• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Family visits to a biodiversity exhibit: An analysis of emotional responses during free visits and visits mediated by explainers

    2023-12-21 13:41:36LuisaMassarani
    科學(xué)文化(英文) 2023年2期

    Luisa Massarani

    National Institute of Public Communication of Science and Technology (INCT-CPCT),House of Oswaldo Cruz/Fiocruz,Brazil

    Graziele Scalfi

    National Institute of Public Communication of Science and Technology (INCT-CPCT),Brazil

    Rosicler Neves

    Museum of Life,House of Oswaldo Cruz/Fiocruz;National Institute of Public Communication of Science and Technology (INCT-CPCT),Brazil

    Monica Santos Dahmouche and Luiz Bento

    CECIERJ Foundation,Brazil

    Abstract In this study,we analyse the role of emotion in the context of informal science education experiences,specifically seeking to understand: (a) which emotions can be stimulated in families visiting interactive expositions,and (b) the extent to which parents/caregivers and museum explainers engage the children’s emotional expressiveness.To this end,a protocol based on the core affect model was employed to examine the visits of 10 family groups(26 individuals)who visited an exhibit on biodiversity in a science museum in Rio de Janeiro,Brazil.Five families went on free visits and the other five had their visits mediated by explainers.We observed that,in general,positive emotions,such as excitement and surprise,were among those most often expressed.Negative emotions,such as disagreement and doubt,also stood out in this study,but they were not exclusively associated with the negative aspects of the experience,and were instead linked to challenging moments that facilitated the construction of meaning by the families.The data suggest that visits mediated by explainers occasioned more frequent emotional displays than free visits,indicating the importance of explainers in families’ interactions and emotional responses.

    Keywords Science museums,emotions,family visit,exhibit explainers,conversations

    1.Introduction

    Museum experiences evoke a number of emotional responses that are capable of influencing the public’s involvement and engagement with exhibits and educational initiatives (Marandino et al.,2018).Moreover,emotions are responsible for stimulating the interest necessary for the learning process to occur and can provide memorable experiences that lead visitors to think and feel differently after their visit (Falk,2009).

    In the words of Wagensberg (2005: 11),‘emotions,not anything else,are the most important elements of museographic language’,allowing visitors to experience a museum in its totality.When proposing that communication in science museums must involve touch (hands-on),reflection (minds-on) and emotion (hearts-on),Wagensberg (2001: 346) highlights that ‘stimulus and emotion are the key words for the zero point of all knowledge’.

    Several authors have defended the impossibility of understanding why people visit museums and engage in related activities when emotions are removed from consideration (Gregory and Witcomb,2007;Smith and Campbell,2016).From this perspective,understanding how emotions affect interactions and conversations can contribute to a better understanding of visitor experiences at museums,among other things.In this study,specifically,we investigated how exhibits can influence the emotional responses of family groups and how emotions are expressed in social interactions between adults and children.In particular,we considered whether differences exist in emotional responses between free visits and visits guided by museum explainers.

    1.1 Emotional responses during the visit experience

    Emotions play an important role in visitor behaviour(Camarero-Izquierdo et al.,2009).Del Chiappa et al.(2014) analysed whether emotions influence visitor satisfaction and observed that groups demonstrating positive emotional responses reported higher satisfaction with their visit and evaluated the museum in question more favourably in terms of its attractiveness and singularity.Ruiz-Alba et al.(2019) and Palau-Saumell et al.(2016) verified that positive emotions are an indicator of visitors’ intentions to revisit a museum and recommend it to others.

    In general,the emotions that are most strongly associated with museum visits are positive ones(Falk and Gillespie,2009;Mastandrea and Maricchiolo,2016).However,this varies according to the type of museum visited.Mastandrea and Maricchiolo (2016) carried out a survey in several countries and regions(Austria,France,Hungary,Italy,New Zealand,Portugal,China’s Taiwan and the United States)on motivations,attitudes,emotions and learning processes in three types of museums:ancient art,modern art and the sciences.In this investigation,curiosity was the most frequently cited emotion in science museums,while art museums were more strongly associated with aesthetic pleasure.

    Emotions such as excitement and pleasure have been associated with better long-term learning outcomes in science(Falk and Gillespie,2009)and conservation (Ballantyne et al.,2007).Piscitelli et al.(2003),in their investigation of the interactions of young children at science museums,affirm that emotions such as excitement,anxiety,happiness,frustration,empowerment,success and pleasure are experienced as children interact with exhibits.Other studies (Evans et al.,2018;Rappolt-Schlichtmann et al.,2017) have also discussed the role of negative emotions in learning processes in museum spaces,showing that emotions such as frustration and confusion are associated with periods of high activation.The authors describe this state as one of ‘productive struggle’ in which children exhibit more concentrated and engaged behaviour,suggesting a relationship between feelings of‘struggle’ and significant learning.On this topic,Falk(2009) states that the stronger the emotional ‘value’of an experience,the higher the probability that relevant sensorial information will be committed to memory.Emotion can be interpreted in this context as a signal of the event’s significance.

    Luke et al.(2022) analysed social and emotional behaviour during the interactions of 606 4-and 5-year-old children at 14 children’s museums and 12 community playgrounds.In their analysis,the authors made use of categories centred around four socio-emotional abilities: (a) emotional expression,which registers instances of positive and negative feelings;(b) emotional regulation,which registers instances of positive and negative reactions to frustration;(c) behavioural regulation,which registers positive and negative involvement with ageappropriate activities;and (d) relational abilities with peers.Noteworthy among these results is the observation that social emotions(those demonstrated during play with other children)and emotional regulation were expressed with more negativity at science museums than at playgrounds.Luke et al.(2022)suggest that this may be a reflection of museums as spaces constructed for play that allow children to make (and fail) multiple attempts at completing tasks,becoming frustrated in the process.As such,science museums provide opportunities for children to practise these important social abilities.

    In Brazil,there have been few studies completed to date that specifically focus on investigating emotions during museum visits.Among the studies that investigate the visiting public and provide data on the role of emotions in their experiences,we highlight Leporo (2015),who analysed how children share their perceptions through spoken interactions with objects and knowledge on display at an exhibit on micro-organisms.Leporo repeatedly identified expressions of pleasure,displeasure and surprise in the children’s conversations,which were the third most frequent conversation type among those identified by Leporo during the visit,and the most frequent conversation type during the interview.Cerati (2014),who investigated family visits to a botanical garden exhibition using scientific literacy indicators,showed that the aesthetic/affective indicator was the second most prevalent indicator in the families’ speech,supporting the assertion that personal affinities with the natural environment favoured aesthetic/affective experiences over the course of exhibit visits.Freitas et al.(2018) investigated how learning conversations were presented during explainer engagement with students on a visit to an itinerant marine biology museum.This study ranked affective conversations—represented by verbal expressions of students’ emotional reactions in the categories of pleasure,displeasure,surprise and intrigue,among others—as the second most frequent conversation type.

    These studies demonstrate that emotion plays a crucial role in visitor motivation,engagement and behaviour.However,the way in which emotion affects people of different demographic backgrounds is not yet well understood.To contribute to this understanding,this study focuses on the experiences of families with children on free and mediated visits to a science exhibition.The exhibition fits into the typology of collections on the natural sciences,according to the Brazilian Institute of Museums(Instituto Brasileiro de Museus—Ibram,2011),and therefore is situated in the broad category of science museums.

