• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Enhanced inhibitory control during re-engagement processing in badminton athletes:An event-related potential study

    2019-11-21 01:18:16JihengChenYnnLiGunghuiZhngXinhongJinYingzhiLuChenglinZhou
    Journal of Sport and Health Science 2019年6期

    Jiheng Chen ,Ynn Li ,Gunghui Zhng ,Xinhong Jin ,Yingzhi Lu ,*,Chenglin Zhou ,*

    a School of Kinesiology,Shanghai University of Sport,Shanghai 200438,China b Sports Department,Jinan University,Zhuhai Campus,Zhuhai 519000,China c Department of Mathematical Information Technology,University of Jyv¨askyl¨a,Jyv¨askyl¨a 40100,Finland Received 26 August 2018;revised 26 October 2018;accepted 29 November 2018 Available online 17 May 2019

    Abstract Purpose:The purpose of present study was to investigate the impact of sport experience on response inhibition and response re-engagement in expert badminton athletes during the stop-signal task and change-signal task.Methods:A total of 19 badminton athletes and 20 nonathletes performed both the stop-signal task and change-signal task.Reaction times(RTs)and event-related potentials were recorded and analyzed.Results:Behavioral results indicated that badminton athletes responded faster than nonathletes to go stimuli and to change signals,with faster change RTs and change-signal RTs,which take into consideration the variable stimulus onset time mean.During successful change trials in the change-signal task,the amplitudes of the event-related potential components N2 and P3 were smaller for badminton athletes than for nonathletes.Moreover,change-signal RTs and N2 amplitudes as well as change RTs and P3 amplitudes were significantly correlated in badminton athletes.A significant correlation was also found between the amplitude of the event-related potential component N1 and response accuracy to change signals in badminton athletes.Conclusion:Moderation of brain cortical activity in badminton athletes was more associated with their ability to rapidly inhibit a planned movement and re-engage with a new movement compared with nonathletes.The superior inhibitory control and more efficient neural mechanisms in badminton athletes compared with nonathletes might be a result of badminton athletes'professional training experience.2095-2546/?2019 Published by Elsevier B.V.on behalf of Shanghai University of Sport.This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license.(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

    Keywords:Badminton athletes;Change-signal task;Event-related potentials;Inhibitory control;Stop-signal task

    1.Introduction

    Inhibitory control,which encompasses both response inhibition and response re-engagement,plays an essential role in goal-directed behavior in daily life1as well as in sports.Previous studies have described a positive relationship between sport performance and inhibitory control.2-6However,most of these studies focused on response inhibition,which refers to the ability to stop an ongoing or planned action that is no longer appropriate,7-9rather than re-engagement to an alternate response.In real life,people are able to withhold ongoing actions when unpredictable events or changes occur,but in the most situations an alternative response has to be executed instead.10,11Therefore,response re-engagement may provide another important source of information about the nature of inhibitory control of human executive function.12

    In sports,ceasing an action precedes,in most cases,adjusting to an alternative movement.For example,in badminton,a typical reactive sport,players must adjust their movements constantly to an ever-changing environment.Frequently,badminton athletes need to suppress a swing completely,such as when the shuttlecock is outside their range.But more often,they need to inhibit their initial swing first and then switch to a modified one.This response re-engagement reflects cognitive flexibility in inhibitory control,9which obviously plays a key role in winning a match.Therefore,it is necessary to consider the 2 aspects of inhibitory control to observe the influence of athletic training on human beings.The current study aimed to investigate the effects of badminton expertise on inhibitory control and to explore whether this expert advantage would be revealed in response re-engagement.

    Most previous studies that have observed response inhibition in athletes utilized the Go/Nogo task and found that athletes showed higher response accuracy in Nogo trials,13,14as well as a faster Go reaction time(RT),14-16compared with nonathletes,suggesting superior ability for response inhibition.Event-related potential(ERP)has also been used to further explore the underlying neuronal activity during cognitive processing.In a specific paradigm,several ERP components are elicited by a stimulus.For example,the N2 component,which peaks at approximately 200-350 ms after the target stimuli,always occurs in inhibition tasks and is considered a marker of inhibitory processes.17,18Studies of expert baseball athletes and fencers have found a greater amplitude and a shorter latency of N2 in the Nogo portion of Go/Nogo tasks compared with nonathletes,5,14-16likely reflecting an improvement of inhibitory control in athletes performing reactive sports.Usually,the P3 component is observed after N2 in the response inhibition task, which occurs approximately 300-500 ms after the target stimuli presentation.In inhibition tasks,the P3 component is believed to reflect the allocation of attentional resources during the inhibitory processing,19but other studies have posited that it reflected aspects of active inhibition of the motor response.20However,when P3 amplitude has been compared between athletes and nonathletes,the results have been inconsistent.For example,some studies have found that the P3 amplitude was larger in athletes than in nonathletes during Nogo trials.The studies on fencing have suggested that,because of the higher requirement of highspeed responses to target stimuli(e.g.,when a Nogo stimulus is detected),the more cognitive resources was took during the inhibitory processing.15,16This hypothesis is supported by the increased P3 amplitude observed in fencers.In contrast,Zhang et al.14found a reduced P3 amplitude during Nogo trials in experienced fencers compared with nonathletes and suggest that this reduced Nogo-P3 in fencers reflects their improved inhibitory function,which allows their brains to evaluate and monitor response inhibition more efficiently.

    In addition to these 2 classic components discussed,the early and exogenous component N1 is also an important component in inhibitory tasks.The N1 component reflects the attention paid to the target stimuli.21An enhanced N1 amplitude,indicative of early visual processing,has also been observed among athletes during response inhibition.15,16Using the stop-signal task(SST),researchers have reported that the amplitude of N1 in successfully stopped trials is larger than that in unsuccessfully stopped trials,suggesting a positive association between N1 amplitude and response inhibition.21Together with N2 and P3,the N1 component in athletes needs further consideration in the SST.In the present study,we aim to describe the relationship between early visual processing(reflected by the N1 amplitude)and response inhibition(reflected by N2 and P3)in expert athletes compared with nonathletes.

    On the basis of these studies,it seems that athletes engaged in reactive sports have developed superior response inhibition,as reflected by their correspondingly altered neural activities,although some results remain unclear.However,little focus has been given to response re-engagement,and further studies are needed to get a complete understanding of the effects of expert advantage on inhibitory control.The change-signal task(CST)was designed to investigate response re-engagement in inhibitory control.9The latency of re-engagement can be measured by change RT;the latency of inhibition can also be determined by calculating the change-signal RT (CSRT).Thus,the 2 components of inhibitory control—response inhibition and re-engagement—can be measured and assessed using the SST and CST,respectively.

