• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    The exercise-cognition relationship:A virtuous circleMichel Audiffren*,Nathalie Andr′e

    2019-10-16 02:35:22
    Journal of Sport and Health Science 2019年4期

    Abstract Exercise and health psychology have generated 2 sets of empirical studies guided by separate theory-driven axes.The first axis focuses on the causal relationship between chronic exercise and cognition and, more particularly, high-level cognitive functions such as executive functions(EFs).The second axis examines factors influencing the adherence process to physical activity(PA).Research conducted during the past decade shows that these 2 topics are closely linked,with EFs and effortful control playing a pivotal role in the bidirectional relationship linking PA and mental/brain health. The present article supports the idea that an individual engaged in the regular practice of effortful PA initiates a virtuous circle linking PA and effortful control in a bidirectional way. On the one hand, chronic exercise leads to an improvement of EFs and effortful control.On the other hand,gains in EFs and effortful control effectiveness lead to a reciprocal facilitation of the maintenance of PA over time.Some limitations and perspectives to this effort hypothesis are proposed in the last part of the article.2095-2546/? 2019 Published by Elsevier B.V.on behalf of Shanghai University of Sport.This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license.(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

    Keywords: Adherence;Effort;Executive functions;Physical activity;Salience network

    1. Introduction

    Starting a program of physical exercises after a long period of sedentariness or muscular deconditioning is often experienced as painful and difficult,physically as well as psychologically. However, the feelings of painfulness and drudgery gradually attenuate while individuals persist in regular exercise. Physical training generally leads to improvements in cardiovascular fitness and muscular strength, as well as psychological benefits that facilitate the adherence process. This article focuses on the virtuous circle linking exercise and cognition, which allows an individual to maintain the healthy behavior of exercising thanks to cognitive benefits (Fig. 1),despite the fact that costs and barriers associated with exercise are initially high and only decrease through practice. The physiological benefits of exercise are well-documented and well-recognized by physicians as well as scientists.1In contrast,the cognitive benefits are less well-known and need to be promoted more widely.In this article,we explore the complete virtuous circle, first describing the positive effects of chronic exercise on cognitive performance and brain health and then showing the benefit of high capacity in effortful control and executive functions (EFs) on adherence to physical activity(PA) programs. As we discuss later, two causal relationships involved in the virtuous circle need more empirical evidence:(1) the causal relationship between chronic exercise and the connectivity of two large-scale neuronal networks underpinning effortful control and EFs,respectively,and(2)the causal relationship linking efficiency of effortful control and/or EFs to exercise programs adherence.Consequently,in the last section of this article,we suggest some experimental methodologies to address these questions in the future.

    Cross-sectional studies showing a positive relationship between level of PA and cognitive performance have an important limitation: they cannot establish a causal relationship between practicing PA and improvement in cognitive performance.However,the positive correlation observed between the level of PA and cognitive performance can be interpreted in two ways. The first interpretation converges with results from numerous intervention studies and considers that practicing PA regularly leads to an improvement of cognitive performance.2-5The second interpretation is less popular and has been subject to fewer investigations. It considers that people who have good cognitive functioning are inclined to practice PA more easily and more regularly. In fact, in the present article,we assume that both interpretations are valid and influence each other in a virtuous circle(Fig.1).

    Fig. 1. Schematic illustration of the bidirectional relationship between physical activity/chronic exercise and executive functions/effortful control. The right arrow illustrates the causal link between regular physical activity/chronic exercise and executive functions/effortful control;that is, regularly practicing exercise leads to an improvement of executive functions and effortful control.The left arrow illustrates the causal link between executive functions/effortful control and practicing regular physical activity/exercise; that is, a high efficiency in executive functions and effortful control leads to a better adherence to exercise.

