• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Partial Masquerading and Background Matching in Two Asian Box Turtle Species (Cuora spp.)

    2022-09-27 03:10:52RongpingBUFanrongXIAOGeorgeLOVELLJichaoWANGandHaitaoSHI
    Asian Herpetological Research 2022年3期

    Rongping BU ,Fanrong XIAO* ,P.George LOVELL ,Jichao WANG and Haitao SHI*

    1 Ministry of Education Key Laboratory for Ecology of Tropical Islands, Key Laboratory of Tropical Animal and Plant Ecology of Hainan Province,College of Life Sciences,Hainan Normal University,Haikou 571158,Hainan,China

    2 Division of Psychology and Forensic Sciences,School of Applied Sciences,Abertay University,Dundee DD1 1HG,UK

    Abstract Animals living in heterogeneous natural environments adopt different camouflage strategies against different backgrounds,and behavioral adaptation is crucial for their survival.However,studies of camouflage strategies have not always quantified the effect of multiple strategies used together.In the present study,we used a human visual model to quantify similarities in color and shape between the carapace patterns of two Cuora species and their preferred habitats.Our results showed that the color of the middle stripe on the carapace of Cuora galbinifrons(Indochinese box turtle) was significantly similar to the color of their preferred substrates.Meanwhile,the middle stripe on the carapace of C.mouhotii (keeled box turtle) contrasted more with their preferred substrates,and the side stripe matched most closely with the environment.Furthermore,the carapace side stripe of C.galbinifrons and the carapace middle stripe of C.mouhotii highly contrasted with their preferred substrates.We quantified the similarity in shape between the highcontrast stripes of both Cuora species and leaves from their habitats.The carapace middle stripe of C.mouhotii was most similar in shape to leaves from the broadleaves substrate,and the carapace side stripe of C.galbinifrons was the most similar in shape to leaves from the bamboo-leaves substrate.We determined that these species adopt partial masquerading when their entire carapace is exposed and partially match their background when they semi-cover themselves in leaf litter.To the best of our knowledge,this is the first study to demonstrate that partial masquerading and background matching improve the camouflage effect of Asian box turtles in their preferred habitats.This is a novel study focusing on the influence of the shape and color of individual carapace segments on reducing detectability and recognition.

    Keywords camouflage,Cuora galbinifrons,Cuora mouhotii,patterns,protective coloration

    1.Introduction

    Camouflage,the phenomenon in which the characteristics of an object or organism are arranged to prevent detection or recognition by an observer,is a common anti-predator strategy employed by many animals (Endler,1978;Merilaita and Lind,2005;Stevens and Merilaita,2011) and is crucial for survival in many species (Cott,1940;Ruxtonet al.,2004).Background matching,an important camouflaging strategy that helps an animal reduce detection by predators (Stevenset al.,2015;Stevens and Merilaita,2011),refers to the matching of the animals’ color,lightness,orientation,and pattern with their background (Kanget al.,2012;Marshallet al.,2016;Trosciankoet al.,2016).Masquerading animals appear to closely resemble inedible or inanimate objects,such as twigs,leaves,stones,or bird droppings (Endler,1981;Skelhornet al.,2010a),preventing predators from detecting or recognizing them.

    However,it is difficult to determine whether a predator has detected and misidentified a camouflaged individual (due to masquerade) or entirely failed to detect the prey individual (due to background matching and disruptive coloration) (Skelhornet al.,2010a;Skelhorn and Ruxton,2011).Thus,considering these two camouflage strategies separately is neither a requirement nor a realistic interpretation of the real world (Stevenset al.,2011).Prey animals can exploit background matching and masquerading simultaneously to increase the anti-predation benefits of the strategies.Under these circumstances,the benefit and impact of each strategy may differ,as would the overall effect of the combined strategies (Hallet al.,2017).

    To maximize the effectiveness of the camouflage,animals likely choose microhabitats that closely complement their color and shape (Lovellet al.,2013;Priceet al.,2019;Stevens and Ruxton,2018).For example,the common potoo bird (Nyctibius griseus) selects perching sites that enhance its background matching and masquerading effects (Cestariet al.,2018).Furthermore,many animals decorate themselves or modify their habitats to better conceal themselves (Gómezet al.,2018;Marshallet al.,2016;Suzuki and Sakurai,2015).

    Camouflage is likely the main defense mechanism for turtles,as they are slow moving and relatively easy to catch (Hallet al.,2017).Thus,camouflage is an important anti-predation mechanism that improves their fitness.Some tortoise species have been described as having a likeness to stones (Willemsen and Hailey,2003),and evidence suggests that desert tortoises(Gopherus agassizii) select habitats that contain stones similar in size to their own body size,which can reduce their detectability(Nafuset al.,2015).There have been studies of color variation induced in turtles based on their habitats (McGaugh,2008;Roweet al.,2014).However,few studies have explored color and shape similarities between turtles and habitats,or their models(see Xiaoet al.,2016,2021).Moreover,these studies examined the animal as a whole rather than examining whether certain body parts of the animal impact camouflage.

    In our long-term field studies on Indochinese box turtles(C.galbinifrons) and keeled box turtles (C.mouhotii) (Buet al.,2020;Xiaoet al.,2017),we discovered that the carapace roof of the former exhibits a bamboo-leaved pattern,specifically two yellow stripes (Figure 1A),while the carapace roof of the latter exhibits a broad-leaved pattern,created by two keels (Figure 1B).These patterns facilitate disruptive camouflage in their preferred substrates (Buet al.,2020).Both box turtle species bury their bodies in leaf litter when they are still,leaving the middle stripe exposed,which appears to match the surrounding substrate (Figure 2).Both species face predators in the wild,mainly mammals,such as the masked palm civet (Paguma larvata) and wild boar (Sus scrofa) (Xiaoet al.,2020).

