆网| 国产私拍福利视频在线观看| 少妇高潮的动态图| 亚洲色图av天堂| 只有这里有精品99| 22中文网久久字幕| 我要搜黄色片| 黄片无遮挡物在线观看| 永久免费av网站大全| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 成人毛片a级毛片在线播放| 久久99精品国语久久久| 亚洲伊人久久精品综合 | 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 国产精品麻豆人妻色哟哟久久 | 国产一区二区在线观看日韩| 日韩人妻高清精品专区| 日韩制服骚丝袜av| 久久久久久伊人网av| 国产亚洲最大av| 91在线精品国自产拍蜜月| 69av精品久久久久久| 久久午夜福利片| 狠狠狠狠99中文字幕| 性色avwww在线观看| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 麻豆乱淫一区二区| 久久久久久久久久久丰满| 青春草视频在线免费观看| 伊人久久精品亚洲午夜| 夜夜看夜夜爽夜夜摸| av天堂中文字幕网| 免费观看在线日韩| 一本—道久久a久久精品蜜桃钙片 精品乱码久久久久久99久播 | 老司机影院成人| 亚洲国产精品专区欧美| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 纵有疾风起免费观看全集完整版 | 午夜精品国产一区二区电影 | 国产亚洲精品久久久com| 欧美另类亚洲清纯唯美| 国产免费福利视频在线观看| 日韩欧美三级三区| 午夜老司机福利剧场| 亚洲久久久久久中文字幕| 中文字幕制服av| 亚洲精品一区蜜桃| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区| 国产av码专区亚洲av| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 大话2 男鬼变身卡| 白带黄色成豆腐渣| av福利片在线观看| 日产精品乱码卡一卡2卡三| www.av在线官网国产| 国产成人精品一,二区| 在线播放无遮挡| 夜夜爽夜夜爽视频| 国产淫语在线视频| 国产精品人妻久久久久久| 国产淫语在线视频| 国产精品人妻久久久久久| 亚洲中文字幕日韩| 国产精品久久久久久精品电影| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜爱| 日韩视频在线欧美| 又粗又爽又猛毛片免费看| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 尤物成人国产欧美一区二区三区| 亚洲综合色惰| 国产免费视频播放在线视频 | 亚洲自偷自拍三级| 久久久久久伊人网av| 免费观看性生交大片5| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类| 插逼视频在线观看| 国产真实伦视频高清在线观看| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 视频中文字幕在线观看| 成人性生交大片免费视频hd| 欧美成人a在线观看| 嫩草影院新地址| 狠狠狠狠99中文字幕| 国产精品三级大全| 亚洲18禁久久av| 2021少妇久久久久久久久久久| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 一级毛片我不卡| 国语对白做爰xxxⅹ性视频网站| 欧美另类亚洲清纯唯美| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 99久久精品一区二区三区| 成人高潮视频无遮挡免费网站| 成人欧美大片| 男的添女的下面高潮视频| 亚洲精品乱久久久久久| 亚洲综合精品二区| 亚洲国产精品久久男人天堂| 九草在线视频观看| 国产淫语在线视频| 看黄色毛片网站| 免费观看精品视频网站| 两个人的视频大全免费| 午夜福利视频1000在线观看| 久久久精品大字幕| 亚洲中文字幕日韩| av视频在线观看入口| videos熟女内射| 2021天堂中文幕一二区在线观| 男插女下体视频免费在线播放| 91狼人影院| 美女国产视频在线观看| 久久这里只有精品中国| 男人狂女人下面高潮的视频| 成年版毛片免费区| 天天躁夜夜躁狠狠久久av| 欧美xxxx黑人xx丫x性爽| 亚洲人成网站在线观看播放| 成人毛片60女人毛片免费| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜爱| 麻豆一二三区av精品| 少妇高潮的动态图| 久久这里只有精品中国| 又爽又黄无遮挡网站| 中文字幕制服av| 国产大屁股一区二区在线视频| 麻豆久久精品国产亚洲av| 日本五十路高清| 亚洲怡红院男人天堂| 亚洲成人精品中文字幕电影| 寂寞人妻少妇视频99o| 欧美一区二区亚洲| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 在线天堂最新版资源| 国产乱人视频| 毛片女人毛片| 看免费成人av毛片| 精品久久久久久久久亚洲| 丝袜美腿在线中文| 99热这里只有是精品50| 级片在线观看| 国产在视频线在精品| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 久久久久久久久久成人| 日本猛色少妇xxxxx猛交久久| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 亚洲国产精品国产精品| 91午夜精品亚洲一区二区三区| 男女啪啪激烈高潮av片| 成年女人永久免费观看视频| 七月丁香在线播放| 成人鲁丝片一二三区免费| 国产老妇女一区| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 一级毛片aaaaaa免费看小| 国产极品天堂在线| 午夜免费激情av| 少妇熟女aⅴ在线视频| 国产精品三级大全| 国产精品一及| 大香蕉97超碰在线| 色播亚洲综合网| 亚洲高清免费不卡视频| 少妇的逼好多水| 午夜爱爱视频在线播放| 免费黄色在线免费观看| 国产亚洲av片在线观看秒播厂 | 夜夜看夜夜爽夜夜摸| 国产成人91sexporn| 综合色丁香网| 亚洲人成网站高清观看| 久久久午夜欧美精品| 日韩中字成人| 中文乱码字字幕精品一区二区三区 | 欧美日韩国产亚洲二区| 欧美性猛交黑人性爽| 亚洲图色成人| av在线观看视频网站免费| 国产精品一区二区性色av| 欧美zozozo另类| 99热精品在线国产| 国产精品国产高清国产av| 日本黄大片高清| 草草在线视频免费看| 精品欧美国产一区二区三| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 成人毛片60女人毛片免费| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 精品人妻熟女av久视频| 麻豆一二三区av精品| 一本—道久久a久久精品蜜桃钙片 精品乱码久久久久久99久播 | www.