    Many intervention studies suggest that the positive effects of chronic exercise on cognition are preferentially observed in older adults and children.2-5These two populations are particularly targeted because specific components of cognition,such as EFs,are developing in children and declining in older adults and are, therefore, more sensitive to the effects of moderators that improve or impair EFs. The most popular hypothesis explaining the positive effect of chronic exercise on cognition,called the “neurotrophic hypothesis”, posits that, while exercising, the organism releases several neurotrophic molecules that stimulate hippocampal neurogenesis, brain angiogenesis, and the synthesis of monoamines.6From the perspective of the neurotrophic hypothesis, the accumulation of exercise sessions leads to durable changes enhancing brain integrity and brain functioning and, consequently, cognitive health.However,alternative hypotheses have been proposed.7This article focuses on one of these hypotheses, the so-called effort hypothesis. It considers that while practicing effortful physical exercises, individuals strengthen the connectivity of large-scale neuronal networks involved in effortful control and EFs.The benefit acquired in these two cognitive domains then leads to a facilitation of stopping unhealthy sedentary behaviors and adhering to healthy exercising.In Section 2,we define EFs, self-regulation, and effortful control and examine their roles in practicing PA.

    2. EFs,self-regulation,effortful control,and PA

    According to a well-known and frequently used taxonomy in cognitive psychology,8at least three main and elementary components of EFs can be identified.They include(1)maintenance and updating of relevant information in working memory,(2)intentional inhibition of prepotent impulses,unwanted and intrusive thoughts, embarrassing emotions, or automatic responses,and(3)mental set shifting,also known as cognitive flexibility. Other high-level cognitive processes such as volition,planning,sustained and selective attention,and self-regulation have also been considered intrinsically linked to EFs.9-11The concept of self-regulation,which is discussed in Section 4, refers to psychophysiological processes enabling an individual to guide his or her goal-directed activities over time and across changing circumstances.12Self-regulation encompasses a large variety of regulatory activities from homeostatic adjustments automatically ensured by the hypothalamus to effortful self-control underpinned by the prefrontal cortices.EFs subserve and are a prerequisite of selfregulation.11The regular practice of physical exercises according to exercise science recommendations requires the use of the three components of EFs described.First,the maintenance and updating of exercise-related goals in working memory are crucial for performance achievement.For instance, when running, walking, or cycling along a trail, individuals often set one or several intermediate goals throughout the whole route of the race in terms of maintaining an exercise intensity to reach waypoints before given times.Maintaining and updating these intermediate intensity-related goals in working memory through persistent activity is essential to having a good performance in line with the self-referenced goal for the whole race.Second, practicing exercise requires the intentional inhibition to resist the desire to stop exercise when the feeling of discomfort or fatigue is too high. Third, the regular practice of PA also requires planning the exercise session and remembering when to exercise at the planned time (prospective memory).EFs and self-regulation require mental effort as soon as the individual mobilizes them to cope with a stressful situation or carry out a demanding cognitive, social, or physical task. For instance, in all exercise performed at an uncomfortable intensity that needs to be maintained over time,mental effort is necessary to sustain the motor drive throughout the entire bout of exercise. From this perspective, cognitive-energetic models consider mental effort as a capacity-limited resource managed by the mechanism of effort.13,14This mechanism allows the individual to maintain task performance under disturbance from stressors and to prevent the loss of task goals under circumstances, such as increased processing demands, exercise demands, and competition from other tasks. Underpinned by frontal and limbic structures that take part in a large-scale brain network called the “salience network (SN)” (described in Section 3),the mechanism of effort exerts active and effortful control over brain systems underlying EFs and self-regulation.15In fact, effortful control is necessary for the activation and effectiveness of brain systems underlying EFs and selfregulation.As mentioned,this effortful control is capacity limited, but the effort hypothesis posits that this capacity can be improved through cognitive or physical training.Training programs aiming to improve effortful control must involve effortful cognitive processes such as EF or effortful physical exercises. According to homeostatic, reward, and other taskrelated feedback signals,the mechanism of effort makes decisions about maintaining an uncomfortable intensity, reducing it, stopping exercise, or shifting to another more comfortable activity.15

    EFs are generally assessed through performance variations observed in cognitive tasks engaging these functions. The most commonly used tasks for this purpose include the n-back task and running-span task for updating working memory;the Stroop task, Eriksen flanker task, Simon task, go-no-go task,stop-signal task, and anti-saccade task for intentional inhibition; the trail-making test, Wisconsin card sorting test, and switching task for cognitive flexibility;and the Tower of London task for planning. Moreover, effortful control exerted by the mechanism of effort during the task(i.e.,the mental effort invested in the task) can be assessed through several psychophysiological indices such as pupillary dilation,heart rate variability,or systolic pre-ejection period recorded throughout the duration of the task.16Finally,self-regulation can be measured through behavioral indices such as adherence to exercise programs. In summary, effortful control and EFs are the cornerstones of the virtuous circle; they benefit from healthy behaviors such as regular exercise and play a crucial role in the adherence process allowing the maintenance of these healthy behaviors in the long term.