    The SST and the Go/Nogo are 2 popular tasks that can be used to assess response inhibition,but studies have shown that these 2 tasks are not completely identical measures of response inhibition.22In the Go/Nogo task,the response is stimulus dependent,which requires a response selection between executing and inhibiting a motor response.That is,a motor response is made to Go stimuli and withheld to Nogo stimuli.In SST and CST,responses are made on every trial unless a stop or a change signal is presented,and the inhibitory processing is triggered by a stop/change signal following the Go signal.This means that subjects have to retract an executive response that has already been triggered by the Go signal to successfully stop.23Therefore,we believe that the response processes of SST are more relevant to actual sports scenarios than are those of the Go/Nogo task,even though the latter has been more frequently studied in athletes.Furthermore,the 2 classic components N2 and P3,which are elicited by the Nogo stimuli and stop signal,have different explanations in these 2 tasks.For example,Nogo-N2 reflects the inhibitory processes for this task,24,25whereas other studies have suggested that the stop-N2 does not reflect response inhibition but instead reflects evaluation of the stop signal.20,26Nogo-P3 reflects the attentional processing involved in stimulus evaluation,27and stop-P3 represents the cognitive control engagement in monitoring outcome during the inhibitory process.28,29Because most studies have investigated the response inhibition in athletes using the Go/Nogo task,it is necessary to apply SST and CST to further explore the advantages of response inhibition and response re-engagement in reactive athletes,and thus make a contribution to our knowledge of the comparison of Go/Nogo and SST.

    In the present study,we aimed to investigate the effects of response inhibition and re-engagement in 2 tasks of inhibitory control in badminton athletes compared with nonathletes.We anticipated that,as an effect of long-term badminton training,badminton athletes would show enhanced inhibition and reengagement relative to nonathletes.In their ERP components,we expected that the typical inhibitory control-related components N2 and P3 would show significant differences between badminton athletes and nonathletes.In addition,we expected that there would be increased effort in perceptual processing,as reflected by the N1 component in badminton athletes compared with nonathletes.Finally,on the basis of previous studies that found correlations between inhibition performance and ERP components,5we hypothesized that a similar correlation would be found among the participants in our study.

    2.Methods

    2.1.Participants

    We conducted the power analysis before the study,for a 2(number of groups)×2(number of measurements)repeated measures design,the total sample size was determined using G*power30with the expected effect size of 0.4 and a significance level of 0.05 at the desired power of 0.95.For the athletes group,19 badminton athletes(5 women aged 19.03±0.71 years,and 14 men aged 20.01±1.20 years,mean±SD)were recruited from the Shanghai University of Sport badminton team.The inclusion criteria included(1)having more than 5 years of professional training(>12 h/week)before being placed on the college team,(2)maintaining skill training(>5 h/week)during college,and(3)these athletes are above the 2nd level of the national standard.

    For the nonathletes group,20 college students(6 women aged 18.24±0.42 years and 14 men aged 19.42±1.91 years)were recruited from the Shanghai University of Sport.The inclusion criteria included(1)with no sports training experience,and(2)watch the badminton matches less than 5 times in all during the past 5 years.The research was approved by the Research Ethics Committee at Shanghai University of Sport(approval number 2015003).

    All participants completed a personal information table before they started the experiment. All participants were undergraduates who did not smoke.They all had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity and were right handed.None reported mental or organic diseases.All participants provided written informed consent before inclusion in this study and were financially compensated for their participation.

    2.2.Task procedures

    The participants were provided with instructions for the 2 tasks:the SST and the CST—and the sequence counterbalance was used.In SST,all trials started with a 500-ms presentation of a cross on a screen.The participants were instructed to press number keys on the numeric keypad with their right index finger,resting on but not pressing“5”before and after each trial.The Go stimulus,an arrow pointing to the left or right(50%each),was presented after the cross disappeared.Participants were instructed to press the“4”key when the arrow pointed to the left and the“6”key when the arrow pointed to the right.Participants were told to respond as quickly as possible.The arrow disappeared from the screen if no key was pressed within 500 ms(Fig.1,left).There was a 30%chance in each block of trials that the stop signal for the SST(a red square outline)would appear surrounding the arrow.The appearance of the stop signal in the SST indicated that the participants had to withhold all key presses until the next trial(Fig.1,center).A blank screen appeared between each trial for a randomly generated time ranging between 1000 ms and 1500 ms.

    In the CST,the procedures were much the same as for the SST.The trials consisted of the Go stimulus and change signals.Participants were instructed to press the“4”key and the“6”key according to the orientation of the arrow(Fig.1,left).There was a 30%chance in each block of trials that the change signal for the CST(a red circle outline)would appear surrounding the arrow.The appearance of the change signal in the CST indicated that the participants should switch target keys,such that arrows pointing to the left now required pressing“8”and those pointing to the right required pressing“2”(Fig.1,right).

    The delay between a Go stimulus and a stop signal or a change signal fluctuated(stimulus onset asynchrony)from 50 ms to 700 ms;the delay was determined by a staircase-tracking algorithm that adapted to the response rate.31,32The original delay in each block was 200 ms,and when participants successfully stopped a stop signal trial in the SST or successfully changed their response in the change signal trial in the CST,the stimulus onset would increase by 50 ms in the next stop or change signal trial to increase the task difficulty.Otherwise,the stimulus onset was decreased by 50 ms to decrease the difficulty.This tracking procedure ensured an approximately 50%success rate in the stop or change signal trials.

    Fig.1.Illustration of the 3 types of trials:go(left),stop(center),and change(right).The SST consists of go trials and stop trials,whereas the CST consists of go trials and change trials.Stimulus onset(SSD for SST;CSD for CST)fluctuates between 50 ms and 700 ms.Numbers with gray background indicate which keys are to be pressed for each task.CST=change-signal task;CSD=change-signal delay;SSD=stop-signal delay;SST=stop-signal task.

    The experiment was conducted in a dimly lit,sound-attenuated room.Participants were seated 100 cm in front of a screen,which was positioned at eye level.All participants were required to read the instructions before they began.They conducted 20 trials of each task for practice;these results were not included in the analyses.In the formal experiment,there were 3 blocks for the SST and 3 blocks for the CST,with each block containing 120 trials comprising 84 go trials and 36 stop trials for the SST or change trials for the CST.The sequence in which the participants performed the 2 tasks was counterbalanced across participants.All tasks were programmed using Matlab(The MathWorks Inc.,Natick,MA,USA),and stimuli were presented on a 19-inch cathode ray tube screen(1024×768 pixels;refresh rate:100 Hz).