    Figure 1 The carapace of an Indochinese box turtle (left) and a keeled box turtle (right).A indicates the yellow stripe of the Indochinese box turtle,and B is the middle stripe of the keeled box turtle.

    Figure 2 Both turtle species bury themselves in leaf litter,leaving the middle stripe exposed.Arrows indicate Indochinese box turtle(Cuora galbinifrons) (A) and keeled box turtle (Cuora mouhotii) (B).

    Figure 3 Four kinds of backgrounds: A is an example of a broad-leaves substrate,B is a bare ground substrate,C is a bamboo-leaves substrate,and D is a grass substrate.

    In the present study,we predicted that the middle stripes on the carapace roof of these twoCuoraspecies had evolved to match their preferred habitat,masquerading as leaves and serving an anti-predation purpose.To the best of our knowledge,this study is the first to focus on the anti-predation benefits associated with partial stripe color and shape variation in box turtles.

    2.Materials and Methods

    2.1.Ethics statementFieldwork was carried out in strict accordance with the guidelines of the Animal Research Ethics Committee of Hainan Provincial Education Centre for Ecology and Environment,Hainan Normal University(HNECEE-2019-006),which conforms to the Law of the People’s Republic of China.No turtles were euthanized for this study,and none were injured while in the traps.All turtles were released in the original capture area at the end of the experiment.

    2.2.Microhabitat selection,photography,and calibrationThis study was conducted from September to December 2019.Field work was conducted in a rainforest in the Diaoluoshan National Nature Reserve (18°43′53′N,109°52′10′E)in Hainan Province,People’s Republic of China.In total,12 adult Indochinese box turtles (carapace length ≥120 mm;seven females and five males) and 12 adult keeled box turtles(carapace length ≥140 mm;eight females and four males)from the reserve were used for this study.The turtles did not exhibit obvious sexual dimorphism in terms of their carapace color.The preferred microhabitats of the keeled box turtle were broad leaves (Figure 3A) and bare ground (Figure 3B),and the preferred microhabitats of the Indochinese box turtle were broad leaves (Figure 3A) and bamboo leaves (Figure 3C),as determined using radio tracking data that recorded the turtles’ movements from April 2015 to February 2016 (for detailed methodology see Xiaoet al.,2017).Grass microhabitats were not favored by either species and served as the control(Figure 3D).Turtles and substrates were photographed using a Canon EOS 550D camera with a Canon lens (EF-S24-70 mm f/2.8,8.2 megapixels).Photography in the field took place between 9:00 a.m.and 4:00 p.m.,when natural light allowed turtles to be seen.A black umbrella was used to counter the direction of light entry and ensure uniform illumination of the photos.A tripod was used to ensure that the lens was perpendicular and placed at the same distance from the substrates,so that a constant image scale could be maintained.We collected 20 images of each substrate type and 24 images of turtles,totaling 104 images.

    Images were saved in an uncompressed RAW format(Stevenset al.,2007) for subsequent color measurements and analyses.The images were calibrated using the “Generate multispectral image” function in ImageJ 1.53e (Wayne Rasband and contributors,National Institutes of Health,USA)(Troscianko and Stevens,2015).The images were ordered linearly based on radiance and standardized to control for differing light conditions by using an 18% standard gray card (Mennon,China,14.5 cm×9.5 cm) (Stevenset al.,2007;Troscianko and Stevens,2015).

    2.3.Color measurement and calculationStandardized images were used to quantify the color contrast between the turtles’ carapace and the substrates,following previously published protocols for images and survey methods (Buet al.,2020).A model of human discrimination was produced using MICA toolbox in ImageJ 1.53e (Wayne Rasband and contributors,National Institutes of Health,USA) (Troscianko and Stevens,2015;Priceet al.,2019;van den Berget al.,2020).As visual searching is an important method that humans use to find turtles (Nafuset al.,2015;Xiaoet al.,2016),we used human eyesight to evaluate whether color differences are distinguishable.According to the RNL model (Receptor Noise Limited Model) (Vorobyev and Osorio,1998),the chromatic just-noticeable difference (CJND) and luminance just-noticeable difference (LJND) determine whether two objects can be differentiated,with increasing values suggesting decreased color similarity.In the present study,we evaluated the color contrast between both species and their substrates by eye.We used the elm 52-mm human D65 calibration profile as a cone model.We compared the entire carapace,the middle stripe,the side stripes(Figure 4),and five randomly selected areas of each substrate with the substrate color.In addition,we randomly selected three to five leaves from each substrate to represent the leaf color.

    Figure 4 Color and structure segmentation diagram for a Cuora galbinifrons individual,with a carapace length of 176.64 mm,including CGM=middle stripe and CGS=side stripe,and a Cuora mouhotii individual,with a carapace length of 161.78 mm,with CMM=middle stripe and CMS=side stripe.