    3. Causal relationship between chronic exercise and EFs and effortful control

    A recent meta-analysis focusing on adults over 50 years of age that included 36 intervention studies showed a moderate effect size of 0.34 of chronic exercise on EFs.17Another metaanalysis focusing on children from 4 to 18 years of age that included 36 intervention studies also showed small to moderate effects of chronic exercise on EFs.18Given the data presented in these two meta-analyses, it can be concluded that there are strong empirical arguments for a real effect of chronic exercise on EFs and effortful control. However, to strengthen the rationale supporting the right arrow of Fig. 1,we now examine the effect of chronic exercise on brain networks and structures underpinning EFs and effortful control.

    Correlational brain imaging studies conducted over the past 20 years provide strong arguments for three large-scale interconnected networks that underpin high-level cognitive functions.19The default mode network (DMN) shows a tasknegative activity profile. Its activation is associated with selfreferenced thoughts,emotional processing,and autobiographical memory.20The DMN mainly includes the orbital frontal cortex, the lateral temporal cortex, the inferior parietal lobe,the posterior cingulate/retrosplenial cortex,and the hippocampus/parahippocampal cortex. The central executive network(CEN), also described as the frontoparietal network, shows a task-positive activity profile because it is activated during a large variety of cognitive tasks that subserve EF.19The CEN is mainly anchored in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex,the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex,and the lateral parietal cortex.19,21Finally,the SN,alternatively called the cingulo-opercular network,also shows a taskpositive activity profile. It underpins the mechanism of effort,making decisions about the direction and the intensity of the behavioral response in a specific situation (i.e., fight, flight,submission to a higher-ranking individual, resignation,computation of the amount of invested effort, etc.), and switching between the DMN and the CEN.15,16,19,22,23The switching process activating the CEN and deactivating the DMN operates each time an individual engages in a cognitive task or an effortful exercise.Conversely,the switching process activating the DMN and deactivating the CEN operates each time an individual disengages his or her attention from the task and is distracted by self-referenced thoughts. The SN mainly consists of the orbital frontoinsular cortex, the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex(dACC),the anterior insula(AI),and the superior temporal gyrus.19,21We assume that the CEN and the SN benefit from chronic exercise and that these benefits in connectivity within and between these two networks improve the efficiency of EFs and facilitate adherence to exercise programs.The contribution of the DMN to the virtuous circle cannot be excluded (see Section 5). However, because it is not directly related to effortful control and EFs,we made the choice not to include it in the following analysis of the literature.

    Several intervention studies using structural brain imaging showed a positive effect of chronic exercise on gray or white matter volume of brain structures underpinning EFs and the mechanism of effort.24-28Colcombe et al.24demonstrated that, among a group of community-dwelling older adults, a 6-month intervention of aerobic training 3 times per week led to an increase in gray matter volume of two main hubs of the SN, the dACC, and the left superior temporal lobe. Reiter et al.25showed that, among a group of older adults with mild cognitive impairment, improvement in cardiorespiratory fitness induced by a 3-month walking intervention 4 times per week was positively correlated with increased cortical thickness in the left insula and left superior temporal gyrus, two important hubs of the SN. Ruscheweyh et al.26showed that,among a group of community-dwelling older adults who participated in a 6-month Nordic walking or gymnastics program 3 times per week, total PA was positively associated with increases in local gray matter volume in the prefrontal and cingulate cortex, two areas involved in the CEN and the SN.Scheewe et al.27showed that, among a group of patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorder, the cardiorespiratory fitness improvement induced by a 6-month intervention of aerobic and resistance exercises 2 times per week was significantly associated with thickening in the left hemisphere of the frontal,temporal, and cingulate cortex. Voss et al.28showed that,among a group of community-dwelling older adults, greater aerobic fitness derived from a 12-month intervention of walking was associated with greater increases in white matter integrity in the frontal and temporal lobes and with greater improvements in short-term memory. Thus far, only two intervention studies using functional brain imaging have shown a positive effect of chronic exercise on connectivity within and between large-scale networks. Voss et al.29showed that among a group of older adults, a 12-month intervention program of walking 3 times per week led to increased functional connectivity in the DMN and CEN associated with greater improvements in EF.Voss et al.30showed that,among a group of communitydwelling older adults, connectivity between the right anterior lateral prefrontal cortex and the left hippocampus decreased significantly in a walking group compared with a control group in a 12-month intervention program.30This literature review shows that exercise and health psychologists need more brainimaging studies testing the causal link between chronic exercise and strengthening of connectivity within and between the CEN and the SN.