    1.2 The role of social interaction in children’s emotional responses during the visit experience

    Museum visits are inherently social activities.Children,especially those of preschool age,do not customarily visit museums alone (Jensen,1994).On the contrary,they are typically accompanied by adult family members—usually parents and/or other caregivers,such as grandparents,aunts,uncles and so on.Studies have shown that,in these types of groups,parents/caregivers direct children’s attention during interactions (Bamberger and Tal,2007;DeWitt and Storksdieck,2008;Povis and Crowley,2015),guiding their behaviour (Crowley and Callanan,1998;Falk and Dierking,2000),encouraging reading (Borun et al.,1996;Massarani et al.,2021b)and contributing to learning conversations (Fender and Crowley,2007;Siegel et al.,2007).Additionally,parents/caregivers have been recognized as emotional support systems for children,facilitating conversations about memories,making personal connections and developing joint understanding of the museum experience (Hein,1998).

    Koron et al.(2022) analysed the ways in which parental instructions moulded children’s attention and focus in terms of the emotional and elemental content of paintings (e.g.,colours,lines,devices,techniques).The results showed that,in groups in which parents were instructed to ask their children specific questions,the children’s focus and attention were more directed,and the children were encouraged to think about the emotional and elemental content in more detail.During interactions without directed guidelines,children chose emotional words less frequently in their descriptions of the works of art observed.These results suggest,according to Koron et al.(2022),that parents/caregivers dampen children’s emotional experiences when they provide educational instructions.

    Another noteworthy point addresses the role that explainers can play in family interactions with children by facilitating (or not) their emotional engagement during visits.Norris and Tisdale (2013) show that explainers,when well prepared,are capable of engaging visitors with the exhibition content,themes and design elements that have the largest emotional potential.Shaby et al.(2019)also demonstrate that explainers are fundamental in the potentiation of visitors’ emotional responses.Similarly,Rodari and Merzagora (2008) argue that explainers are the only truly bidirectionally interactive museum element,as they are able to listen to visitors and respond to their reactions,questions and emotions.This ability allows explainers to adapt their responses and presentations to different audiences and contexts,making their activity dialogical.The explainer team’s conversations and behaviour are also considered to influence the cognitive and emotional interactions of visiting groups(Macías-Nestor et al.,2020).

    1.3 Theoretical background

    Employing the sociocultural perspective found in Vigotsky (1999,2004),we define mediation as a process of intervention in a relationship by intermediate elements,in which these elements potentially take the form of instruments and/or symbols(e.g.,tools,conversations,written texts or images,among other possibilities) and human action (e.g.,interaction with others).As such,both interventions function as mediators of emotions.For Vigotsky(2004),emotion is a concomitantly biological and social phenomenon that can be understood as a process that develops and transforms over time.In alignment with this perspective,we define emotion as:

    a complex and pluralistic set of interactions between objective and subjective variables,mediated by neural and hormonal systems,that motivate and organize cognition and action.Moreover,emotions include an aspect of interpretation on the part of the subject in terms of their own lived emotional state,as well as the cognitive evaluations of socio-communicative signs and expressions that,in turn,are capable of motivating behavior.(Scalfi et al.,2022)

    Therefore,we envision emotions as possessing additional characteristics beyond the biological(hormones,connections,cerebral structure) in order to study emotions in informal spaces,choosing to include relationships with other individuals and elements,the environment and lived experiences in this operational definition (Fredricks et al.,2004;Pekrun and Linnenbrink-Garcia,2012;Rowe et al.,2023).Emotions can therefore be defined for the purposes of this study as being based in two fundamental dimensions: valence and arousal (Russell,1980;Russell and Barrett,1999).These form a bidimensional‘core affect model’,the various combinations and degrees of which result in affective experiences.Here we consider valence (positive and negative experiences) and arousal (level of activation or deactivation;for example,feeling energetic versus feeling sleepy)as emotions that sustain a direct relationship with(a)epistemological aspects of learning and construction of meaning,(b)social relations,and(c)previous experiences and memories.These emotions can be expressed through behaviours(e.g.,gestures and/or facial expressions) and through conversations with other individuals and interactions with exhibition elements during the visit experience.This conception allows for a more detailed description of emotions in which any emotion can be assigned to one of four categories: weak positive emotions,strong positive emotions,weak negative emotions or strong negative emotions.

    2.Methodology

    This study is part of a larger research project being carried out by the National Institute of Public Communication of Science and Technology(INCT-CPCT),the objective of which is to understand visitor experiences at scientific-cultural spaces in Latin America (Massarani et al.,2019a,2019b,2021b).This project was approved by the Joaquim Venancio Polytechnic School Research Ethics Committee (EPSJV/Fiocruz),case number 466/2012,and under National Health Council/Ministry of Health operational norm n?001/2013.

    In this study,we investigate the emotions experienced by family groups during visits to an exhibit on biodiversity.Two questions guided this investigation: (a) Which emotions can an interactive exhibit stimulate in visiting families? (b) To what extent do parents/caregivers and explainers stimulate children’s emotional expressions? Specifically,we examined whether a difference in emotional responses exists between free visits and visits guided by museum explainers.

    This study adopted a qualitative approach,is exploratory in character,and can be categorized as a case study.These choices are justified by the fact that case studies allow for the close examination of data in situations in which the separation of a phenomenon of interest from its context is unfeasible(Yin,2014).

    2.1 The ‘Forest of the Senses’ exhibition

    The interactive ‘Forest of the Senses’ (Floresta dos Sentidos) exhibition was developed as a science communication strategy designed for 5–8-year-old children;it focuses on topical biodiversity issues,such as animal trafficking,biopiracy,resource disputes between native and invasive species,and conservation.

    The exhibit simulates a 42-square-metre nanoforest in which printed images,objects,textured materials,sound effects and scenic illumination immerse the visitor in the forest environment (Figure 1).At the entrance,there are two scenographies of trees that envelop a touchscreen computer.The exhibition associates the three-dimensional nanoforest with ‘gamified’language in which visitors are invited to participate in a search that is completed by obtaining clues.In summary,the visitors could (a) use the computer to obtain instructions in the form of clues,which are delivered by a sloth mascot;(b) enter the forest and complete an activity within the stipulated time of 180 s in order to find a clue;and (c) return to the computer to answer a question and obtain a new clue.To assist in the discovery of clues,visitors receive a kit of curiosity-stimulating,low-cost objects,such as magnifying glasses and flashlights.

    Figure 1.Forest of the Senses.(A) Computer with instructions;(B) and (C) Children performing activities;(D) An area of the ‘forest’.Credit: Peter Iliciev.