    We converted the standard multispectral image to a cone catch,used the human visual system in sunlight (D65),and set the Weber coefficient as 0.02 (Wyszecki and Stiles,1982)to analyze the chromaticity and luminance of each image(Troscianko and Stevens,2015).The luminance channel was set to “l(fā)um” for the human vision model.We used pixels with a starting size of 2 and an ending size of 512.At each step,the pixels were multiplied by 1.414,to represent exponential growth.Luminance measurements ranged from 0 to 65,535 (Nokelainenet al.,2020).These calculations were performed by a third party who was unaware of the aim of the study.The values of CJND ranged from three to eight,depending on the image content and on each individual subject (Linet al.,2015).

    2.4.Shape measurement and similarity calculationsStandardized images were imported into ImageJ 1.53e and converted into TIFF format.Scales with actual measurement values for area (A) and perimeter (P) were selected.In addition,four further descriptors were used: aspect ratio (AR),solidity (S),circularity (C),and roundness (R).These six shape descriptors were used to quantify the shapes of turtles’ carapaces and the leaves in the substrates.

    According to the similarity algorithm (Samalet al.,2004),descriptor similarity is obtained by standardizing the differences between the prey and the model,as follows:

    where sim (X,Y) is the similarity between objectsXandY;dis(X,Y) represents the Euclidean distance between objectsXandY;andUis the maximum distance between the corresponding features of the two elements.

    The weighted average of similarity between shape descriptorsXandYwas calculated as follows:

    The shape similarity between turtles’ carapace and substrate leaves was then calculated based on Equations 2 and 3,withq=6 representing the six shape descriptors.The values of shape similarity ranged from 0-1,with increasing values equating to higher misidentification rates by human predators (Xiaoet al.,2021).We calculated shape similarity for the entire carapace,the middle stripe,and the side stripes (Figure 4).

    2.5.Statistical analysisAll statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics for Windows,version 18.0 (SPSS Inc.,Chicago,IL,USA),and all data are expressed as mean ± SE.A Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to test the normality of CJND,LJND,and the shape similarity data before analysis.Differences in CJND,LJND,and shape between each species,and similarity differences between each substrate type,were analyzed using the independent samples t-test when data were normally distributed;the rest of the data were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U-tests.The significance level was set atP<0.05.

    3.Results

    3.1.Color contrast between turtles and substratesThe CJND of the middle stripe and entire carapace ofC.galbinifronsindividuals were significantly lower in the turtles’ preferred substrates (broad leaves and bamboo leaves) than in the grass substrate.Their side stripe had the lowest contrast with the bamboo-leaves substrate and contrasted more with the broadleaves substrate than with the grass substrate.The CJND of the entire carapace ofC.mouhotiiindividuals was higher on bare ground than on the grass substrate;however,it did not differ significantly between the grass and broad-leaves substrates.The CJND of their middle stripe was significantly higher in their preferred substrates than in the grass substrate,whereas the CJND of their side stripe was significantly lower in their preferred substrates than in the grass substrate (Figure 5,Tables 1-2).

    Table 1 Chromatic just-noticeable difference (CJND) and luminance just-noticeable difference (LJND) between both turtle species and the substrates.

    Table 2 Differences in chromatic just-noticeable difference (CJND) and luminance just-noticeable difference (LJND) between stripes of both turtle species and substrates.

    Figure 5 Vision modeling results of chromatic and luminance contrasts.The top panel shows the chromatic contrast values and the bottom panel shows the luminance contrast values.Different lowercase letters above the columns represent significant differences between each species’ carapace pattern on different substrates (t-tests or Mann-Whitney U-tests;P<0.05),with letter “o” indicating outliers in the data.

    The LJND of the middle stripe,side stripe,and entire carapace ofC.galbinifronsindividuals had the lowest contrast with the bamboo-leaves substrate;however,the LJND of their side stripe was higher in the broad-leaves substrate than it was in the grass substrate.The LJND of the middle stripe and the entire carapace ofC.mouhotiiindividuals was significantly higher in their preferred substrates than in the grass substrate.However,the LJND of their side stripe was lowest in the broadleaves substrate and greater on bare ground than in the grass substrate (Figure 5,Tables 1-2).

    3.2.Shape similarity between turtles and leavesThe entire carapace was the body part that had the lowest similarity between its patterns and the leaves from the three substrate types for both species.ForC.galbinifrons, the middle stripe had the highest similarity with leaves from the broad-leaves substrate,whereas the side stripe had the highest similarity with leaves from the bamboo-leaves substrate.ForC.mouhotii,all carapace parts had the highest shape similarity with leaves from the broad-leaves substrate (Figure 6,Tables 3-4).

    Figure 6 Shape similarity between each species and the leaves in the substrates.Different lowercase letters above the columns represent significant differences between each species’ carapace pattern on different substrates (t-tests or Mann-Whitney U-tests;P<0.05),with hollow cycle “o” indicating outliers in the data.

    Table 3 Average shape similarity between both turtle species and leaves in the substrates.

    Table 4 Shape similarity between stripes of both turtle species and leaves in different substrates.

    3.3.Color contrast between the contrast stripe of both turtle species and leavesThe entire carapace of each turtle species was the body part with the lowest similarity in shape to the leaves of each substrate,meaning that the shape of the entire carapace differs greatly from that of the leaves.Moreover,the carapaces of both species were divided by color or structure,making it an unsuitable feature to analyze color similarity as a whole.It was thus unnecessary to analyze any color similarity between the turtles’ entire carapace and the leaves.