    In this section, we presented behavioral and neurophysiological arguments for a causal link between chronic exercise and EFs as well as effortful control, represented by the right arrow of the virtuous circle (Fig. 1). An examination of the reciprocal relationship illustrated by the left arrow of Fig. 1 now follows.

    4. Causal relationship between EFs and effortful control and adherence to exercise

    The effort hypothesis posits that adherence to PA and other healthy behaviors depends on effortful control capacity and EF effectiveness, which are underpinned by the strength in connectivity within and between the CEN and the SN(see Section 3).For instance,as mentioned in Section 2,planning and intentional inhibition are two EFs that require mental effort and are very useful for practicing exercise sessions regularly.Consequently,it can be inferred that a low level of effortful control capacity and/or EF effectiveness would lead to adherence problems and, more generally, to behavioral change failures. Conversely, a high level or a gain in these cognitive functions would lead to increased adherence to healthy behaviors and a facilitation of behavioral change.As we will see later a low level in effortful control and/or EF effectiveness may come from dispositional factors (e.g., a low level of trait self-control, genetic polymorphisms detrimental for EFs) or from periods of temporary weakening,which may last from a few days to several years. The first part of this section focuses on problems of health behaviors associated with a dispositional low level of effortful control and EFs.

    According to the projections of the World Health Organization (WHO) for 2030, five of the leading causes of death worldwide among the top 20 will be ischemic heart disease,stroke, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, diabetes mellitus,and HIV/AIDS.31Mortality is attributable to major health risk factors such as obesity, tobacco and drug use, sedentariness, and unprotected sex. For instance, the number of obese individuals is expected to increase by 65 million and 11 million in the United States and the United Kingdom, respectively, in the next 15 years. Worldwide,it is similarly expected that the total number of tobacco-related deaths will increase from 6.4 million in 2015 to 8.3 million in 2030. Likewise, in the next 15 years, serious deleterious health consequences and 2 million deaths per year can be attributed to a lack of PA.These projections have economic consequences for society and require a better understanding of how to help individuals change their unhealthy habits. All change starts with one new behavior choice,but the difficulty is not only in making a decision to change, but also in initiating and maintaining this change as long as possible.32Behavioral change is a deliberate and voluntary process that requires cognitive resources to be successful. Effortful cognitive processes such as EFs are necessary for any individual to change his or her behavior and,particularly,to maintain a sufficient amount of PA per week to have health benefits.

    A method to show that EFs are necessary to regulate behavior is to examine whether people engaged in behaviors such as psychoactive substance addiction (e.g.,tobacco, alcohol, or cannabis), game or sex addiction, or compulsive behavior (e.g.,snacking or kleptomania) also display a dysfunction in EFs. It could be hypothesized that a dysfunction in EFs would lead to a decrease in the ability to refrain from habitual unhealthy and/or anti-social behavior and then to a higher probability of occurrence of the deviant behaviors in these individuals. There is an extensive and growing body of literature on this topic supporting a link between EFs and the ability to refrain from unhealthy or antisocial behavior throughout the lifetime.33,34Overall, individuals with weaker EFs tend to display more deviant behaviors compared with their stronger EF counterparts. For instance,Goudriaan et al.33showed that pathological gamblers and alcohol-dependent patients in their study were characterized by diminished executive functioning in inhibition, time estimation,cognitive flexibility,and planning tasks in comparison with control participants.33Similarly, poor EFs have also been reported in youths who are at high risk for substance misuse,35and EFs assessed at age 10-12 predict substance use disorder at age 19.36It is also important to note that unhealthy behaviors such as smoking,drug use,and a sedentary lifestyle can have a detrimental effect on executive functioning,37leading to a greater loss of control for concerned individuals and thus aggravating the situation. In other words, someone who starts and maintains an unhealthy behavior enters a vicious circle from which it is more and more difficult to escape.