    This exhibit was produced by the Museum of Life(Museu da Vida),which is an interactive museum associated with the Oswaldo Cruz Foundation/Fiocruz and the Federal University of Rio de Janeiro,and was supported by the Rio de Janeiro Research Foundation Carlos Chagas Filho.At the time of data collection,the exhibit was located at the Science and Life Museum (Museu Ciência e Vida;MCV),which was supported by the CECIERJ Foundation (Rio de Janeiro State Centers for Science and Higher Distance Learning;Centros de Ciência e Ensino Superior a Distancia do Estada do Rio de Janeiro).The MCV is located in downtown Duque de Caxias,in the metropolitan region of the state of Rio de Janeiro;this area was assessed with one of the lowest Human Well-Being Index scores in the country (Observatório de Metrópoles,2010,2013).The Duque de Caxias municipality has been considered an area of high economic growth in recent decades,but little social benefit has resulted from this growth when analysed in terms of regional per-capita gross domestic product and/or human development indices(Camaz,2015).

    2.2 Procedures and participants

    Data collection occurred from March 2015 to June 2015 on Saturdays,which are the day of the week when the MCV has the highest number of spontaneous visitors.To participate in the study,family groups were required to have at least one adult and at least one child between 5 and 8 years old,with a maximum of six members to allow for adequate multimedia data collection.This age range was aligned with the exhibit’s target audience.Families were approached at the exhibit entrance by researchers who explained the study’s general objectives and verified whether the groups adhered to the selection criteria.

    Point-of-view cameras were deployed for observation and visit recordings (Burris,2017;Glǎveanu and Lahlou,2012;Massarani et al.,2019a,2019b,2021b);specifically,aLooxciecamera was attached to helmets worn by the participants to document their interactions with the exhibit and other individuals.One limitation of the study was that people knew they were being watched,which may have made them self-aware and led them to modify their behaviour.Glǎveanu and Lahlou(2012),Burris(2017)and other previous studies developed by our research group (Massarani et al.,2021a;Scalfi et al.,2022)show that,when recording the visit,some children tend to initially focus their attention on the camera.However,this behaviour is reduced and even disappears during the visit.The 10 participating family groups were composed of 14 adults (10 women and 4 men),12 children (9 boys and 3 girls) and one teen.Five families were accompanied by exhibit explainers,and five went on free visits.

    2.3 Analysis protocol

    Previously described procedures were adopted in this study for the analysis of emotional experiences in public scientific-cultural spaces (see Massarani et al.,2022b;Scalfi et al.,2022;Rowe et al.,2023).After collection,audiovisual data was loaded into Dedoose 8.0.23 software,and significant events and conversations were selected using a methodology similar to those proposed in Ash et al.(2007),Rowe and Kisiel (2012),Rowe et al.(2023),Massarani et al.(2022b) and Scalfi et al.(2022).Events were considered significant if they (a) had a clear beginning,middle and end;(b) contained spoken content related to the exhibit;(c) contained externalized conversations among participants or carried out by one speaker with themself or an imagined individual;and (d) contained linguistic emotional descriptors(spoken or gesticulated).

    Later,appropriate emotional descriptors were assigned to each event using the proposals found inEmotion Annotation and Representation Language(EARL) andWheel of Emotion(Plutchik,2001) as references;this allowed us to categorize positive and negative emotions (valence) and their degrees of activation or deactivation (arousal).To gain a better understanding of these emotions,the EARL and Wheel of Emotion emotional descriptors employed in this study were grouped into larger categories based on their degree of arousal(activation or deactivation) and valence (positive and negative)proposed by the core affect model (Russell,1980,2003;Russell and Barrett,1999),as shown in Figure 2.The model consists of two bipolar,independent dimensions.The valence dimension is interpreted as reflecting the aspect of emotion that offers information on well-being,for example,by attributing a good or bad meaning to an experience.Arousal,in turn,is the dimension of experience that corresponds to the mobilization or expenditure of energy;that is,it is represented as a continuum from relaxation (e.g.,sleep state) to activation (e.g.,vigilance) (Russell and Barrett,1999).In this model,the four diagonally arranged variables are not dimensions in and of themselves,but rather assist in the definition of the quadrants in the core affect model (Russell,1980).Thus,the proposed structure avoids reducing the complexity of emotions to a small number of basic emotional labels.This approach allowed for clearer visualization of differential prevalence among quadrants during the families’visits.

    Figure 2.Examples of EARL and Wheel of Emotion descriptors.Source: Author compilation;adapted from Russell(2003).

    Then,the sections of the visits containing significant events were transcribed using a detailed transcription code (Kasper and Wagner,2014;Perakyla and Sorjonen,2012).The use of the transcription code (Figure 3) contextualized dialogue with temporal and sequential elements,such as superimposed conversations,sentences combined by speakers without pausing,silence,intervals and pauses,as well as intonation,volume,variations in tone or speed,laughter,and breathing,all of which are essential to refining the emotional analysis process.

    Figure 3.Transcription conventions employed.

    3.Results and discussion

    3.1 Which emotions were evoked during the visits?

    In total,192 min of recordings were produced,of which 18.7% (35 min 59 s) were characterized as significant events.Next,18 emotional descriptors were identified.Visitors’ emotional data were grouped and sorted by quadrant according to the core affect model (Russell,2003);193 occurrences of high activation and positive valence were recorded,along with 124 occurrences of high activation and negative valence.Low activation was identified in six occurrences of negative valence and one occurrence of positive valence,as shown in Figure 4.

    In Figure 5,free and mediated visits are separated to visualize their potential similarities and differences.Free and mediated visits had similar mean durations of 17 and 16 min,respectively.Eight descriptors in the high activation,positive valence category were identified(curiosity,surprise,fun,excitement,enthusiasm,confidence,interest and joy),with a greater number occurring during mediated visits (n=113) than free visits (n=80).Seven high activation,negative valence descriptors were identified (doubt,disagreement,estrangement,frustration,irritation,tension and impatience);these also occurred more frequently in mediated visits(n=80)than in free visits(n=44).Among the low activation descriptors,two negative valence descriptors were identified (boredom and fear),which occurred more in free visits (n=5) than in mediated visits(n=1).One positive valence descriptor (enchantment) occurred only during mediated visits.

    The results demonstrate that the exposition offered positive experiences to the families,given that positive valence descriptors were more common.Excitement (n=61,n=34) and surprise(n=25,n=16) stood out more prominently in mediated than in free visits,respectively.Among the negative valence emotions experienced by the families,disagreement (n=40,n=15) and doubt(n=24,n=17) were prominent in both mediated and free visits,respectively.

    In general,mediated visits garnered more descriptors,indicating that greater stimulation was achieved in the presence of these professionals in terms of the families’ interactions and emotional responses,as discussed in Emotions present during children’s social interactions with parents/caregivers and explainers.In the following sections,we consider the data generated in this study in the light of academic literature on the subject,beginning with general reflections concerning the emotions presented during the families’ interactions with the exhibit,illustrated by example observations.

    3.2 Emotions observed during family–exhibit interactions

    From an experiential perspective,the presentation of inspiring stimuli to visitors is an inherent part of museum design.Numerous authors maintain that an exhibit’s arrangement is a potentially emotion-evoking element for visitors (Legrenzi and Troilo,2005;Norris and Tisdale,2013).Similarly,Joy and Sherry (2003) and Wang (2020) highlight the capacity that exhibit atmosphere and arrangement have to engage visitors and elicit reactions that stimulate all five senses.In this study,numerous elements of the investigated exhibit were identified as being fundamental in stimulating visitor emotions,connecting families to their senses,and creating a meaningful experience.