    2.3.Data collection

    The behavioral response data were collected using Matlab software (R2013b; The MathWorks Inc.). Meanwhile, the electroencephalographic(EEG)data were recorded using a Brain Vision EEG system(Brain Products GmbH,Gilching,Germany)referenced against FCz with a 64-channel amplifier and a sampling frequency of 500 Hz.Continuous EEG measurements were taken,averaged from the right and left mastoids,and the ground electrode was located on the mid-forehead.Horizontal eye movement blinks and vertical eye movements were recorded.The electrode impedances were maintained below 10 kΩ for all electrooculogram and EEG electrodes.

    2.4.Data and statistical analyses

    2.4.1.Behavioral data

    Four dependent variables were calculated for each task and group.First,response accuracy was calculated for go trials and stop trials in the SST,and for the go trials and change trials in the CST.Second,the go RT of go trials in 2 tasks was determined by measuring the time interval between the time when go signal appeared and time when participants made the correct response.Third,the signal RT(stop-signal RT(SSRT),CSRT)was calculated based on the mean stimulus onset(stop/changesignal delay(SSD,CSD))in correct signal trials and the mean go RT for both the SST and CST using the following equations:SSRT=mean go RT-mean SSD;CSRT=mean go RT-mean CSD.Fourth,the re-engagement RT was averaged for the correct responses.Specifically, the re-engagement RT was recognized as the change RT,which was calculated as the time duration between the onset of the change signals and the participants'correct key pressing(the dependent variable table is presented in Supplementary Table 1).

    The Monte Carol simulations of Band et al.33showed that the integration method resulted in reliable SSRT estimates for central SSDs,that is,SSDs for which p(respond|signal)is close to 0.50.Therefore,we reanalyzed the data using the integration method,that is,subtracting the mean SSD/CSD from the nth RT,where the nth RT is determined by multiplying the number of RTs in the go RT distribution by the overall p(respond|signal).

    Furthermore, dependent variables for errors and stop/change-signal delays were calculated for each task and group.(The table of variables for errors and signal delays is available in Supplementary Table 2.)

    Two-way repeated measures analysis of variance(ANOVA)was performed for go accuracy,inhibition(stop/change)accuracy,go RT,and signal RT(SSRT/CSRT),with group(badminton athletes vs.nonathletes)as the between-subjects factor and the inhibitory task(SST vs.CST)as the within-subjects factor.For change RT,variables for errors,and stop/change-signal delays,independent-samples t tests were used between groups.

    2.4.2.ERP data

    The EEG data were analyzed using EEGLAB toolbox34in Matlab R2013b.All channels were re-referenced offline to the average of the 2 mastoid electrodes.Eye movement and blinks were rejected for ocular correction. The recorded EEG data were filtered with a digital bandpass filter set from 0.1 Hz to 30.0 Hz to reduce low-frequency content that was irrelevant to the components of interest.Additionally,a notch filter(50 Hz)was applied to the data.The EEG data were then segmented with respect to stimulus markers into 1900-ms epochs,with 1100 ms before and 800 ms after the onset of the stop or change stimulus.Only trials that were correctly responded to were included in analyses.Trials with amplitudes exceeding±100 μV were excluded.To baseline correct to stable brain activity,the mean amplitude of the 900-to 1100-ms(blank screen)prestimulus interval was selected as the baseline,because the go stimulus was always before the signal stimulus and the ERP elicited by the go stimulus would be overlapped with the prestimulus interval if this were selected as the baseline.The ERP data were averaged for each group and condition.Finally,fast Fourier transformation35was used to filter the ERPs.

    Using the grand average waveforms and scalp topographic distributions,we analyzed 3 time windows:120-180 ms for N1,160-220 ms for N2,and 300-400 ms for P3(The figures for waveforms are available online as Supplementary Fig.1 for SST and Supplementary Fig.2 for CST).For the N1 component,2 electrode sites were grouped in the left hemisphere(O1,PO7)and 2 in the right hemisphere(O2,PO8).A two-way repeated measures ANOVA was used to examine N1 amplitudes,with group(badminton athletes vs.nonathletes)as the between-subjects factor and hemisphere(left vs.right)as the within-subjects factor.For the N2 and P3 components,according to our topographic distributions and previous studies,19,28we selected 4 midline electrodes(Fz,FCz,Cz,and Pz)for further analysis,and the amplitudes were entered into a two-way repeated measures ANOVA with group(badminton athletes vs.nonathletes)as the between-subject factor,electrode sites(Fz,FCz,Cz,and Pz)as the within-subjects factor.(The peak amplitudes and latencies of N1,N2,and P3 components are available online as Supplementary Data,the Result section.)

    To investigate associations between cognitive processing and inhibitory control ability,we analyzed the following bivariate correlations:stop/change accuracy and N1 amplitude at the occipital electrodes(O1,O2,PO7,and PO8).We performed the K-S test on some data that are used in Pearson's correlations between 2 groups (SSRT, CSRT, stop ACC,change ACC,change RT,and the amplitudes of N1,N2,and P3).The results show that the distribution of these dependent variables is normal in controls(all p >0.1)and in badminton athletes(all p >0.1,except the stop ACC,p=0.036).Considering the abnormal distribution of the stop accuracy in badminton athletes,we did an outlier analysis and took out 2 outliers.The K-S test shows the normal distribution of stop ACC(badminton athletes)after taking out the 2 outliers(Z=0.772,p=0.591).Pearson's correlation was then performed for SSRT/CSRT and N2 amplitude and for N1 and P3 amplitude in CST.

    For all analyses(behavioral and EEG data),p values of less than 0.05 were statistically significant.The p value ranges from 0.05 to 0.08 were marginally significant.(Degrees of freedom were corrected using the Greenhouse-Geisser method.)The effect size for each comparison are reported,and the partial eta-squared(η2p)was used as an index of effect size.Post hoc tests of significant main effects were conducted using the least significant difference method.The test was highly sensitive,and slight differences in the mean value of each level might be detected.

    3.Results

    3.1.Behavioral results

    The mean go RT,SSRT,CSRT,and change RT were included in the statistical analyses.The stop rate and change rate of both the experts and nonathletes were close to the expected 50%(Table 1).

    For the go accuracy, the main effect of task (F (1,37)=0.031, p=0.861, η2p=0.001) and group (F (1,37)=0.357,p=0.554,η2p=0.010)were not significant.The interaction between task and group was not significant(F(1,37)=0.559,p=0.459,η2p=0.015).

    For the inhibition accuracy,the main effect of task(F(1,37)=9.144,p=0.005,η2p=0.198)was significant,indicating that the stop accuracy(48.63%)was higher than the change accuracy(46.27%)and the interaction between task and group was significant(F(1,37)=4.096,p=0.050,η2p=0.100).Further analyses revealed that the stop accuracy in badminton athletes was slightly lower than in nonathletes(p=0.073).The main effect of group was not significant(F(1,37)=0.002,p=0.963,<0.001).