色视频.com| 国产成人91sexporn| 亚洲欧美日韩无卡精品| 一区二区三区免费毛片| 伊人久久精品亚洲午夜| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| 中文在线观看免费www的网站| 纵有疾风起免费观看全集完整版 | 最近2019中文字幕mv第一页| 成人性生交大片免费视频hd| 高清在线视频一区二区三区 | 婷婷色麻豆天堂久久 | av免费在线看不卡| 日韩三级伦理在线观看| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 日本与韩国留学比较| 亚洲欧洲日产国产| 免费在线观看成人毛片| 伦理电影大哥的女人| 亚洲av熟女| 欧美性感艳星| 黄色欧美视频在线观看| 日本免费在线观看一区| 男女视频在线观看网站免费| 伦精品一区二区三区| 91久久精品国产一区二区三区| 大又大粗又爽又黄少妇毛片口| 在线观看一区二区三区| 成人性生交大片免费视频hd| 一级黄色大片毛片| 99久久精品热视频| 精品人妻熟女av久视频| 日本黄大片高清| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 国产色爽女视频免费观看| 看非洲黑人一级黄片| 免费播放大片免费观看视频在线观看 | 一级毛片aaaaaa免费看小| 草草在线视频免费看| 韩国av在线不卡| 亚洲成av人片在线播放无| 国产一级毛片在线| 国产色婷婷99| 日韩欧美在线乱码| 国产午夜精品久久久久久一区二区三区| 亚洲国产精品专区欧美| 亚洲最大成人手机在线| 国产色婷婷99| 国产免费又黄又爽又色| 91av网一区二区| 国产精品熟女久久久久浪| 赤兔流量卡办理| 人人妻人人澡欧美一区二区| 日韩高清综合在线| 欧美高清成人免费视频www| 亚洲精品,欧美精品| 国产成年人精品一区二区| 欧美性感艳星| videossex国产| 免费黄网站久久成人精品| 国产精品无大码| 女的被弄到高潮叫床怎么办| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 国产精品人妻久久久久久| 国产老妇女一区| 能在线免费看毛片的网站| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 国产69精品久久久久777片| 国产三级中文精品| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 99久久精品一区二区三区| 国产精品三级大全| 久久久久久久国产电影| 免费观看精品视频网站| 久久久久久伊人网av| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看| 亚洲av日韩在线播放| 久久久久久大精品| 欧美变态另类bdsm刘玥| 国产成人91sexporn| 日韩精品有码人妻一区| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 免费大片18禁| 日韩一区二区视频免费看| 免费av观看视频| 亚洲自拍偷在线| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 国产黄色视频一区二区在线观看 | 国产精品日韩av在线免费观看| av在线老鸭窝| 国产成人精品久久久久久| 久久精品人妻少妇| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 汤姆久久久久久久影院中文字幕 | 亚洲美女搞黄在线观看| www日本黄色视频网| 99久久精品一区二区三区| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 成人欧美大片| 久久久久久久午夜电影| 成人亚洲欧美一区二区av| 一级毛片我不卡| 亚洲中文字幕日韩| 国产v大片淫在线免费观看| 久久精品夜夜夜夜夜久久蜜豆| 国产午夜精品一二区理论片| 免费看美女性在线毛片视频| 成人毛片60女人毛片免费| 久久久久九九精品影院| 久久热精品热| av免费观看日本| 国产91av在线免费观看| 黄片wwwwww| 日韩强制内射视频| 晚上一个人看的免费电影| 亚洲成人中文字幕在线播放| 国产精品久久久久久精品电影小说 | 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 国产在视频线精品| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 天美传媒精品一区二区| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 三级男女做爰猛烈吃奶摸视频| 天美传媒精品一区二区| 乱码一卡2卡4卡精品| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 久久久久久久国产电影| 亚洲色图av天堂| 九色成人免费人妻av| 亚洲精品乱码久久久v下载方式| 国产成人freesex在线| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av| 久久精品久久久久久久性| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看 | 水蜜桃什么品种好| 亚洲国产精品成人久久小说| 国产精品久久久久久久久免| 三级毛片av免费| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 真实男女啪啪啪动态图| 啦啦啦韩国在线观看视频| 老司机影院毛片| 菩萨蛮人人尽说江南好唐韦庄 | 91精品伊人久久大香线蕉|