    The Forest of the Senses exhibit thematically implements gamified language to incentivize children to interact and have fun,and to engage the family as a whole.Lazaro (2004) explores this topic,arguing that the gaming experiences,in general,include adrenaline rushes,a sense of adventure and mental challenges.The data generated in this study demonstrate that the exhibit was exciting(activation) and evoked high levels of energy and enthusiasm.These emotions were observed during two instances in particular.The first was that of searching for clues in the forest.Searching for and getting close to answers excited children and parents/caregivers,as can be observed in Example 1.

    Example 1.The flower clue.

    In Example 1,the family (G5) is searching for a clue about bees.The father/caregiver guides the children,who rush to find the clue before the allotted time expires.They search for,select and define which flowers are the correct ones to bring back to the computer.This interaction elicited a high degree of excitement from them.The other instance in which excitement was prevalent in the families’emotional responses was during their interactions with the computer;specifically,when they chose the correct answers,as narrated by the sloth mascot(illustrated in Example 2).

    Example 2.Fruit texture clue.

    Examples 1 and 2 demonstrate how families had fun during their experiences.Lazaro (2004),who observationally analysed the emotions of 34 adults playing video games (by studying their behaviours and facial expressions),shows that the participants mentioned excitement as one of the positive emotions that they experienced,associating it with moments of adrenaline,novelty and challenge.Although excitement was positive in this context,we observed a collateral effect on family interactions in which members became more concerned with finding the correct answer before the allotted time expired than with reflecting on biodiversity issues.

    Surprise was another high activation/positive valence emotion that appeared during the families’experiences.Generally,surprise is an emotion experienced in short durations as a prologue or prelude to other emotions (Ramachandran,2012).In other words,a person often feels happy or angry shortly after feeling surprised.In this study,surprise was observed as a sudden change associated with positive emotions upon seeing objects or animals in the nanoforest,as illustrated in Example 3.

    Example 3.Visiting forest spaces.

    Mazzanti (2021) states that museums employing multisensory and multimedia approaches represent a new experiential trend that promotes dialogue between exhibits and visitors.Mazzanti discusses the importance of designing exhibits with emotional stimuli in mind in order to engage visitors and motivate them to interact with the content while also taking care to allow free visitors to choose their own emotions.This is underscored by Rappolt-Schlichtmann et al.’s(2017)insight that different visitors value different emotions during their experiences.While some prefer strongly positive stimulation and feelings,others prefer to feel challenged,which can involve negative feelings,such as confusion,difficulty and frustration.Therefore,a diverse array of emotional responses must be considered and supported during exhibit design (Mann and Cohen,2017).

    Although the Forest of the Senses exhibit incentivizes a playful approach,its content covers serious and topical biodiversity issues with the intent of engaging families in scientific discussion,particularly in the area of biodiversity.This juxtaposition inherent in the exhibit’s design accentuated the duality between positive and negative emotional expressions.Themes such as biopiracy,resource disputes between native and invasive species and concerns regarding wildlife conservation all carry the potential to cause discomfort in visitors.Hayes(2016),for example,argues that such discomfort,evoked by exhibits,allows for a meaningful dialogue to take place and a more grounded,significant understanding of the theme by the visitor.Mann and Cohen (2017) also extol the potential of exhibits that allow visitors to experience complex and negative emotions that lead to reflection.Nonetheless,the significant events analysed in this study,even those that occurred in the presence of explainers,indicate that families engaged only superficially with this type of content,given the presence of only a single example (see Example 11) of a more reflective discussion on the themes addressed.

    Another aspect that we observed was that of cognitive effort during the search for clues.Studies have demonstrated that,during play,children practise cognitive skills including language,problem solving,creativity and divergent thinking (Russ,1988;Vigotsky,1999).The game in the Forest of the Senses exhibit was fundamentally based on the use of problem-solving skills,favouring the expression of emotions that typically arise during this type of activity.For example,disagreement—a high activation/negative valence emotion characterized by conflicting ideas—was observed during interactions with the sloth at the computer station in which the family was asked to choose between several clues,as illustrated in Example 4.

    Example 4.The jackfruit clue.

    During this interaction,family members were demonstrably motivated to engage in learning opportunities.Russ (1988) states that the divergent thinking ability can play a role in helping children to consider different ideas on how to regulate their emotions.Doubt was another frequently observed emotion during gameplay.Although doubt is classified in the field of psychology as a high activation/negative valence emotion (as it typically causes confusion,resentment and defensiveness),the field of education regards it positively in teaching and learning processes (Schuck and Buchanan,2012).Dewey(1930),Sch?n (1995),and Freire and Macedo (1987)are among the authors who consider doubt to be an important process in problem solving,generating new questions and a critical perspective.In the words of Schechter (2004: 172),‘doubt is the necessary spark for the beginning of a learning process’.In Example 5,we present an episode that illustrates the presence of doubt during the families’conversations.

    Example 5.Getting to know the forest.

    Example 5 illustrates an episode in which the family was still familiarizing itself with the nanoforest space.On viewing one of the animals in the scenario,the family members displayed doubt as to whether it was a tapir (C2) or an anteater (A1).The adult,in this case,recalled the animal’s characteristics but not its name,after which the child stated that it was an anteater.This stretch demonstrates the fact that doubt is often erroneously interpreted as a lack of knowledge.Nonetheless,this episode shows that previous experiences—that is,tests of previous knowledge—contribute to learning experiences,demonstrated here by the correct identification of the animal in question.

    In summary,doubt and disagreement are not exclusively related to negative experiential aspects.As in the studies performed by Evans et al.(2018),doubt and disagreement are evidence of ‘productive struggle’.For Rappolt-Schlichtmann et al.(2017),challenging experiences,reflected in negative emotional expression,can help pave the way for collaboration and the construction of meaning.Although few museums presently encourage negative emotional responses in their visitors,pressure is mounting for museums to recognize and incorporate the fact that exhibit narratives are imbued with meaning,and that emotions,be they positive or negative,must be capitalized on to achieve inspiration,reflection,effective proposals of significant engagement,and lasting impressions (Mann and Cohen,2017;Watson,2021).

    3.3 Emotions present during children’s social interactions with parents/caregivers and explainers

    Parent/caregiver interactions with children in this study were characterized by moments of support and stimulation,in which adults incentivized children verbally and through affectionate touches and gestures (for details,see Neves,2019).Some of these interactions generated positive emotional responses;for example,when a child answered a question correctly,they might receive a kiss from their parents and give them a kiss back.Another example would be a child in the same situation receiving a pat on the back or the hands and responding with a smile.Other parental behaviours evoked more negative responses from children.For example,holding a child’s hand to guide them or control them in some way generated frustration and irritation.

    At distinct junctures,parents and caregivers attempted to guide and aid children in identifying clues.However,as they themselves were not yet familiar with the environment,the groups on free visits took indirect or confused trajectories through the forest and experienced difficulty in completing the game’s tasks,as illustrated in Example 6.

    Example 6.The bird egg clue.