    For the RT in the go trials of both tasks,the main effect of group was marginally significant(F(1,37)=3.362,p=0.075,η2p=0.083),indicating that the go RT in badminton athletes(428.34±57.46 ms)was shorter than that in nonathletes(455.22 ± 44.64 ms). The main effect of task(F(1,37)=0.012,p=0.913,η2p<0.001)and the interaction between task and group (F (1, 37)=1.508, p=0.227,η2p=0.039)were not significant.

    By contrast,for the signal RT,the main effect of task(F(1,37)=12.127,p=0.001,η2p=0.248)and the interaction between task and group were significant(F(1,37)=6.953,p=0.012,η2p=0.158).Further analyses revealed that the badminton athletes had a significantly shorter CSRT than nonathletes did(p=0.046)and that the CSRT was significantly longer than the SSRT in nonathletes(p <0.001).The comparisons of the mean signal RTs and change RT are available Supplementary Fig. 3. The main effect of group was not significant(F(1,37)=2.146,p=0.151,η2p=0.055).

    For the change RT,an independent-samples t test revealed that the change RT in badminton athletes was significantly shorter than in nonathletes(t(37)=2.718,p=0.01,Cohen's d=0.873).

    For the error-related variables and stop/change-signal delay,an independent-samples t test revealed that there were no significant differences between 2 groups except the go-error in SST.The table of behavioral data for errors is available in Supplementary Table 3.

    3.2.ERP Results

    3.2.1.Stop-signal ERPs

    No significant differences in amplitude were found for the N1 component(150-180 ms).

    Table 1 Behavioral data associated with the 2 tasks.

    For the N2 component (160-220 ms) amplitude, the main effect of group was significant (F(1,37)=4.227,p=0.047,η2p=0.103),with the badminton athletes(-0.92±0.67 μV)displaying a smaller amplitude than the nonathletes(-2.84±0.65 μV).The main effect of electrode site was also significant(F(2,61)=7.674,p <0.001,η2p=0.172),with the smallest N2 amplitude on Fz(-0.55±0.53 μV)(all p <0.002;FCz:-2.08±0.59 μV,Cz:-2.72±0.57 μV,Pz:-2.17±0.52 μV).The interaction of electrode site by group was not significant(F(2,64)=1.933,p=0.160,η2p=0.050)(Fig.2A).

    No significant differences were found in the amplitude of the P3 component(300-400 ms).3.2.2.Change-signal ERPs

    For the amplitude of the N2 component(160-220 ms),the main effect of group was significant(F(1,37)=4.770,p=0.035,η2p=0.114), with the badminton athletes (0.28±0.66 μV)displaying a smaller amplitude than the nonathletes(-1.72±0.64 μV).The main effect of electrode site was also significant(F(1,55)=7.066,p=0.004,η2p=0.160),with the smallest N2 amplitude on Fz(-0.55±0.51 μV)(all p <0.004;FCz:-0.59±0.53 μV,Cz:-1.21±0.57 μV,Pz:-1.63±0.61 μV).The interaction of electrode site by group was not significant(F(1,55)=1.358,p=0.261,η2p=0.035)(Fig.2B).

    For the amplitude of the N1 component,the main effect of hemisphere was significant (F (1, 37)=7.467, p=0.010,η2p=0.168),with the N1 amplitude in the left hemisphere(-7.11±0.62 μV)larger than that in the right hemisphere(-6.20±0.53 μV).The main effect of group on the average amplitude was not significant(F(1,37)=2.847,p=0.100,η2p=0.071)nor was the interaction of hemisphere and group(F(1,37)=0.827,p=0.369,η2p=0.022)(Fig.2C).

    For the amplitude of the P3 component(300-400 ms),the main effect of group was marginally significant(F(1,37)=3.833,p=0.058,η2p=0.094),with the badminton athletes(2.26±0.50 μV)displaying a smaller amplitude than the nonathletes(3.64±0.49 μV).The main effect of electrode site was also significant(F(1,48)=13.928,p <0.001,η2p=0.273),with the smallest P3 amplitude on Fz(1.63±0.43 μV)(all p <0.004;FCz:3.08±0.42 μV,Cz:3.42±0.39 μV,Pz:3.67±0.41 μV).The interaction of electrode site by group was not significant (F (1, 48)=0.097, p=0.825,η2p=0.003)(Fig.2D).

    3.3.Correlation between EEG and behavioral data

    3.3.1.N1 amplitude and accuracy

    Fig.2.Comparison of the amplitudes at various electrode sites between badminton athletes and nonathletes for each task.(A)The N2 amplitude at 4 electrode sites in participants performing the SST.(B)The N2 amplitude at 4 electrode sites in participants performing the CST.(C)The N1 amplitudes for both hemispheres in participants performing the CST.(D)The P3 amplitudes at 4 electrode sites in participants performing the CST.*p <0.05 significant difference.CST=changesignal task;SST=stop-signal task.

    In SST, a correlation analysis of badminton athletes revealed a significant negative correlation between accuracy in the change trials and the N1 amplitude on the PO7 electrode site(p=0.015,r=-0.578),as well as that in CST(p=0.036,r=-0.483).However,the correlation of nonathletes was not significant(p=0.731,r=-0.082)(Fig.3A,B).

    3.3.2.N2 amplitude and accuracy,RT

    In the CST,a correlation analysis of badminton athletes revealed negative correlations between the change ACC and the N2 amplitude at the FCz(p=0.021,r=-0.523)and Cz(p=0.001,r=-0.698)electrode sites.These same correlations in the nonathletes were not significant(FCz:p=0.374,r=-0.210;Cz:p=0.254,r=0.268)(Fig.3C).

    Correlation analyses of badminton athletes revealed positive correlations between the CSRT and the N2 amplitude at the FCz(p=0.045,r=0.464)and Cz(p=0.042,r=0.471)electrode sites.However,the correlation of nonathletes was not significant(FCz:p=0.294,r=-0.247;Cz:p=0.326,r=-0.231)(Fig.3D).

    3.3.3.N1 amplitude during change trials and P3 amplitude

    In CST, there was a significant negative correlation between N1 amplitude(left hemisphere)and P3 amplitude at the Fz(p=0.052,r=-0.452),Cz(p=0.061,r=-0.438)and Pz(p <0.001,r=-0.740).However,the correlation of nonathletes was not significant (Fz: p=0.469, r=0.172; Cz:p=0.103,r=-0.385;Pz:p=0.134,r=-0.347).The figure showing this correlation is available in Supplementary Fig.4.