    The end of this section focuses on the maintenance of healthy behavior targeted by the present article: exercising.Martin Ginis and Bray38pointed out that exercise adherence requires self-regulatory skills such as anticipating and developing plans to overcome exercise barriers, creating exercise plans and schedules, and managing exercise-related pain and discomfort.As mentioned in Section 2,all of these self-regulatory skills are directly related to EFs. For instance, the initiation and maintenance of a behavior such as practicing exercise several times a week requires the intentional inhibition to resist the desire to stop exercise when the feeling of discomfort or fatigue is too high;39the impulse to stop is certainly more pronounced in sedentary adults and seniors who have maintained a sedentary lifestyle throughout their lives. Practicing exercise regularly also requires planning the exercise sessions in the coming week (i.e., goal setting), remembering when to exercise at the planned time (i.e., prospective memory), and accepting that positive effects of PA occur in the long term(i.e., delay of gratification), which are three higher cognitive functions directly related to EF.9,40,41The predictive power of EF on PA engagement has been mainly demonstrated with cross-sectional studies.42,43To our knowledge,no intervention study has been conducted with the aim of demonstrating a causal link between an improvement in EF efficiency and adherence to exercise. We only found one intervention study showing the effects of an aerobic exercise training program on EFs,specifically inhibitory control,and the positive transfer of the gain in EFs to self-regulation in the dietary domain.44However, a very interesting longitudinal study assessing adherence to exercise over 1 year after a 12-month resistance exercise training program showed that individuals with higher levels of EF are more likely to engage in the exercise routine because they have a higher capacity for overriding impulse rewards and shifting focus to delayed outcomes.45In summary, EF and self-regulation are essential and very useful in the behavioral change process and the maintenance of a sufficient level of PA.Several articles have highlighted the bidirectionality of the relationship between PA and EF and/or selfregulation,46-49which has been formalized in Hall and Fong’s temporal self-regulation theory for PA.50This model highlights that the difficulty in maintaining long-term engagement in health behaviors is due to the short-term costs of the target behavior (e.g., fatigue or pain for exercising, privation for a diet regimen) and few long-term benefits. The relevance of this model is the association between health models such as the theory of planned behavior,51which considers intention as a determinant of behavior and the role of EF and behavioral prepotency as moderators of the link between intention and behavior. Behavioral prepotency can be defined as behaviors that constitute a strong habit and that need to be changed,such a sedentary lifestyle, snacking, or smoking. Determining the role of these cognitive processes in behavioral change has provided an alternative and heuristic approach to explain success or failure to initiate and maintain such changes.11,52