    In Example 6,both positive emotions (excitement,surprise,enthusiasm,curiosity and fun) and negative emotions (impatience,disagreement and doubt) were identified.The parents/caregivers gave spatial instructions to help their children find the clues,but they were not always in agreement,demonstrating doubt and disagreement.A1 also corrected one of the children’s erroneous identifications of an animal: ‘A1: Is it the parrot?/ C1: Yea./ A1: No(.) it’s not a parrot [DISAGREEMENT]’.Such corrections occurred in other instances as well,as can be observed in Example 7,in which a child,unsure as to whether they have found the correct clue,asks,‘Is this it? [DOUBT]’,and A1 disagrees,saying,‘No (.) it’s not that o:ne (0.3) no:[DISAGREEMENT]’.

    She reminded her how she had not followed her instructions, but had eaten the first onion with all its skins, on which account her first-born had been a lindorm

    Example 7.The toad clue.

    Although the assistance received by the children while identifying clues about animals or plants appeared consistently throughout the conversations,the parents/caregivers were not always well versed on the content in question;nor did they always understand the clues and hints correctly,as Example 7 demonstrates.

    Another frequently observed aspect of family visits,as Whitaker (2016) highlights,is that some parents/caregivers are not predisposed to engage with the visit.This was not observed in our study.Even when unfamiliar with the space,the game and/or the content,the parents/caregivers generally made an effort to be involved in the children’s experiences by playing together,allowing for varied emotional expressions during these familial interactions.

    Regarding the role of explainers in the interactions of children with their families,we observed that the explainers,due to their previous knowledge of clues and familiarity with the space,provided hints,offered assistance in finding clues and oriented the children to help them complete the activities within the time limit,as Example 8 demonstrates.

    Example 8.The giraffe clue.

    In Example 8,C1 confidently names the animal when prompted after receiving the explainer’s help.In general,the explainer had the effect of expanding and amplifying emotional situations,which is also depicted in Example 9,in which the explainer encourages the child to put their hand in a box to feel a sensory clue about a fruit,as well as in Example 10,in which the explainer encourages the child to search for a clue on the ground and incentivizes them to respond correctly,which the child does with enthusiasm.

    Example 9.The fruit texture clue.

    Example 10.The leaf clue.

    Examples 9 and 10 demonstrate how the explainers facilitated the families’ emotional relationship with the exhibit to engage them as much as possible and keep them interacting.To accomplish this,they employed strategies such as the use of open questions,the direction of the children’s attention,and the stimulation of their curiosity.Some studies (see Kamolpattana et al.,2015;Massarani et al.,2022a;Pattison and Dierking,2012;Shaby et al.,2019)show that explainers create the space for deeper science-related discussions.In this study,there was only one example of a significant event that qualified as a deeper discussion of the exhibit’s themes.This discussion was about the conservation of a certain toad species (Example 11).

    In Example 11,the explainer directs and applies information about a species and its conservation;this information became meaningful to the mother,who had previous knowledge regarding the interruption of an important infrastructure project in Rio de Janeiro due to the animal in question,though she did not know exactly why.During the dialogue,the mother surprisedly states: ‘A1: I didn’t know!=[SURPRISE]’.In summary,these results demonstrate that explainers stimulate the visiting families to interact with the exhibit by employing stimulating phrases,interesting facts and assistance,consequently creating emotional stimuli as well as supporting learning experiences.

    Shaby et al.(2019) analysed the potential of explainers to stimulate visitors’ emotions during a visit to science museums,investigating the interactions between students,museum explainers and the physical exhibit environment.Their results show that explainers used emotional engagement—understood as interest,curiosity,feelings,guidance,and positive and negative affects inherent in the learning process—to engage the students with the exhibit in numerous instances.The results generated in the present study are similar to those observed by Shaby et al.(2019).

    3.4 Contrasting roles: Parents/caregivers and mediators,and children’s emotional responses

    Comparing how parents/caregivers and explainers interacted with children and stimulated their emotional responses indicates that both behaviours created pleasurable and fun moments,and provided assistance in clue discovery.During this process,positive emotions,such as excitement,surprise and fun,are notably more prevalently expressed.Although the roles of both types of adults (parents/caregivers and explainers) involve supporting the children,we suggest that this constant facilitation may also have inhibited moments of frustration or irritation when people were faced with incorrect answers or failure to find a clue.

    Adults’ specific activities while interacting with the children,which incidentally implied affective attachments,elicited greater cooperation,as parents actively engaged in play and affectionately incentivized their children with words and gestures.These behaviours were associated with moments of confidence(n=8)and joy(n=6),and occurred more frequently on free visits.

    Family intimacy also produced a greater incidence of disagreements over child behaviour,specifically in terms of moral conduct.For example,some parents/caregivers showed feelings of irritation or impatience when children did not do what they had previously agreed to do,or when they did not do what their parents/caregivers requested,as demonstrated in Example 12.

    Example 12.The lion tamarin clue.

    Example 12 demonstrates how emotions shape people’s actions.In the phrase ‘A1: C2 (.) Geez man I already asked you(.)stay close to me(.)otherwise we’re leaving [IRRITATION]’,the mother warns her son,in an irritated tone,about what might happen if he doesn’t respect their agreement.For Rowe et al.(2023),specific social institutions(such as scientific,educational or familial institutions) and culture shape ‘what’ is felt and ‘how’during interpersonal interactions.Similarly,Illouz(2007: 3) observes that emotion reveals as much‘a(chǎn)bout the self as it does about the relationship with others in a cultural context’.In Example 12,demonstrating alignment with this argument,the mother elicits a high activation/negative valence feeling(irritation)from her child that may or may not result in a behavioural change on the child’s part.Emotions are indeed,as argued by Morton (2013),central factors in individuals’ moral lives.

    The explainer can be conceptualized as fulfilling the role of a catalyst,assisting families in effectively explaining information that otherwise would have remained inaccessible to the children.As previously discussed in Emotions present during children’s social interactions with parents/caregivers and explainers,the explainers’support elicited both positive emotional responses (such as excitement and surprise) and negative emotional responses (such as disagreement and doubt)in the children.The experience of tension represents a noteworthy divergence between parent/caregiver and explainer interactions with children.While tension was typically associated with conflict during familial interactions,it was associated with fun or excitement during interactions with explainers upon encountering the unknown,as shown in Example 13.

    Example 13.The animal body sensory clue.

    In Example 13,the explainer incentivizes the children to affectively engage with the activity,think logically about the clue object,and play.Using the Vigotskyan perspective as a framework,Colliver and Veraska (2021) demonstrate that mediators contribute to the intellectual comprehension of the emotions of children at play.They suggest that explainers have the duty of helping children identify and experience their emotions in the context of cultural learning and thus facilitate their learning about culture and the value of emotions.Our study presents corroborating observations,supporting the fundamental role that explainers play in facilitating children’s construction of their own emotions.

    It is also noteworthy that explainer activity can be used to guide family groups to answer questions correctly,restricting the group’s ability to find clues and construct solutions themselves.Another important aspect is that,as Massarani et al.(2022a) indicate,there is a tendency for explainers to focus their attention on the children in the group,excluding adults from the museum experience in certain respects.As it happens,the explainer in this study also directed the most of their speech towards the children,often leaving adults in a passive role during their interactions.

    4.Final considerations

    Studies on emotions in scientific and cultural spaces offer an opportunity to construct more meaningful interpretations of visitor experiences.In this study,the objective was to understand which emotions the Forest of the Senses interactive exhibition stimulated in family groups and to what extent the parents/caregivers and museum explainers elicited these emotional responses.