    The side stripe ofC.galbinifronshad a lower CJND with leaves from the bamboo-leaves substrate than with leaves from the broad-leaves and grass substrates.Furthermore,there was no significant difference between the CJND of its side stripe with the grass substrate and that of the broad-leaves substrate.ForC.mouhotii,the CJND between its middle stripe and leaves from its preferred substrates was higher than that between its middle stripe and the grass substrate.

    ForC.galbinifrons,there were no significant differences in LJND between its side stripes and the leaves from grass and broad-leaves substrates;however,the highest LJND value was observed between its side stripes and leaves from the bambooleaves substrate.ForC.mouhotii,the highest LJND occurred between its middle stripe and leaves from the broad-leaves substrate (Figure 7,Tables 5-6).

    Table 6 Differences in chromatic just-noticeable difference (CJND) and luminance just-noticeable difference (LJND) between contrast stripes in each species and leaves in different substrates.

    Figure 7 Vision modeling results of chromatic and luminance contrast between each species’ stripe and leaves in the substrates.The top panel shows the chromatic just-noticeable difference (CJND) and luminance just-noticeable difference (LJND) values between the side stripe of C.galbinifrons and leaves.The bottom panel shows the CJND and LJND values between the middle stripe of C.mouhotii and leaves.Different lowercase letters above the columns represent significant differences between each species’ carapace pattern on different substrates (t-tests or Mann-Whitney U-tests;P<0.05),with hollow cycle “o” indicating outliers in the data.

    Table 5 Chromatic just-noticeable difference (CJND) and luminance just-noticeable difference (LJND) between both species’ contrast stripes and leaves in the substrate.

    4.Discussion

    We assessed the camouflage abilities of twoCuoraspecies in broad-leaves,bamboo-leaves,bare ground,and grass substrates;in all substrates,the CJND was within the human visual discrimination threshold.The color contrast analysis showed that the side stripe ofC.mouhotiiindividuals on their preferred substrates had the lowest CJND and LJND,whereas their middle stripe had the highest CJND and LJND.Conversely,the middle stripe ofC.galbinifronsindividuals on their preferred substrates had the lowest CJND and LJND,whereas their side stripe had the highest CJND and LJND.LJND is not only affected by color but also by the distance from the observer.The part closer to the observer is brighter,while the part further away is darker (Nakayama and Silverman,1986),resulting in a greater LJND in the stripes in different planes and the substrates.The high contrast between the middle and side stripes of each species also indicates that the partial stripes are highlighted in the substrate,which increases the disruptive camouflage for each species in their preferred substrate (Buet al.,2020).

    Our previous long-term studies found that bothCuoraspecies prefer deciduous leaf substrates (Lian,2009;Wang,2007;Xiaoet al.,2017) and habitually semi-cover themselves in leaf litter.The middle stripe ofC.galbinifronsmatched the preferred substrates;however,the greater color difference observed between the middle stripes ofC.mouhotiiand the substrate may be because they are not in the same horizontal plane.When they semi-cover themselves,the middle stripes are in the same plane as the substrate,and the difference is expected to be much smaller.Both box turtle species adopt this strategy of covering themselves with leaves to better match their preferred substrate.Previous studies have demonstrated that some birds,crabs,and insects that cover themselves with decorations can reduce their detectability by predators (Holvecket al., 2017;Ruxton and Stevens,2015).This suggests that potential predators,such as humans,cannot discriminate visually between certain species and the substrate within which they are camouflaged.This strategy adds more noise to object discrimination at an early stage (Stevens and Cuthill,2006) and makes it more difficult for a predator to group the same features together visually(Espinosa and Cuthill,2014;Farmer and Taylor,1980).Our results indicate that both box turtle species may adopt a partial background matching camouflage strategy that reduces the contrast in their predator’s visual field,and they achieve this by semi-covering themselves in leaf litter.

    The middle stripe on the carapace ofC.mouhotiiand the side stripe on the carapace ofC.galbinifronsdid not match their preferred substrates.A high-contrast pattern that does not match the background can impose a high survival cost (Cott,1940).In visual search experiments with specific targets,humans quickly learned that an unusual color is a clue that their target is present (Fraseret al.,2007).In many cases,the patterns on animals appear to be stand out from their background,which may make them vulnerable to predation (Cott,1940;Stevens and Merilaita,2011).However,in some cases a predator may detect prominent features of a prey individual but does not recognize them as part of an object that is worth examining(Cott,1940),an effect that has been demonstrated in humans through eye-tracking experiments (Websteret al.,2013).The middle stripe ofC.mouhotiiand the side stripe ofC.galbinifronsmay be interpreted by predators as leaves rather than as recognizable parts of turtles.Studies have shown that internal contrast markers can in fact provide protection,as predators treat them as separate objects by dividing the entire object into visually disconnected parts (Cuthillet al.,2005;Osorio and Srinivasan,1991;Schaefer and Stobbe,2006;Stevenset al.,2009).