    4.Discussion

    In this study,we investigated response inhibition and reengagement of expert badminton athletes compared with nonathletes.It is the first time that these 2 aspects of inhibitory control have been investigated among athletes in 1 study.The present study illustrates the cognitive superiority of badminton athletes and sheds new light on the impact of athletic training on inhibitory control.Subjects were asked to inhibit a prepared response completely or to switch actions upon the appearance of a stop signal(in SST)or a change signal(in CST).Behaviorally,athletes showed shorter response inhibition and reengagement times than nonathletes.Electrophysiologically,we found that,compared with nonathletes,experts showed reduced N2 amplitudes during stop trials,and reduced N2 and P3 amplitudes during change trials.Significant correlations between CST behavioral performance and ERPs were found in the badminton athletes but not in the nonathletes.

    Fig.3.Correlations between behavioral and ERP data.(A)Correlation between the N1 component amplitude at the PO7 electrode site and the accuracy of badminton athletes and nonathletes during performance of a stop trial.(B)Correlation between N1 amplitude at the PO7 electrode site and the accuracy of badminton athletes and of nonathletes during performance of a change trial(C)Correlation between N2 amplitude at the FCz electrode site and the accuracy of badminton athletes and nonathletes during performance of a change trial.(D)Correlation between N2 amplitude at the FCz electrode site during performance of the CSRT of badminton athletes and of nonathletes.CSRT=change-signal RT;ERP=event-related potential.

    The present behavioral results are consistent with the hypothesis that badminton athletes would exhibit a shorter CSRT than nonathletes would.According to the race model,32response inhibition depends on competition between the processes of response execution and inhibition,and a successful inhibition requires the latter process to“win the race”.Thus,a shorter CSRT indicated that badminton athletes were faster at inhibiting an initiated movement than were nonathletes.Moreover,as previously described,the shorter response time of badminton athletes in go trials further support the notion of enhanced inhibition,namely,experts can successfully cease the action even though this action was too fast.We found that experts also had shorter CSRTs than nonathletes in the CST;that is,badminton athletes were faster than nonathletes at switching to a new action.Together,these results suggest that badminton athletes outperform nonathletes in inhibitory control,especially when it comes to response re-engagement processing.

    However,not all of our results were consistent with previous studies;we did not observe a significant difference between groups in response inhibition as assessed by the SST.36Considering the faster go response time and lower Nogo accuracy in badminton athletes (described previously), we hypothesized that badminton athletes would take the go trial as a priority,resulting in a response that could not be stopped immediately and quickly.However,in the CST,they seemed to prioritize the change trial and response to go trial equally,allowing them to keep the same inhibition time as in the SST,despite the increased cognitive load with CST.9For nonathletes, the longer response time in the CST than in the SST might be due to the larger cognitive load associated with the CST,in which participants need to complete 2 successive cognitive processes.Accordingly,we argue that cognitive processing in the CST fits the actual processing of badminton athletes during their real-world sports experience better than it does in the SST.Measures obtained in the relatively simpler SST did not distinguish badminton athletes from nonathletes,indicating an advantage of examining inhibitory control processing in athletes using the CST rather than the SST.

    Our EEG results also showed differences between experts and nonathletes,primarily on the CST.Compared with nonathletes, experts showed reduced amplitudes of both the stop-N2(the N2 component elicited by stop trials)and the change-N2(the N2 component elicited by change trials).Considering that EEG studies on the re-engagement processes in athletes are lacking,we will cite studies on nonathletes to discuss these results.Studies have shown similar neurophysiological activity during both stopping and changing of a motor response.1,37Thus,we believe that the change-N2 amplitude may reflect that of the stop-N2. However, interpretation of N2 component results across different inhibition processing tasks remains controversial.Nieuwenhuis et al.38argued that the N2 component reflects the conflict arising from competition between the execution and the inhibition of a single response;this interpretation suggests that the conflict is smaller in badminton athletes than in nonathletes because the inhibitory response overrode the go response much more quickly.In addition,the positive correlation we found between change-N2 amplitude and CSRT in badminton athletes also supported this interpretation of N2 amplitude,suggesting that badminton athletes stop faster because of the reduced conflict,and thus reduced workload,in their brains during task performance.Taken together,these findings indicate that badminton athletes are better than nonathletes at stopping a prepared response in both tasks because of reduced cognitive load.Notably,this result is in contrast to most previous studies examining the N2 amplitude in highly trained athletes performing a Go/Nogo paradigm, which instead show an increased stop-N2 amplitude.This indicates that processing of stop/change signals in the SST/CST cannot be simply equated with processing of Nogo stimuli in the standard Go/Nogo task.

    Badminton athletes also displayed reduced P3 amplitudes elicited by change signals compared with nonathletes.This result is consistent with a previous study among fencers,where a reduced Nogo-P3 was reported,which the authors interpreted as reduced cognitive effort.14As a late ERP component,P3 reflects the attentional processing involved in stimulus evaluation27and the cognitive control engagement in monitoring outcome during the inhibitory process.28,29The lower P3 amplitude in badminton athletes compared with nonathletes in our study suggests that the experts allocated less attention to the re-engagement response,whereas nonathletes used more cognitive and attentional resources to monitor the results of the inhibition and to ensure the processing of a re-engagement response.In addition,we found a negative correlation between P3 amplitude and N1 in the experts;that is,a smaller P3 amplitude was associated with a larger N1 amplitude.The foregoing correlation result showed that the larger N1 amplitude was associated with the higher change ACC in badminton athletes and suggests that badminton athletes deliberately allocated fewer attentional resources to evaluate and monitor the results of the response inhibition. However, they engaged more resources in the early processing of the change signal to ensure the higher change ACC and the response movement execution,thus making their responses faster than those of the nonathletes.The negative correlation between N1 and P3 amplitude may reflect the efficient resource allocation in badminton athletes.The reduced P3 amplitude was detected only during the change task,not the stop task,providing further evidence that the training experience in reactive sports is more similar to the demands of the CST than to the demands of the SST.

    We unexpectedly observed that there was no difference between experts and nonathletes in the change-N1 amplitude,suggesting that early visual processing of change signals is not altered as a function of athletic experience.However,we did find a significant negative correlation between change-N1 amplitude and change accuracy in badminton athletes,but not in nonathletes.Several studies have described a positive relationship between early attentional processing and inhibitory control.21,39-41Thus,our results suggest that for badminton athletes successful change was related to attentional effort on the change signal.We agree with that this finding indicates a kind of specialized processing in experts'brains described by Callan and Naito42which posited“a specific brain region(or network of regions)carrying out processes related to aspects of a task through experience-dependent learning,thus allowing for better performance”(p.2).Further study will be required to investigate the early attentional processing involved in performance of inhibitory control tasks in badminton athletes.This would help to optimize the training program and the selection of badminton athletes.