    The difficulty in maintaining long-term engagement can also be related to momentary,acute,or chronic,but nondispositional, failure of self-regulation. On the one hand, people exercise less on days when their EFs and related self-regulatory skills are weakened,and this relationship has been shown to be stronger in individuals with low trait self-control.53In the same way,participants exposed to an acute self-regulatory depletion manipulation generated lower levels of work during a 10-min bicycling task and planned to exert less effort during an upcoming exercise bout compared with control participants.38On the other hand, long periods of self-regulation exhaustion can lead to maladaptive behaviors,such as relapses or absenteeism.54If training is an effective technique to enhance EF and self-regulation,this technique could be disastrous when applied to individuals with chronic self-regulation exhaustion. When individuals cope with chronic self-regulation exhaustion, the training of self-regulatory skills has to be used with caution given the risk of aggravating the fatigue state. Other perspectives aiming to restore self-regulation resources can be proposed, such as combining progressive self-regulatory training with cognitive-behavioral therapies,55recovery techniques,56and/or habit formation.57There is a body of work that suggests that cognitive-behavioral therapies focusing on self-regulation(called self-system therapy)can be used to target dysfunction in self-regulation by changing the availability, the importance, and the patterns of goal-directed behavior.58,59These therapies have recently been used in association with graded exercise therapy in people who have a persistent failure of self-regulation by acting on their motivational dysfunction rather than their cognitive dysfunction.Such therapies have yielded interesting results, particularly in relapse prevention.60Other techniques that rely on the recovery of self-regulation resources can also be used.These recovery techniques have been studied mainly with sleep,which has an important impact on cognitive functioning and self-regulatory capacity.For instance,a set of cross-sectional studies has shown the usefulness of sleep hygiene in predicting psychological strain at work61and the ability of individuals to engage in regular exercise.62Another technique registered as a behavioral cognitive technique concerns habit formation.63Gardner and Lally have extensively studied habit formation in the health domain and have shown that habit formation can be triggered automatically and can occur in the absence of effortful control.57,64According to Bargh,65the repetition of a behavior in a specific context gives the potential to activate the behavior in the absence of cognitive effort,deliberation,or effortful control because alternative behaviors become less accessible in memory. In other words, because the context is associated with motivational reward, it becomes a signal showing an opportunity and an incentive to act. Lally et al.66and Gardner57reported that the formation of healthy habits renders healthy behaviors more resistant to lapses and relapses and facilitates the maintenance of new behavior after the intervention. In summary, the virtuous circle characterized by the relationship between chronic exercise and EFs and/or effortful control can become a vicious circle when people cease this healthy behavior and do not replace it by another effective training method or when techniques used to improve EFs are not adapted given the current weakness of self-regulatory skills. The accurate evaluation of failures in self-regulation is essential to target the most appropriate techniques. For instance, in the case of self-regulation exhaustion, it is appropriate to avoid techniques based on self-regulation training and engage in goal-directed-based techniques. In contrast, in the case of emotional dysregulation (e.g., systematic use of anxiety to cope with the anticipated consequences of a decision),the use of habit formation techniques must be preferred over any other technique.

    5. Future research directions and conclusions

    In this article, we have highlighted that there is currently a lack of empirical evidence for two causal relationships involved in the virtuous circle,while the link between chronic exercise and EF is fairly well-established using behavioral measures of EF.The first causal relationship that lacks empirical evidence is the relationship between chronic exercise and network connectivity within and between the CEN and the SN. As mentioned by Buckley et al.,67it could be interesting to include the DMN in these analyses because it could be involved in contemplation periods during which people engage in introspection on their aspirations for exercise,planning exercise sessions, and evaluating exercise goals and discrepancies. The DMN could also be involved in the cognitive-behavioral therapies mentioned in Section 4, which used self-referenced processes.The second causal relationship that needs more empirical evidence is the relationship between improvement in EFs and adherence to exercise programs,which both require the mobilization of mental effort.The present section proposes two categories of interventions that would be useful to conduct in the next decade. They would allow a more careful examination of links necessary to establish the validity of the virtuous circle described in Fig.1.The costs of such interventions are relatively high, but they are fundamental to testing the effort hypothesis.It is important to emphasize that,if the effort hypothesis is valid,several applications might follow in the domain of health and sport psychology to help patients stop unhealthy behaviors or maintain healthy behaviors,thus generating individual and societal benefits.The present article provides numerous arguments supporting the effort hypothesis but delineates two categories of interventions that would be necessary to more accurately test its validity.