    The middle stripe of bothCuoraspecies shared the highest shape and color similarity levels with leaves from the broadleaves substrate,whereas the side stripe ofC.galbinifronshad the highest shape similarity with the bamboo-leaves substrate.When a predator has a short time frame within which to make a decision,the similarity between the species’ patterns and the background can provide protection (Hallet al.,2017).Several studies have demonstrated that patterned objects on a patterned background result in the slowest identification by predators.When objects are very similar in appearance,a predator’s tracking performance is impeded (Feria,2012;Hallet al.,2017;Howe and Holcombe,2012).The most promising evidence comes from a study on the four-eyed turtle (Sacalia quadriocellata),which showed that higher shape similarity resulted in a higher recognition error rate,indicating more effective camouflage (Xiaoet al.,2021).Interestingly,the middle stripe ofC.mouhotiihad greater chromatic contrast with their preferred substrates than with the grass substrate,meaning that the middle stripe in the preferred substrate can be easily detected by a predator.Despite contrasting in color with their preferred substrates,the middle stripe ofC.mouhotiiand the side stripe ofC.galbinifronsdid have a similar shape to that of the leaves.Previous studies have found that masquerading animals are usually similar in size,color,and shape to their model objects or species (Karpestamet al.,2014;Panettaet al.,2017;Skelhornet al.,2010b;Suzuki and Sakurai,2015).Furthermore,they frequently select the appropriate microhabitats to increase their similarity to the model (Nafuset al.,2015;Skelhorn and Ruxton,2013;Xiaoet al.,2021).For example,the caterpillars of the early thorn moth (Selenia dentaria) masquerade better when the density of a similar model increases (Skelhornet al.,2011).The common potoo bird selects perches on tree stumps that will effectively enhance their masquerading effect (Cestariet al.,2018).We believe that bothCuoraspecies adopt a partial masquerading strategy,rather than using their entire carapace as a means of camouflage.This allows them to increase their masquerading efficiency in varied substrates.

    Our results showed that area accounted for the largest amount of similarity;however,perimeter,AR,S,C,and R(except for the roundness of the side stripe forC.galbinifrons)had high similarity among the four substrate types.Shape and size also have an important influence on the efficiency of camouflage through masquerading.Chiao and Hanlon(2001) found that cuttlefish (Sepia pharaonis) adjust their body markings based on the area of the objects against which they need to camouflage.Skelhorn and Ruxton (2013) found that the size of the moth larvae determines the efficiency of masquerading;however,Suzuki and Sakurai (2015) confirmed that,despite having the same size and area,caterpillars can affect their masquerading efficiency by changing their shape.

    In the present study,area,perimeter,and the other four shape descriptors all had high similarity across habitats.This suggests that the similarity in shape between the turtle species’contrast stripes and the leaves of the habitat types is also a crucial aspect of their masquerading.Furthermore,human trichromatic vision is more sensitive to visual signals than the dichromatic vision of some other mammals (Caveset al.,2018;Trosciankoet al.,2017).Therefore,both turtle species would be camouflaged against the vision of these non-human mammals due to their shape similarity.

    In conclusion,this is the first study to demonstrate that partial masquerading and background matching have a significant impact on turtles’ camouflage efficiency and habitat choice.C.galbinifronsandC.mouhotiiboth exhibited two types of camouflage strategies in their preferred substrates;partial masquerading was apparent when the entire carapace was exposed,and background matching was evident when the turtles were semi-covered in leaf litter.This study introduces a novel approach to camouflage studies that focuses on the influence of shape and color of parts of the body to reduce detectability or recognition.This study evaluated the camouflage effect for human vision,but did not analyze its effect on the turtles’ actual predators in the wild.Future research should explore the effects of turtles’ camouflage strategies on real predators in the wild.

    AcknowledgmentsWe are grateful to Ling WANG,Tingting HUANG,and Zihao YE for assistance with image analysis.We also thank the Diaoluoshan National Nature Reserve for access to the field site and for providing assistance where necessary.Funding was provided by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31772486) and the Hainan Provincial Natural Science Foundation of China(319MS047 and 320CXTD437).