    The present findings highlight the superior inhibitory control of athletes compared with nonathletes when undertaking 2 different tasks.However,this study had some limitations that should be considered.Although we speculate that our findings might be broadly relevant to long-term reactive sports training,the generalizability of these results across different activities remains to be determined.And the insufficient sample size prevents us from coming up with a stronger conclusion.Furthermore,it is widely understood that a strong relationship exists between physical fitness and cognitive function.Thus,future studies should take into account the physical fitness of participants,43for example,by determining body mass index and maximum oxygen uptake,to elucidate the specificity of reactive sports training on inhibitory control processes.

    5.Conclusion

    In the present study we observed superior inhibitory control in badminton athletes,who inhibited their responses and reengaged alternative movements more quickly than did the nonathletes. Measures of brain activity in badminton athletes suggested that they were more efficient at stopping a prepared response owing to their allocation of fewer cognitive resources to updating the movement.This advantage could indicate a more rational distribution of neural resources in badminton athletes,which would allow them to achieve better and more stable performance than nonathletes.The efficient neural mechanisms and superior inhibitory control observed in badminton athletes may be a direct result of their long-term professional training.

    Acknowledgment

    This research received specific grants from the Natural Science Foundation of China(31571151,31700985)and the Scientific and Technological Commission of Shanghai(17080503100).

    Authors’contributions

    JC designed and carried out these experiments,and drafted the manuscript;GZ participated in the EEG data analysis;YL and XJ participated in the coordination of the study and helped polish the manuscript;YL and CZ conceptualized the study,revised/reviewed the manuscript,and provided the financial support for this research.All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript,and agree with the order of presentation of the authors.

    Competing interests

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    Supplementary materials

    Supplementary material associated with this article can be found in the online version at doi:10.1016/j.jshs.2019.05.005.