    The first category of intervention studies focuses on the effects of chronic exercise on large-scale neuronal networks involved in effortful control and EF. Based on Colcombe and Kramer’s meta-analytic results5,68and on the literature presented in Section 3,we recommend randomized controlled trials(RCTs) with an intervention of a minimal duration of 6 months. In addition, at least 2 groups of participants should be planned: a treatment group participating in effortful physical exercise at least 3 times a week at the end of the intervention and a control group practicing effortless physical exercises with the same session schedule as the treatment group. Effortful physical exercises can be defined as any exercise requiring sustained attention or effort to maintain an uncomfortable intensity to reach the goal of the exercise.The duration of these exercises should be progressive and range between 15 and 45 min. The intensity of the exercises should be progressively increased on an individual basis. The progressivity in increasing exercise intensity should also be calibrated to obtain good compliance with the program (≥80%). The effortless physical exercises could use the same PA basis, for instance, walking or jogging,but practiced at a comfortable speed that requires very little attention or effort throughout each bout of exercise. Feelings of mental effort and pleasure during exercise should be measured throughout the program and in the two arms of the RCT. The completion of a battery of several cognitive tasks engaging EFs and,most particularly,intentional inhibition and planning and/or updating of working memory should be planned before the beginning and after the end of the intervention program. These two EFs are specifically targeted because they play an important role in the adherence process to exercise programs (see Section 2 for examples). Psychophysiological indices, such as pupillary dilation and heart rate variability, could be recorded throughout the cognitive tasks to examine the possible variations in effort engagement with time on task. For that reason, we recommend the use of continuous cognitive tasks with a minimal duration of 15 min. Finally, two resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging scans at rest should be planned, one before the beginning and the other after the end of the intervention. Then,seed-based correlation analyses should be conducted to examine the chronic effect of exercise on functional connectivity within and between the DMN,the SN,and the CEN.

    The second category of intervention focuses on the effects of cognitive training on PA adherence. The aim of the intervention would be to improve EFs and effortful control through cognitive training in a cohort of older adults and to observe the repercussions of this potential improvement on adherence to PA and other health-related behaviors in the current life of the participants. Why is it important to use cognitive training instead of physical training to improve EFs and effortful control in the validity test of the second causal relationship? Intervention studies that test the validity of the first causal relationship between chronic exercise and EFs generally use performance scores obtained in tasks engaging EFs and effortful control as the principal outcomes, i.e., tasks involving both CEN and SN, such as all tasks listed in Section 2. In addition, the effectiveness of the physical training program is generally validated through the improvement in physical fitness (e.g., increase in maximal oxygen consumption and/or increase in muscular strength). In contrast, in the validity test of the second causal relationship between EFs and adherence to exercise,the principal outcome of the RCT would be adherence to exercise and diet. In that case, it would not be suitable to use the same physical training program to improve the EFs and measure adherence to the physical training program as the main outcome. Indeed, it is problematic that the content of the physical training program is directly and intrinsically linked to adherence to this program. To clearly separate the cause and the consequence in the improvement in EF, it would be more pertinent to use cognitive training rather than physical training. However, a pertinent case of physical training planned to improve self-control strength leading to a transfer effect on a different physical exercise can be found in a study by Bray et al.69These investigators examined the effect of a 2-week training program including maximal isometric handgrip squeezing exercises on performance in a cardiovascular endurance cycling performance carried out just before the beginning and after the end of the program. They showed that participants who completed the 2-week self-control training program improved their performance in the intensity-graded cycling exercise by increasing their time to failure in comparison with the control participants. Although it seems adequate to use a physical training program to improve effortful control and examine the consequence of this improvement on exercise adherence, we recommend using cognitive training to enhance far-transfer effects rather than near-transfer effects. Several narrative and meta-analytic reviews have highlighted the effectiveness of cognitive training and mental stimulation to improve EF.70-72It should be noted that cognitive training programs have shown a greater effect size at improving EFs in comparison with physical training programs.73These recent findings from process-specific EF training indicate that all the key domains of EF can be improved by cognitive training,not only in childhood and adolescence,71but also in older adults.73There is also evidence for transfer from one EF component to other components of EF (e.g., from taskswitching training to working-memory abilities). The maintenance of the gains in executive functioning lasts several months. For instance, the benefit in working memory in oldold individuals after a verbal working-memory training program of 3 sessions completed within a 2-week timeframe persisted 8 months after the end of the intervention.74However, the extent to which these improvements generalize and show positive transfers to everyday life activities remains strongly debated. Generally, psychologists have focused transfer analyses on cognitive tasks very similar to the trained task (i.e., near-transfer effects) and on dissimilar cognitive tasks (far-transfer effects), but never specifically on ecological tasks concerned with behavioral change.Based on these reviews, we recommend planning an RCT that includes two arms. In the first arm, the group of participants would take part in a cognitive training program with a volume of at least 10 h (e.g., two 1-h sessions per week for 5 weeks) and using a variety of effortful cognitive tasks engaging EF. In the second arm, the other group of participants would take part in a sham cognitive training program of the same duration and consisting of viewing neutral emotion movies and answering questions about the movies. In parallel with the intervention, the participants in the 2 groups would be instructed to follow the recommendations of the WHO concerning PA and diet, for example, practicing at least 150 min of moderate intensity exercise per week and eating at least 5 portions of fruits and vegetables per day. The adherence to these recommendations would be regularly collected from 1 week before the beginning of the intervention to at least 3 months after the end of the intervention. Concerning the measurement of PA adherence, the main outcome of the RCT, we recommend the use of two instruments: a weekly/daily PA notebook and actimetry.