    国产亚洲av片在线观看秒播厂 | 国产精品一及| 免费观看性生交大片5| 精品人妻熟女av久视频| 国产伦精品一区二区三区四那| 亚洲成色77777| 人人妻人人澡欧美一区二区| 国产欧美另类精品又又久久亚洲欧美| 久久久久久久久大av| 日韩欧美国产在线观看| 在线免费十八禁| 亚洲人与动物交配视频| 国产精品伦人一区二区| 韩国高清视频一区二区三区| 99热6这里只有精品| 精品人妻熟女av久视频| 插阴视频在线观看视频| 亚洲五月天丁香| 两个人视频免费观看高清| 深爱激情五月婷婷| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 校园人妻丝袜中文字幕| 美女cb高潮喷水在线观看| 亚洲最大成人手机在线| 免费观看性生交大片5| 欧美激情在线99| 男插女下体视频免费在线播放| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| 直男gayav资源| 久久久精品大字幕| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 丰满乱子伦码专区| 国产一区亚洲一区在线观看| 亚洲内射少妇av| 综合色av麻豆| 精品午夜福利在线看| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 久久国内精品自在自线图片| 一级毛片电影观看 | 日本午夜av视频| 美女大奶头视频| 日韩三级伦理在线观看| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| 久久精品影院6| 久久精品综合一区二区三区| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 嫩草影院精品99| 国产精品女同一区二区软件| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 97热精品久久久久久| 99久久无色码亚洲精品果冻| 美女cb高潮喷水在线观看| 免费看日本二区| 欧美另类亚洲清纯唯美| av线在线观看网站| 成人美女网站在线观看视频| 大又大粗又爽又黄少妇毛片口| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 色网站视频免费| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看 | 激情 狠狠 欧美| 亚洲高清免费不卡视频| 亚洲高清免费不卡视频| 日本一二三区视频观看| 久久久久性生活片| 午夜久久久久精精品| av黄色大香蕉| 欧美97在线视频| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| 婷婷色麻豆天堂久久 | 亚洲国产色片| 特大巨黑吊av在线直播| 国产色婷婷99| 人妻制服诱惑在线中文字幕| 三级男女做爰猛烈吃奶摸视频| 婷婷色麻豆天堂久久 | 亚洲一区高清亚洲精品| 欧美高清性xxxxhd video| 精品久久久噜噜| 欧美高清成人免费视频www| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 久久久亚洲精品成人影院| 精品久久久久久久久亚洲| 免费播放大片免费观看视频在线观看 | 久久热精品热| 国产精品一及| 国产黄a三级三级三级人| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 国产麻豆成人av免费视频| 日韩一区二区三区影片| 欧美极品一区二区三区四区| 久久久久久久久久久丰满| 午夜视频国产福利| 内射极品少妇av片p| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 亚洲av日韩在线播放| 草草在线视频免费看| 日韩三级伦理在线观看| 搡老妇女老女人老熟妇| 最近视频中文字幕2019在线8| 国产免费视频播放在线视频 | 亚洲四区av| 亚洲成人久久爱视频| 精品久久久久久久久久久久久| 美女被艹到高潮喷水动态| 欧美激情久久久久久爽电影| 精品不卡国产一区二区三区| 欧美日韩在线观看h| av免费在线看不卡| 亚洲最大成人av| 最近2019中文字幕mv第一页| 中文在线观看免费www的网站| 看片在线看免费视频| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 亚洲自拍偷在线| 国产视频首页在线观看| 免费观看人在逋| 禁无遮挡网站| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 国产成人精品一,二区| 免费看光身美女| 91久久精品电影网| 国产不卡一卡二| or卡值多少钱| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 天堂av国产一区二区熟女人妻| 老司机影院毛片| 少妇人妻一区二区三区视频| 久久综合国产亚洲精品| 国产亚洲一区二区精品| 丝袜喷水一区| 日韩国内少妇激情av| 精品国产一区二区三区久久久樱花 | 国产熟女欧美一区二区| 美女大奶头视频| 长腿黑丝高跟| 一个人免费在线观看电影| 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 国产精品野战在线观看| 天堂影院成人在线观看| 亚洲丝袜综合中文字幕| 中文字幕av成人在线电影| 床上黄色一级片| 色播亚洲综合网| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 中文在线观看免费www的网站| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 久久热精品热| 国产视频首页在线观看| 2021少妇久久久久久久久久久| 高清在线视频一区二区三区 | 亚洲国产精品sss在线观看| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 国产片特级美女逼逼视频| 青春草视频在线免费观看| 1000部很黄的大片| 免费观看a级毛片全部| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 成人性生交大片免费视频hd| 亚洲精品乱码久久久v下载方式| 欧美人与善性xxx| 只有这里有精品99| 中文精品一卡2卡3卡4更新| 男女视频在线观看网站免费| 婷婷色av中文字幕| 中国美白少妇内射xxxbb| 22中文网久久字幕| 亚洲av福利一区| 欧美成人a在线观看| 婷婷色av中文字幕| 欧美变态另类bdsm刘玥| 国产白丝娇喘喷水9色精品| 精品人妻偷拍中文字幕| 亚洲av男天堂| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 亚洲精品456在线播放app| 亚洲欧美成人精品一区二区| 九九在线视频观看精品| 国产不卡一卡二| 国产成年人精品一区二区| 久久99热这里只频精品6学生 | 国产精品电影一区二区三区| 人妻制服诱惑在线中文字幕| 边亲边吃奶的免费视频| 男人舔女人下体高潮全视频| 在线观看美女被高潮喷水网站| 精品一区二区三区视频在线| 高清在线视频一区二区三区 | 精华霜和精华液先用哪个| 有码 亚洲区| 一个人观看的视频www高清免费观看| 午夜精品国产一区二区电影 | 欧美日韩在线观看h| 一级二级三级毛片免费看| 中文字幕精品亚洲无线码一区| 啦啦啦观看免费观看视频高清| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 色吧在线观看| 成人无遮挡网站| 国产在线一区二区三区精 | 久久久久久伊人网av| 国模一区二区三区四区视频| 国产av一区在线观看免费| av天堂中文字幕网| 国产精品av视频在线免费观看| 