    国产av在哪里看| 国产成年人精品一区二区| 国产成人福利小说| 国产精品女同一区二区软件 | 一区福利在线观看| 午夜亚洲福利在线播放| 国产单亲对白刺激| 国产精品乱码一区二三区的特点| 国产真实伦视频高清在线观看 | 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av | 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添小说| 国产精品98久久久久久宅男小说| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 我要搜黄色片| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区| 老司机深夜福利视频在线观看| 亚洲 国产 在线| 国产av不卡久久| 久久午夜亚洲精品久久| 国产激情欧美一区二区| 亚洲av成人不卡在线观看播放网| 国产精品日韩av在线免费观看| 真人一进一出gif抽搐免费| 欧美大码av| 免费av不卡在线播放| 少妇丰满av| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清| 少妇人妻精品综合一区二区 | 亚洲av五月六月丁香网| 久久99热这里只有精品18| 日日干狠狠操夜夜爽| 国产69精品久久久久777片| 欧美最黄视频在线播放免费| 成人精品一区二区免费| 一二三四社区在线视频社区8| 美女大奶头视频| 久久国产乱子伦精品免费另类| 在线免费观看的www视频| 性色av乱码一区二区三区2| 久久亚洲真实| 精品欧美国产一区二区三| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区| 欧美色欧美亚洲另类二区| 久久久国产成人免费| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 黄色丝袜av网址大全| 美女被艹到高潮喷水动态| 成人国产综合亚洲| 日韩中文字幕欧美一区二区| 久久久久久九九精品二区国产| 国产一区在线观看成人免费| 亚洲av成人av| 香蕉丝袜av| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看 | 91九色精品人成在线观看| 三级男女做爰猛烈吃奶摸视频| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 欧美又色又爽又黄视频| 99久久精品热视频| 国产精品女同一区二区软件 | 波多野结衣高清无吗| 啦啦啦免费观看视频1| 美女大奶头视频| 国产三级在线视频| 精品国产三级普通话版| 久久久久久久午夜电影| tocl精华| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区| 国产伦精品一区二区三区四那| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 悠悠久久av| 每晚都被弄得嗷嗷叫到高潮| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 国产欧美日韩一区二区三| 日韩成人在线观看一区二区三区| 他把我摸到了高潮在线观看| 久久久久久大精品| 听说在线观看完整版免费高清| 噜噜噜噜噜久久久久久91| 国产黄色小视频在线观看| 国产一区二区亚洲精品在线观看| 黄片小视频在线播放| 十八禁网站免费在线| 国产精品自产拍在线观看55亚洲| 亚洲七黄色美女视频| 亚洲精品久久国产高清桃花| aaaaa片日本免费| 国产精品亚洲av一区麻豆| 欧美大码av| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 欧美三级亚洲精品| 国产高清视频在线播放一区| 成人av一区二区三区在线看| 长腿黑丝高跟| 久久性视频一级片| 日韩欧美在线二视频| 国产在视频线在精品| 欧美日本视频| 婷婷亚洲欧美| 成人国产一区最新在线观看| 美女高潮喷水抽搐中文字幕| 午夜福利免费观看在线| 深爱激情五月婷婷| 18美女黄网站色大片免费观看| xxxwww97欧美| 怎么达到女性高潮| 欧美日本视频| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 亚洲专区中文字幕在线| 久久久久久久久大av| 一夜夜www| 别揉我奶头~嗯~啊~动态视频| 尤物成人国产欧美一区二区三区| 天堂av国产一区二区熟女人妻| 99视频精品全部免费 在线| 全区人妻精品视频| 色哟哟哟哟哟哟| 欧美日本亚洲视频在线播放| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 国产精品自产拍在线观看55亚洲| 嫩草影院入口| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区| 国产色爽女视频免费观看| 亚洲av五月六月丁香网| 大型黄色视频在线免费观看| 国产免费av片在线观看野外av| 极品教师在线免费播放| 久久天躁狠狠躁夜夜2o2o| 午夜激情欧美在线| 成人18禁在线播放| 伊人久久精品亚洲午夜| 亚洲专区国产一区二区| 久久久久久九九精品二区国产| 91麻豆精品激情在线观看国产| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 麻豆国产av国片精品| 欧美一级毛片孕妇| 中文字幕av在线有码专区| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 极品教师在线免费播放| 99久久综合精品五月天人人| 在线十欧美十亚洲十日本专区| 国产视频一区二区在线看| 亚洲成人中文字幕在线播放| 亚洲美女视频黄频| 性色av乱码一区二区三区2| 久久草成人影院| av专区在线播放| 欧美日韩综合久久久久久 | 99久久无色码亚洲精品果冻| 日韩欧美精品免费久久 | 人妻久久中文字幕网| 国产精品野战在线观看| 亚洲 国产 在线| 18禁国产床啪视频网站| 久久精品影院6| 亚洲无线在线观看| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 少妇人妻精品综合一区二区 | 舔av片在线| 亚洲精品久久国产高清桃花| 中出人妻视频一区二区| av黄色大香蕉| 白带黄色成豆腐渣| 色在线成人网| 十八禁网站免费在线| 18美女黄网站色大片免费观看| 99久久精品国产亚洲精品| 一本精品99久久精品77| 亚洲最大成人中文| 精品电影一区二区在线| 国产精品一及| 久久久久国内视频| 老司机福利观看| 国产探花极品一区二区| 在线视频色国产色| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 天天躁日日操中文字幕| 欧美一级毛片孕妇| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 91av网一区二区| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品| 在线看三级毛片| 黄色成人免费大全| 国产精品女同一区二区软件 | 日本黄色片子视频| 久久香蕉精品热| 亚洲狠狠婷婷综合久久图片| 国产精品一区二区免费欧美| 午夜a级毛片| 1024手机看黄色片| 黄色成人免费大全| 欧美日韩乱码在线| 可以在线观看毛片的网站| 美女免费视频网站| 久久九九热精品免费| 欧美乱色亚洲激情| 欧美一区二区国产精品久久精品| 国产精品 欧美亚洲| 亚洲专区中文字幕在线| 黄片大片在线免费观看| 9191精品国产免费久久| 欧美黑人欧美精品刺激| 色av中文字幕| 国产乱人视频| 国产精华一区二区三区| 欧美+日韩+精品| 97超视频在线观看视频| 精品99又大又爽又粗少妇毛片 | 免费大片18禁| 欧美最黄视频在线播放免费| 精品国产美女av久久久久小说| 亚洲人与动物交配视频| 动漫黄色视频在线观看| 狠狠狠狠99中文字幕| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 精品国产美女av久久久久小说| 老司机午夜十八禁免费视频| 欧美日韩黄片免| 18禁美女被吸乳视频| 国产单亲对白刺激| 99久久成人亚洲精品观看| 精品免费久久久久久久清纯| 美女cb高潮喷水在线观看| 99久久精品一区二区三区| 热99在线观看视频| www国产在线视频色| 身体一侧抽搐| 免费看十八禁软件| 欧美精品啪啪一区二区三区| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 日韩欧美在线二视频| 亚洲av成人不卡在线观看播放网| 午夜免费激情av| 亚洲最大成人中文| 欧美一区二区亚洲| 丁香六月欧美| 深夜精品福利| 午夜久久久久精精品| 亚洲欧美日韩无卡精品| 国产成人av教育| 久久香蕉精品热| 一个人免费在线观看电影| 国产探花在线观看一区二区| 国产精品,欧美在线| 中出人妻视频一区二区| 人人妻人人澡欧美一区二区| 51国产日韩欧美| 小说图片视频综合网站| 丝袜美腿在线中文| 亚洲va日本ⅴa欧美va伊人久久| 亚洲内射少妇av| 亚洲不卡免费看| 2021天堂中文幕一二区在线观| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 婷婷亚洲欧美| 亚洲七黄色美女视频| 看片在线看免费视频| 久久久久久久久大av| 国产成人欧美在线观看| 午夜免费成人在线视频| 午夜精品久久久久久毛片777| 亚洲精品一卡2卡三卡4卡5卡| 最近视频中文字幕2019在线8| 国产色爽女视频免费观看| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 午夜激情欧美在线| 精品人妻偷拍中文字幕| 一本一本综合久久| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 亚洲性夜色夜夜综合| 国内精品久久久久精免费| 国产伦精品一区二区三区视频9 | 亚洲中文字幕日韩| 一区二区三区免费毛片| 一个人看的www免费观看视频| 亚洲性夜色夜夜综合| 欧美最黄视频在线播放免费| 在线免费观看的www视频| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 18禁黄网站禁片免费观看直播| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人 | 有码 亚洲区| 国产高清videossex| 国产一区在线观看成人免费| 中出人妻视频一区二区| 少妇人妻一区二区三区视频| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 精品久久久久久,| 少妇人妻精品综合一区二区 | 日韩 欧美 亚洲 中文字幕| 在线看三级毛片| 日韩免费av在线播放| a在线观看视频网站| 欧美精品啪啪一区二区三区| 亚洲七黄色美女视频| 亚洲成av人片在线播放无| 哪里可以看免费的av片| 天堂网av新在线| 一进一出好大好爽视频| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆 | 性欧美人与动物交配| 91字幕亚洲| 午夜精品在线福利| 色哟哟哟哟哟哟| 一本久久中文字幕| 99视频精品全部免费 在线| 免费搜索国产男女视频| 网址你懂的国产日韩在线| 观看美女的网站| 国产亚洲精品av在线| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 