    Our analysis indicates that the exhibit’s structure as a ‘treasure hunt’ game,as well as the design of the space,allowed the visitors to become immersed in a representation of forest spaces that stimulated a variety of emotional responses and incentivized families to participate actively.This suggests that the sensory dimension of the exposition was directly related to the emotional responses that occurred during visitor–exhibit interactions.Therefore,it is important to consider that the objectives,narratives and design of an exhibit affect the ways in which visitors respond emotionally.Different types of expository projects offer distinct emotional experiences to their visitors.Moreover,knowledge of visitors’ principal emotional experiences can be used to orient exhibit design to meet visitor expectations and support significant visitor engagement.

    Among the emotions observed during the family visits analysed in this study,those denoted as high activation/positive valence were the most prominent,followed by high activation/negative valence.These results suggest that,although the extant literature has documented that positive emotions are more strongly associated with museum visits,analysis of visitor satisfaction studies generally indicates that both positive and negative valence emotions experienced during the visit play an important role in the understanding of visitor engagement and learning experiences.In particular,results related to relatively high levels of negative emotional experiences reinforce the importance of understanding that negative emotional expression is a legitimate and valuable form of learning for families and an opportunity for building emotional regulation skills.

    Exhibits that deal with topical,quotidian themes that are covered in the media are of redoubled importance to socially fragile locales,such as the community investigated in this study,due to their significant potential to attract local populations to museums and promote scientific culture—a process permeated by a set of emotions that occasion the visit itself and emerge during it.

    This study generated supporting evidence regarding how parents/caregivers and explainers contribute to children’s emotional responses during social interaction.The analysis of the mediated visits,for example,revealed that museum explainers employ strategies to guide the visit experience and,as a result,elicit emotional responses from children and their families.Consequently,we confirm that the presence of educators on such visits attributes an additional dimension to emotions in which they become not only feelings but also an educational tool employed to guide the visit,stimulate curiosity and engage families on scientific issues (in this case,Brazilian biodiversity).Our observations with respect to parents/caregivers support the assertion that they experience the emotions elicited by the game together with their children;therefore,their role is better characterized as a chaperone of child conduct than as one of gameplay orientation.

    The emotional experiences of museum visitors are still far from completely understood.The identification of trends in these complex phenomena and the contextual factors associated with them are fundamental for the creation of efficient and resonant learning experiences for all of the diverse audiences who visit museums.More studies are necessary to deepen and extend the discussions on emotions in museums,as the focus on visitors is the predominant lens for studies in this area.Understanding how these emotions are expressed and how exhibits direct visitors to feel certain things are lines of investigation that will generate important contributions for the design of more meaningful and inclusive visit experiences.

    Acknowledgements

    Luisa Massarani and Graziele Scalfi thank the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development(CNPq) for awarding them a 1B Productivity grant and DTI-B scholarship,respectively.Rosicler Neves thanks the Higher Education Personnel Improvement Coordination for her doctoral scholarship.The authors are also indebted to Denilson Esposito and Cleber Serqueira for their technical support during the exhibition at the Science and Life Museum.The authors also thank the families who participated in the study.

    Declaration of conflicting interests

    The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research,authorship and/or publication of this article.

    Funding

    The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research,authorship and/or publication of this article: This study was carried out within the scope of the National Institute of Public Communication of Science and Technology,with the support of the funding agencies CNPq (no.405672/2013-6 and no.6465658/2014-8) and Rio de Janeiro Research Foundation Carlos Chagas Filho (no.E-26/200.89972018).

    ORCID iD

    Note

    1.In Portuguese,the word for goat (‘cabra’) differs in only one letter from the word for snake (‘cobra’);hence C2’s play on words.