亚洲婷婷狠狠爱综合网| av黄色大香蕉| 亚洲中文字幕一区二区三区有码在线看| av又黄又爽大尺度在线免费看 | 精品人妻偷拍中文字幕| 亚洲va在线va天堂va国产| 国产亚洲精品av在线| 色吧在线观看| 欧美极品一区二区三区四区| 人人妻人人澡欧美一区二区| 99久久精品国产国产毛片| 内射极品少妇av片p| 亚洲av福利一区| 欧美成人精品欧美一级黄| 大话2 男鬼变身卡| videossex国产| 尾随美女入室| 最近最新中文字幕免费大全7| 久久精品久久久久久久性| 国产午夜福利久久久久久| 久久99热这里只频精品6学生 | 色吧在线观看| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 国内少妇人妻偷人精品xxx网站| 91精品伊人久久大香线蕉| a级一级毛片免费在线观看| 国产伦精品一区二区三区视频9| 国产中年淑女户外野战色| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 校园人妻丝袜中文字幕| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验| 国产成人福利小说| 一级爰片在线观看| 99久久精品热视频| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 日韩精品青青久久久久久| 日韩高清综合在线| 国产黄片美女视频| 岛国在线免费视频观看| 色5月婷婷丁香| 亚洲精品乱久久久久久| 国产乱人偷精品视频| 欧美日本亚洲视频在线播放| 国产美女午夜福利| 永久免费av网站大全| 亚洲国产精品成人综合色| 赤兔流量卡办理| 美女国产视频在线观看| 中文字幕制服av| 日韩av不卡免费在线播放| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 内射极品少妇av片p| 日本猛色少妇xxxxx猛交久久| 亚洲精品一区蜜桃| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产| 日本免费在线观看一区| 亚洲国产最新在线播放| 网址你懂的国产日韩在线| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 97超碰精品成人国产| 久久久久久久久久黄片| 级片在线观看| 亚洲伊人久久精品综合 | 亚洲激情五月婷婷啪啪| av女优亚洲男人天堂| h日本视频在线播放| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| av线在线观看网站| 亚洲欧美成人综合另类久久久 | 少妇熟女欧美另类| 久久热精品热| 亚洲国产色片| 国产免费一级a男人的天堂| 色播亚洲综合网| 欧美日韩国产亚洲二区| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品| 99热精品在线国产| 亚洲精品色激情综合| 亚洲久久久久久中文字幕| 午夜福利在线在线| 久久人人爽人人片av| 国产免费福利视频在线观看| 国产亚洲精品久久久com| 人人妻人人澡人人爽人人夜夜 | 久久久久九九精品影院| 最近中文字幕2019免费版| 日本av手机在线免费观看| 国产午夜精品一二区理论片| 变态另类丝袜制服| 免费人成在线观看视频色| 99久久中文字幕三级久久日本| 欧美另类亚洲清纯唯美| 26uuu在线亚洲综合色| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类| 神马国产精品三级电影在线观看| 国产麻豆成人av免费视频| 亚洲欧美精品自产自拍| 大香蕉97超碰在线| 精品熟女少妇av免费看| 99久国产av精品国产电影| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 国产高清有码在线观看视频| 少妇熟女aⅴ在线视频| 乱人视频在线观看| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添av毛片| 激情 狠狠 欧美| 可以在线观看毛片的网站| 国产精品国产三级国产av玫瑰| 久久精品国产亚洲av天美| 国产真实伦视频高清在线观看| 热99re8久久精品国产| 国产女主播在线喷水免费视频网站 | 成人av在线播放网站| av.在线天堂| 六月丁香七月| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 精品酒店卫生间| 嫩草影院新地址| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添av毛片| 青青草视频在线视频观看| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 91久久精品国产一区二区三区| 国产精品电影一区二区三区| 亚洲国产欧美人成| 国产午夜精品一二区理论片| 久久久精品欧美日韩精品| 午夜视频国产福利| 激情 狠狠 欧美| 国产在线男女| 最近视频中文字幕2019在线8| a级一级毛片免费在线观看| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| 久久这里有精品视频免费| 国产高清视频在线观看网站| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 97超视频在线观看视频| 国产一区有黄有色的免费视频 | 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 秋霞伦理黄片| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 麻豆一二三区av精品| 国产av不卡久久| 夜夜爽夜夜爽视频| 国产精品三级大全| 麻豆一二三区av精品| 色哟哟·www| av专区在线播放| 国产成人福利小说| 少妇的逼水好多| 国国产精品蜜臀av免费| 中文字幕av在线有码专区| 国产精品三级大全| 97超碰精品成人国产| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx在线观看| 波多野结衣巨乳人妻| 91久久精品国产一区二区三区| 小蜜桃在线观看免费完整版高清| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 中文字幕av在线有码专区| 久久久久久大精品| 色5月婷婷丁香| 免费大片18禁| 国产精品国产高清国产av| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 日韩国内少妇激情av| 狠狠狠狠99中文字幕| 欧美激情久久久久久爽电影| 亚洲高清免费不卡视频| 亚洲国产高清在线一区二区三| 禁无遮挡网站| ponron亚洲| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 91午夜精品亚洲一区二区三区| 久99久视频精品免费| 国产高潮美女av| 水蜜桃什么品种好| 汤姆久久久久久久影院中文字幕 | 天堂影院成人在线观看| 国产高清视频在线观看网站| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 一级二级三级毛片免费看| 91午夜精品亚洲一区二区三区| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 欧美又色又爽又黄视频| 免费看日本二区| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 高清毛片免费看| 亚洲国产精品专区欧美| 久久久精品大字幕| 亚洲精品国产成人久久av| 日韩在线高清观看一区二区三区| 最近中文字幕高清免费大全6| 