国产私拍福利视频在线观看| 亚洲天堂国产精品一区在线| 国产精品,欧美在线| 国内久久婷婷六月综合欲色啪| 亚洲精品成人久久久久久| 色视频www国产| 亚洲精品在线观看二区| 日本撒尿小便嘘嘘汇集6| 精品电影一区二区在线| 无人区码免费观看不卡| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 97超级碰碰碰精品色视频在线观看| 日韩人妻高清精品专区| 无人区码免费观看不卡| 99国产极品粉嫩在线观看| 黄色片一级片一级黄色片| 一区二区三区高清视频在线| av女优亚洲男人天堂| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在 | 亚洲av电影在线进入| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| 欧美日韩中文字幕国产精品一区二区三区| a级一级毛片免费在线观看| 天堂√8在线中文| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 久久久久久久久久黄片| 国内精品久久久久精免费| 亚洲精品色激情综合| av黄色大香蕉| 88av欧美| 人妻丰满熟妇av一区二区三区| 深夜精品福利| 99热精品在线国产| 琪琪午夜伦伦电影理论片6080| 美女被艹到高潮喷水动态| 哪里可以看免费的av片| 成人精品一区二区免费| 听说在线观看完整版免费高清| 亚洲国产欧美网| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 麻豆一二三区av精品| 亚洲精品国产精品久久久不卡| 免费观看精品视频网站| 国产一区二区激情短视频| 美女被艹到高潮喷水动态| 19禁男女啪啪无遮挡网站| 国产探花在线观看一区二区| 亚洲av日韩精品久久久久久密| 看片在线看免费视频| 日本精品一区二区三区蜜桃| 校园春色视频在线观看| av在线蜜桃| 久久久成人免费电影| 亚洲精品粉嫩美女一区| 国产精品久久久久久久久免 | 久久久久久久亚洲中文字幕 | 老汉色∧v一级毛片| 久久香蕉国产精品| 日本三级黄在线观看| 中文字幕av成人在线电影| 免费看十八禁软件| 欧美日本视频| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| 国产精品久久久久久久久免 | 国产精品日韩av在线免费观看| 男女之事视频高清在线观看| 校园春色视频在线观看| 啦啦啦免费观看视频1| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx| 亚洲欧美日韩无卡精品| 内地一区二区视频在线| 高清日韩中文字幕在线| 99热这里只有是精品50| 亚洲国产欧美人成| 国产熟女xx| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 亚洲一区二区三区不卡视频| 两个人视频免费观看高清| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 午夜老司机福利剧场| 亚洲电影在线观看av| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看 | 啦啦啦韩国在线观看视频| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 国内毛片毛片毛片毛片毛片| 少妇熟女aⅴ在线视频| 久久亚洲真实| 国产一级毛片七仙女欲春2| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 日韩免费av在线播放| 一级黄片播放器| 日本与韩国留学比较| 免费看日本二区| 国产一区二区在线观看日韩 | av国产免费在线观看| 亚洲男人的天堂狠狠| www.色视频.com| 亚洲人成网站高清观看| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 好看av亚洲va欧美ⅴa在| 性色av乱码一区二区三区2| 成年版毛片免费区| 国产亚洲精品久久久com| 99久久久亚洲精品蜜臀av| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产 | 日韩欧美国产一区二区入口| 成年女人毛片免费观看观看9| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线| 亚洲av成人精品一区久久| 九色成人免费人妻av| 色综合婷婷激情| 制服丝袜大香蕉在线| 婷婷亚洲欧美| 亚洲av免费高清在线观看| 精品电影一区二区在线| 免费看日本二区| 国产精品av视频在线免费观看| 九色国产91popny在线| 九色成人免费人妻av| 久久精品国产综合久久久| 欧美成人性av电影在线观看| 欧美日韩综合久久久久久 | 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 欧美乱码精品一区二区三区| 久久亚洲真实| 色哟哟哟哟哟哟| 很黄的视频免费| 欧美在线黄色| 日本一本二区三区精品| 国产一区二区三区在线臀色熟女| 手机成人av网站| 给我免费播放毛片高清在线观看| 精品国产超薄肉色丝袜足j| 国产精品女同一区二区软件 | 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 亚洲精品影视一区二区三区av| 国产一级毛片七仙女欲春2| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产 | 日本五十路高清| 久久草成人影院| 免费av毛片视频| 窝窝影院91人妻| 日韩欧美免费精品| 精品不卡国产一区二区三区| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| www日本黄色视频网| 制服人妻中文乱码| 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 又黄又粗又硬又大视频| 欧美在线一区亚洲| 日本黄大片高清| 亚洲欧美激情综合另类| 久久婷婷人人爽人人干人人爱| 高潮久久久久久久久久久不卡| 嫩草影院精品99| 手机成人av网站| 午夜精品在线福利| 亚洲美女黄片视频| 亚洲天堂国产精品一区在线| 露出奶头的视频| 老熟妇乱子伦视频在线观看| 久9热在线精品视频| 嫩草影视91久久| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 免费电影在线观看免费观看| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 日本成人三级电影网站| 欧美成人a在线观看| h日本视频在线播放| 色精品久久人妻99蜜桃| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 51午夜福利影视在线观看| 亚洲一区二区三区不卡视频| 香蕉丝袜av| 99国产精品一区二区三区| 亚洲av一区综合| 757午夜福利合集在线观看| 我要搜黄色片| 在线观看舔阴道视频| 亚洲av日韩精品久久久久久密| 精品久久久久久久人妻蜜臀av| 午夜福利免费观看在线| 91在线观看av| 久久香蕉国产精品| 国产av一区在线观看免费| 99久久精品热视频| 国产av一区在线观看免费| 黄片小视频在线播放| 久久久久久久亚洲中文字幕 | 午夜福利高清视频| 久久午夜亚洲精品久久| 99国产极品粉嫩在线观看| 国产成人啪精品午夜网站| 国产黄a三级三级三级人| 伊人久久精品亚洲午夜| 久久久色成人| www日本黄色视频网| 午夜精品一区二区三区免费看| 观看美女的网站| 动漫黄色视频在线观看| 国产成人欧美在线观看| 亚洲18禁久久av| 久久精品亚洲精品国产色婷小说| 禁无遮挡网站| 美女大奶头视频| 夜夜躁狠狠躁天天躁| 搡老妇女老女人老熟妇| 身体一侧抽搐| 人妻久久中文字幕网| 波多野结衣高清作品| 国产免费男女视频| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 久久国产乱子伦精品免费另类| 老师上课跳d突然被开到最大视频 久久午夜综合久久蜜桃 | 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 在线天堂最新版资源| 色视频www国产| 岛国视频午夜一区免费看| 又黄又粗又硬又大视频| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看 | 老熟妇乱子伦视频在线观看| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 热99在线观看视频| 18+在线观看网站| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 精品人妻1区二区| 国产aⅴ精品一区二区三区波| 深爱激情五月婷婷| 不卡一级毛片| 欧美成人免费av一区二区三区| 九色成人免费人妻av| 老司机在亚洲福利影院| 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 色综合亚洲欧美另类图片| 在线观看舔阴道视频| 美女cb高潮喷水在线观看| 国产高清视频在线观看网站| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| 亚洲欧美日韩高清专用| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 天堂av国产一区二区熟女人妻| 日本 欧美在线| av天堂在线播放| 真实男女啪啪啪动态图| а√天堂www在线а√下载| 久久久久久人人人人人| 亚洲avbb在线观看| 国产一区二区激情短视频| 变态另类成人亚洲欧美熟女| 夜夜夜夜夜久久久久| 亚洲18禁久久av| 欧美激情久久久久久爽电影| 在线观看免费午夜福利视频| 搡女人真爽免费视频火全软件 | 在线播放无遮挡| 国产三级黄色录像| 在线播放无遮挡| 日日干狠狠操夜夜爽| 又紧又爽又黄一区二区| 1024手机看黄色片| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 亚洲第一欧美日韩一区二区三区| 久久精品国产亚洲av香蕉五月| 日日夜夜操网爽| 最近最新中文字幕大全免费视频| 国产精品98久久久久久宅男小说| 给我免费播放毛片高清在线观看| 又粗又爽又猛毛片免费看| 欧美3d第一页| 国产精品久久久久久精品电影| 国产成人av激情在线播放| 久久久国产精品麻豆| www日本在线高清视频| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 悠悠久久av| 欧美黄色淫秽网站| 他把我摸到了高潮在线观看| 啦啦啦韩国在线观看视频| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| 老司机在亚洲福利影院| xxxwww97欧美| 国产av不卡久久| 三级男女做爰猛烈吃奶摸视频| 国产成人欧美在线观看| 亚洲国产欧美人成| 日韩中文字幕欧美一区二区| 午夜福利成人在线免费观看| 一个人免费在线观看的高清视频| 动漫黄色视频在线观看| 亚洲男人的天堂狠狠| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 亚洲中文字幕日韩| 一卡2卡三卡四卡精品乱码亚洲| 亚洲性夜色夜夜综合| 成人av一区二区三区在线看| 中文字幕人妻熟人妻熟丝袜美 | 国产精品亚洲一级av第二区|