    高清在线国产一区| 成人国产一区最新在线观看| 老鸭窝网址在线观看| 国产激情偷乱视频一区二区| 舔av片在线| 国产精品久久久久久亚洲av鲁大| 看黄色毛片网站| 亚洲人与动物交配视频| 成人午夜高清在线视频| 亚洲熟妇熟女久久| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av在线| 熟妇人妻久久中文字幕3abv| 内地一区二区视频在线| 日本黄色片子视频| 99久久无色码亚洲精品果冻| 波多野结衣高清作品| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av| 久久鲁丝午夜福利片| 老司机影院成人| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| 又粗又爽又猛毛片免费看| 成年女人看的毛片在线观看| 男女啪啪激烈高潮av片| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱 | 日韩欧美精品v在线| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 人人妻人人澡人人爽人人夜夜 | 18禁在线无遮挡免费观看视频 | 观看免费一级毛片| 一卡2卡三卡四卡精品乱码亚洲| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 中文字幕免费在线视频6| 蜜桃亚洲精品一区二区三区| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 久久久精品94久久精品| 97超视频在线观看视频| 搡老岳熟女国产| 久久精品人妻少妇| 日本成人三级电影网站| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 久久久a久久爽久久v久久| 老司机午夜福利在线观看视频| 综合色丁香网| 国产精品一及| 亚洲精品日韩在线中文字幕 | 夜夜夜夜夜久久久久| 欧美性猛交╳xxx乱大交人| 亚洲美女视频黄频| 12—13女人毛片做爰片一| 大香蕉久久网| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 色视频www国产| 精品熟女少妇av免费看| 欧美日韩乱码在线| 99久久久亚洲精品蜜臀av| 亚洲精品乱码久久久v下载方式| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图| 午夜a级毛片| 插逼视频在线观看| 看片在线看免费视频| 干丝袜人妻中文字幕| 久久久久九九精品影院| 性色avwww在线观看| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 国产精品三级大全| 97超视频在线观看视频| 亚洲av免费在线观看| 国产精品一区二区三区四区久久| 久久国产乱子免费精品| 三级国产精品欧美在线观看| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 久久久久久久亚洲中文字幕| 国产精品伦人一区二区| 久久久久性生活片| 亚洲18禁久久av| 又粗又爽又猛毛片免费看| 人妻制服诱惑在线中文字幕| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 成人无遮挡网站| 国产高清视频在线观看网站| 最好的美女福利视频网| 成人高潮视频无遮挡免费网站| 日本三级黄在线观看| 午夜精品在线福利| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 人人妻人人澡欧美一区二区| 在线观看免费视频日本深夜| 联通29元200g的流量卡| 国产精品一区二区免费欧美| 中文字幕免费在线视频6| 99久国产av精品| 欧洲精品卡2卡3卡4卡5卡区| 在线播放国产精品三级| 如何舔出高潮| 久久99热6这里只有精品| 国产麻豆成人av免费视频| 少妇人妻精品综合一区二区 | 观看免费一级毛片| 国产色爽女视频免费观看| 国产综合懂色| 亚洲第一电影网av| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 精品免费久久久久久久清纯| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 亚洲综合色惰| 色吧在线观看| 国产av不卡久久| 日韩强制内射视频| 日日干狠狠操夜夜爽| 亚洲最大成人av| 舔av片在线| 99国产精品一区二区蜜桃av| 亚洲av免费在线观看| 久久精品国产亚洲av香蕉五月| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 亚洲精品亚洲一区二区| 亚洲成人精品中文字幕电影| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 悠悠久久av| 欧美+亚洲+日韩+国产| 亚洲乱码一区二区免费版| 亚洲欧美成人综合另类久久久 | 国产精品国产三级国产av玫瑰| 五月伊人婷婷丁香| 国产成人91sexporn| eeuss影院久久| 久久婷婷人人爽人人干人人爱| 亚洲成人中文字幕在线播放| 亚洲成人精品中文字幕电影| 日本与韩国留学比较| 亚洲精品亚洲一区二区| 国内少妇人妻偷人精品xxx网站| 人人妻人人澡人人爽人人夜夜 | 欧美xxxx黑人xx丫x性爽| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 久久九九热精品免费| 欧美激情在线99| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清| 国产高清视频在线观看网站| 亚洲熟妇熟女久久| 免费看美女性在线毛片视频| 久久久精品94久久精品| 深夜精品福利| 在线免费观看的www视频| 国产 一区精品| 日日啪夜夜撸| 男女啪啪激烈高潮av片| 1000部很黄的大片| 亚洲成人精品中文字幕电影| 深夜a级毛片| 又粗又爽又猛毛片免费看| av国产免费在线观看| 亚洲综合色惰| 久久久久久久午夜电影| 插阴视频在线观看视频| 国产欧美日韩一区二区精品| 日本爱情动作片www.在线观看 | 99热全是精品| 晚上一个人看的免费电影| 欧美极品一区二区三区四区| 国产精品久久电影中文字幕| 99久国产av精品| 免费黄网站久久成人精品| 中文在线观看免费www的网站| 蜜桃久久精品国产亚洲av| 中文资源天堂在线| 精品一区二区三区人妻视频| 精华霜和精华液先用哪个| 国产高清视频在线观看网站| 观看美女的网站| 嫩草影院入口| 午夜爱爱视频在线播放| 国产精品伦人一区二区| av在线老鸭窝| 色5月婷婷丁香| 日本熟妇午夜| av.在线天堂| 又粗又爽又猛毛片免费看| 亚洲av中文av极速乱| av国产免费在线观看| 亚洲精品成人久久久久久| 国产精品久久久久久亚洲av鲁大| .国产精品久久| 91久久精品电影网| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 国产aⅴ精品一区二区三区波| 午夜久久久久精精品| 老师上课跳d突然被开到最大视频| 国产精品永久免费网站| 免费黄网站久久成人精品| 在线播放国产精品三级| 老师上课跳d突然被开到最大视频| 午夜福利成人在线免费观看| 国产91av在线免费观看| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆 | 伦精品一区二区三区| 亚洲人成网站在线观看播放| 少妇的逼水好多| 成人一区二区视频在线观看| 久久久欧美国产精品| 中文资源天堂在线| 国产探花极品一区二区| 一个人看的www免费观看视频| 三级男女做爰猛烈吃奶摸视频| 日本色播在线视频| 日本精品一区二区三区蜜桃| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线| 国产私拍福利视频在线观看| 97超视频在线观看视频| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜爱| 人妻少妇偷人精品九色| 中出人妻视频一区二区| 国内久久婷婷六月综合欲色啪| 99热精品在线国产| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 亚洲乱码一区二区免费版| 日韩中字成人| 国产一区二区亚洲精品在线观看| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图| 97热精品久久久久久| 国产精品美女特级片免费视频播放器| 国产日本99.免费观看| 直男gayav资源| 日韩一区二区视频免费看| 人妻制服诱惑在线中文字幕| 国产熟女欧美一区二区| 亚洲真实伦在线观看| 男女下面进入的视频免费午夜| 色噜噜av男人的天堂激情| 亚洲内射少妇av| 69人妻影院| 好男人在线观看高清免费视频| 最近2019中文字幕mv第一页| 欧美成人免费av一区二区三区| 日本一本二区三区精品| 人妻制服诱惑在线中文字幕| 在线天堂最新版资源| 两性午夜刺激爽爽歪歪视频在线观看| 免费人成在线观看视频色| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| av在线蜜桃| 久久99热这里只有精品18| 亚洲最大成人av| 国产成人aa在线观看| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆 | 在线国产一区二区在线| 两性午夜刺激爽爽歪歪视频在线观看| 在线观看免费视频日本深夜| 亚洲一区二区三区色噜噜| 精品人妻视频免费看| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 男插女下体视频免费在线播放| 国产人妻一区二区三区在| 午夜久久久久精精品| 人人妻人人澡人人爽人人夜夜 | 真实男女啪啪啪动态图| 小说图片视频综合网站| 日本五十路高清| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 麻豆国产97在线/欧美| 春色校园在线视频观看| 中文字幕免费在线视频6| 欧美+日韩+精品| 蜜臀久久99精品久久宅男| 黑人高潮一二区| 国模一区二区三区四区视频| 一级av片app| 搞女人的毛片| 国产三级在线视频| www.色视频.com| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 国产精华一区二区三区| 免费在线观看成人毛片| 日本一本二区三区精品| 国产精品一区二区三区四区免费观看 | 久久草成人影院| 国产91av在线免费观看| 日韩欧美国产在线观看| 男女那种视频在线观看| 亚洲欧美成人精品一区二区| 欧美日韩综合久久久久久| 国产乱人视频| eeuss影院久久| av免费在线看不卡| 日韩三级伦理在线观看| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 亚洲va在线va天堂va国产| 久久精品夜色国产| 亚洲第一电影网av| 久久久久久久久久成人| 国产精品久久久久久久电影| 又粗又爽又猛毛片免费看| 亚洲综合色惰| 两个人视频免费观看高清| av女优亚洲男人天堂| 久久人妻av系列| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 十八禁国产超污无遮挡网站| 亚洲成av人片在线播放无| 在线天堂最新版资源| 深夜a级毛片| 97在线视频观看| 性插视频无遮挡在线免费观看| 一区二区三区四区激情视频 | 久久亚洲精品不卡| 久久6这里有精品| 国产视频内射| 免费看日本二区| 看片在线看免费视频| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 欧美+亚洲+日韩+国产| 天堂动漫精品| 99久久无色码亚洲精品果冻| 如何舔出高潮| 91狼人影院| 国产 一区精品| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| av视频在线观看入口| 亚洲成a人片在线一区二区| 亚洲精品乱码久久久v下载方式| 99精品在免费线老司机午夜| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱 | 国产精品人妻久久久久久| av在线蜜桃| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| 国产亚洲精品久久久com| 精品久久久久久成人av| 日韩一区二区视频免费看| 精品熟女少妇av免费看| 香蕉av资源在线| 中国美女看黄片| 成年女人毛片免费观看观看9| 亚洲人成网站高清观看| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 在线观看av片永久免费下载| 99在线人妻在线中文字幕| 99riav亚洲国产免费| 成人二区视频| 伊人久久精品亚洲午夜| 亚洲成人精品中文字幕电影| 国产精品爽爽va在线观看网站| 高清毛片免费观看视频网站| 在线播放国产精品三级| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 精品一区二区三区人妻视频| 五月玫瑰六月丁香| 午夜福利视频1000在线观看| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 搡老岳熟女国产| 夜夜爽天天搞| 91精品国产九色| 精品少妇黑人巨大在线播放 | 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 亚洲美女搞黄在线观看 | 国产精品人妻久久久影院| 一进一出抽搐动态| 免费人成在线观看视频色| 国产精品综合久久久久久久免费| 69av精品久久久久久| 亚洲精品国产av成人精品 | 看十八女毛片水多多多| 亚洲美女视频黄频| 在现免费观看毛片| 精品久久久噜噜| 日韩人妻高清精品专区| 国产精品女同一区二区软件| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 国产高清三级在线| 精品一区二区三区人妻视频| 变态另类丝袜制服| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| 少妇丰满av| 亚洲精品国产av成人精品 | 国产乱人视频| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 高清毛片免费观看视频网站| 午夜精品一区二区三区免费看| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 亚洲成人中文字幕在线播放| 国产精品久久久久久av不卡| 色尼玛亚洲综合影院| 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 亚洲精品国产成人久久av| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 黄色一级大片看看| 国产成人91sexporn| 久久久久国内视频| 成年免费大片在线观看| 国产成人freesex在线 | 久久久久九九精品影院| 亚洲最大成人手机在线| 乱系列少妇在线播放| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 国产精华一区二区三区| 国产乱人偷精品视频| 高清午夜精品一区二区三区 | 亚洲精品一卡2卡三卡4卡5卡| 国产真实伦视频高清在线观看| 露出奶头的视频| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 欧美激情国产日韩精品一区| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av在线| 精品久久久久久久久av| 桃色一区二区三区在线观看| 国产午夜精品论理片| 国产不卡一卡二| 丝袜美腿在线中文| 成人综合一区亚洲| 午夜影院日韩av| 亚洲av第一区精品v没综合| 亚洲欧美精品综合久久99| 一级黄色大片毛片| 十八禁网站免费在线| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频| 精品久久久久久成人av| 免费无遮挡裸体视频| 天堂√8在线中文| 亚洲国产精品成人久久小说 | 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 大香蕉久久网| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 蜜桃亚洲精品一区二区三区| 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 九九久久精品国产亚洲av麻豆| 国产黄色小视频在线观看| 久久草成人影院| 中文资源天堂在线| eeuss影院久久| 性插视频无遮挡在线免费观看| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区| 黄色日韩在线| 亚洲av免费在线观看| av女优亚洲男人天堂| 91在线观看av| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av在线| 我要看日韩黄色一级片| 极品教师在线视频| 22中文网久久字幕| 国产精品久久视频播放| 亚洲国产精品久久男人天堂| 日韩欧美 国产精品| 亚洲av一区综合| 真实男女啪啪啪动态图| 亚洲激情五月婷婷啪啪| 男女视频在线观看网站免费| 国产探花在线观看一区二区| 婷婷色综合大香蕉| 噜噜噜噜噜久久久久久91| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 国产aⅴ精品一区二区三区波| 成人av一区二区三区在线看| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 免费在线观看成人毛片| 日韩国内少妇激情av| 久久人人精品亚洲av| 91在线精品国自产拍蜜月| 欧美国产日韩亚洲一区| 国产高清视频在线播放一区| 精品乱码久久久久久99久播| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 一级毛片aaaaaa免费看小| 亚洲乱码一区二区免费版| 亚洲婷婷狠狠爱综合网| 啦啦啦观看免费观看视频高清| 1000部很黄的大片| 日韩欧美免费精品| 国产av一区在线观看免费| 麻豆久久精品国产亚洲av| 欧美在线一区亚洲| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| 国产精品av视频在线免费观看| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 青春草视频在线免费观看| 午夜福利18| 春色校园在线视频观看| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| 露出奶头的视频| 又爽又黄a免费视频| 男人舔奶头视频| 国模一区二区三区四区视频| 欧美激情国产日韩精品一区| 天堂√8在线中文| 男人狂女人下面高潮的视频| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| 免费看美女性在线毛片视频| 赤兔流量卡办理| 精品午夜福利在线看| 亚洲国产精品国产精品| 男人舔奶头视频| 亚洲国产精品成人综合色| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 麻豆成人午夜福利视频| 欧美激情久久久久久爽电影| 床上黄色一级片| 欧洲精品卡2卡3卡4卡5卡区| 老熟妇乱子伦视频在线观看| 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 99久久精品国产国产毛片| 亚洲无线观看免费| 国产精品国产三级国产av玫瑰| 在线免费十八禁| 老熟妇乱子伦视频在线观看| 日韩三级伦理在线观看| 国产乱人偷精品视频| 一区福利在线观看| 99久国产av精品| 国产三级中文精品| 国产成人影院久久av| .国产精品久久| 岛国在线免费视频观看| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 亚洲av成人av| a级毛色黄片| 老熟妇仑乱视频hdxx| 男女视频在线观看网站免费| 大香蕉久久网| 免费观看人在逋| 小蜜桃在线观看免费完整版高清| eeuss影院久久| 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看| 最近视频中文字幕2019在线8| 欧美另类亚洲清纯唯美| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 久久午夜福利片| 久久久久久久久久成人| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 中出人妻视频一区二区| 麻豆久久精品国产亚洲av| 嫩草影院入口| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 变态另类丝袜制服| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 青春草视频在线免费观看| 日韩精品有码人妻一区| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 搡老岳熟女国产| 97超级碰碰碰精品色视频在线观看| 亚洲一区二区三区色噜噜| 欧美成人a在线观看| 日韩国内少妇激情av| 五月玫瑰六月丁香| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆 | av中文乱码字幕在线| 99久国产av精品国产电影| 国产视频一区二区在线看| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 黄色欧美视频在线观看| 久久精品国产亚洲av香蕉五月| 久久久国产成人免费| 伦理电影大哥的女人| 精品久久国产蜜桃| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 男女下面进入的视频免费午夜| 亚洲最大成人手机在线| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 欧美性感艳星| 日韩欧美在线乱码| 变态另类丝袜制服| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 男人舔女人下体高潮全视频| 男女视频在线观看网站免费| 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 国产精品爽爽va在线观看网站| 成熟少妇高潮喷水视频| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| 男女之事视频高清在线观看| 久久精品国产亚洲av天美| 最好的美女福利视频网| av女优亚洲男人天堂| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看 | 老女人水多毛片| 国产伦精品一区二区三区视频9| 久久人人爽人人片av| 97碰自拍视频| 国产亚洲精品久久久com| 亚洲欧美精品自产自拍| 亚洲欧美日韩高清专用| 久久热精品热| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产| 欧美区成人在线视频| 久久久久九九精品影院| 久久久久国产精品人妻aⅴ院| 免费高清视频大片| 午夜精品国产一区二区电影 | 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 一级毛片aaaaaa免费看小| 国产精品三级大全| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看 | 尤物成人国产欧美一区二区三区| 成人国产麻豆网| 国内久久婷婷六月综合欲色啪| 深夜a级毛片| 一级av片app| 午夜影院日韩av| 亚洲国产精品成人久久小说 | 久久久久久久久中文| 99在线人妻在线中文字幕| 尤物成人国产欧美一区二区三区| 蜜臀久久99精品久久宅男| 精品无人区乱码1区二区| 小说图片视频综合网站| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区| 舔av片在线| 国产成人一区二区在线| 嫩草影院新地址|