大香蕉久久网| av卡一久久| 插逼视频在线观看| 一区二区三区乱码不卡18| 免费观看在线日韩| 国产人妻一区二区三区在| 久久精品人妻少妇| 国产真实乱freesex| 又粗又爽又猛毛片免费看| 久久久久网色| 寂寞人妻少妇视频99o| 久久午夜福利片| 成人美女网站在线观看视频| 欧美性猛交黑人性爽| av在线老鸭窝| 亚洲av成人精品一二三区| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| 亚洲欧美日韩无卡精品| 最近2019中文字幕mv第一页| 国产高潮美女av| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 免费观看在线日韩| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频| 欧美+日韩+精品| 日韩强制内射视频| 国产伦精品一区二区三区四那| 国产精品一区二区三区四区久久| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆| 男人舔女人下体高潮全视频| 欧美另类亚洲清纯唯美| 亚洲欧美精品专区久久| 女人被狂操c到高潮| 亚洲精品乱码久久久久久按摩| 丰满少妇做爰视频| 国产精品一区二区三区四区免费观看| 免费观看性生交大片5| 欧美变态另类bdsm刘玥| av免费在线看不卡| 欧美成人午夜免费资源| 免费看a级黄色片| 国产美女午夜福利| 免费观看在线日韩| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 一级黄色大片毛片| 久久6这里有精品| 欧美+日韩+精品| 我要看日韩黄色一级片| 欧美潮喷喷水| 在线免费观看的www视频| 午夜精品国产一区二区电影 | 免费观看性生交大片5| 日韩欧美在线乱码| 日本猛色少妇xxxxx猛交久久| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 岛国在线免费视频观看| 中文资源天堂在线| 国产av不卡久久| 亚洲av成人精品一区久久| 日日撸夜夜添| 国产高清三级在线| 最近视频中文字幕2019在线8| 黄色配什么色好看| 日本一二三区视频观看| 婷婷色麻豆天堂久久 | av又黄又爽大尺度在线免费看 | 一边亲一边摸免费视频| 欧美激情国产日韩精品一区| 一级毛片我不卡| 国内少妇人妻偷人精品xxx网站| 亚洲一区高清亚洲精品| 久久久久久久久大av| 成人欧美大片| 两个人视频免费观看高清| 中文亚洲av片在线观看爽| 亚洲欧美中文字幕日韩二区| 哪个播放器可以免费观看大片| 国产在视频线在精品| 亚洲国产欧美人成| 岛国毛片在线播放| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 2021天堂中文幕一二区在线观| 国产久久久一区二区三区| 国产成人精品婷婷| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 国产乱人视频| 欧美成人免费av一区二区三区| 一本—道久久a久久精品蜜桃钙片 精品乱码久久久久久99久播 | 日韩亚洲欧美综合| 黄片无遮挡物在线观看| 老师上课跳d突然被开到最大视频| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品| 精品酒店卫生间| 日韩亚洲欧美综合| 免费观看a级毛片全部| 国产色爽女视频免费观看| 舔av片在线| 在线观看66精品国产| 国产黄色视频一区二区在线观看 | 国产色婷婷99| 一个人免费在线观看电影| 中文亚洲av片在线观看爽| 99热全是精品| 99热网站在线观看| 赤兔流量卡办理| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 22中文网久久字幕| 成年av动漫网址| 床上黄色一级片| 国内精品一区二区在线观看| 欧美色视频一区免费| 久久国产乱子免费精品| 亚洲在线观看片| 综合色丁香网| 日本黄大片高清| 色视频www国产| 国产私拍福利视频在线观看| 日韩成人伦理影院| 亚洲欧美清纯卡通| 国产一区二区三区av在线| 丝袜喷水一区| 免费观看人在逋| 日本免费在线观看一区| 成人鲁丝片一二三区免费| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 尾随美女入室| 国产亚洲精品久久久com| 亚洲成人精品中文字幕电影| 国产综合懂色| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 日本免费在线观看一区| 久久久国产成人精品二区| 亚洲av免费在线观看| 麻豆成人av视频| 国产亚洲av嫩草精品影院| 亚洲成人精品中文字幕电影| 国产成人精品一,二区| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清| 九九在线视频观看精品| 日本黄色片子视频| 国产一区亚洲一区在线观看| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 在线免费十八禁| 免费播放大片免费观看视频在线观看 | 成年女人看的毛片在线观看| 一级av片app| 久久人人爽人人爽人人片va| 少妇高潮的动态图| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 亚洲av.av天堂| 日韩成人伦理影院| 2021少妇久久久久久久久久久| 深夜a级毛片| 中文在线观看免费www的网站| 高清av免费在线| kizo精华| 国产亚洲91精品色在线| 国语对白做爰xxxⅹ性视频网站| 熟女电影av网| 嫩草影院入口| .国产精品久久| 免费观看的影片在线观看| 精品熟女少妇av免费看| 久久久久网色| 日韩av在线免费看完整版不卡| 99热6这里只有精品| 中文欧美无线码| 成年免费大片在线观看| 亚洲欧美日韩无卡精品| 午夜精品国产一区二区电影 | 久久久色成人| ponron亚洲| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 久久久久国产网址| 亚洲精品影视一区二区三区av| 欧美最新免费一区二区三区| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频| 国产免费福利视频在线观看| 久99久视频精品免费| 日韩av在线免费看完整版不卡| 日本黄色片子视频| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| 国产av一区在线观看免费| 久久婷婷人人爽人人干人人爱| 国产高清三级在线| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 成人欧美大片| 一级二级三级毛片免费看| 精品酒店卫生间| 亚洲av免费高清在线观看| 久久99精品国语久久久| 黄色欧美视频在线观看| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 中文欧美无线码| 日韩一区二区视频免费看| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 精华霜和精华液先用哪个| 成人午夜精彩视频在线观看| 国产在视频线精品| 男人舔女人下体高潮全视频| 亚洲欧美成人精品一区二区| 好男人在线观看高清免费视频| 亚洲怡红院男人天堂| 特级一级黄色大片| 成人av在线播放网站| 久久久久久久久大av| 国内精品一区二区在线观看| 韩国av在线不卡| 久久人人爽人人爽人人片va| 一级黄色大片毛片| 国产精品蜜桃在线观看| 久久久精品欧美日韩精品| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 高清在线视频一区二区三区 |