    Unfortunately, the virtuous circle is fragile, and, as mentioned in Section 4, it can be broken or even reversed when life accidents happen. In the case of circle reversal, it is more appropriate to talk about the initiation of a vicious circle that progressively leads the individual to a sedentary lifestyle, a weakening of physical and effort-based mental capacities,and sometimes health problems such as osteoporosis, obesity, or cardiovascular diseases.75,76It seems that physical, brain, and psychological capacities function synergistically, mutually reinforcing one another in the case of the virtuous circle, and conversely making the mobilization of the other more difficult in the case of the vicious circle. From that perspective, Allan et al.48proposed that the elements of the virtuous circle are linked by feedback loops. Our task in the future would be to clearly define environmental and individual factors that facilitate the rotation of the virtuous circle in the right direction and counteract the inverse rotation of the vicious circle. Finally, it is important to emphasize that the effort hypothesis presented in this article is not in opposition to the current and dominant neurotrophic hypothesis. Instead,it would be more appropriate to conceive that the mechanisms strengthening the large-scale networks underpinning effortful control and EF (the effort hypothesis) operate synergistically with the mechanisms enhancing brain plasticity (neurotrophic hypothesis).

    Acknowledgments

    We would like to thank Yu-Kai Chang and Jennifer Etnier for inviting us to submit a paper for this special topic,and Jennifer Etnier and Panteleimon Ekkekakis for their helpful comments on a previous version of this manuscript. The present review was supported by the French National Research Agency(ANR-12-MALZ-005-01).

    Authors’contributions

    MA and NA elaborated the plan of the article, made the review of literature, and drafted the manuscript. Both authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript,and agree with the order of presentation of the authors.

    Competing interests

    Both authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    女人十人毛片免费观看3o分钟| 建设人人有责人人尽责人人享有的 | 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 欧美3d第一页| 亚洲欧美清纯卡通| av在线天堂中文字幕| 一级毛片 在线播放| 三级国产精品欧美在线观看| 国产综合懂色| 国产视频内射| 丰满少妇做爰视频| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 亚洲av成人精品一二三区| 成年版毛片免费区| 精品一区二区三卡| 久久久久久久国产电影| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 国产一区二区在线观看日韩| 五月开心婷婷网| 18+在线观看网站| 亚洲精品乱码久久久v下载方式| 国产精品99久久99久久久不卡 | 少妇人妻精品综合一区二区| 我要看日韩黄色一级片| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 国产熟女欧美一区二区| 国产高清三级在线| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 亚洲最大成人av| 亚洲美女视频黄频| 国内精品美女久久久久久| 欧美变态另类bdsm刘玥| 新久久久久国产一级毛片| 禁无遮挡网站| 少妇被粗大猛烈的视频| 免费看光身美女| 美女视频免费永久观看网站| 嫩草影院入口| 菩萨蛮人人尽说江南好唐韦庄| 国产成人精品久久久久久| 免费人成在线观看视频色| av国产精品久久久久影院| 免费播放大片免费观看视频在线观看| 国产欧美亚洲国产| 中文字幕人妻熟人妻熟丝袜美| 亚洲欧美日韩另类电影网站 | 国产精品一区二区三区四区免费观看| 久久精品夜色国产| a级毛片免费高清观看在线播放| 成年女人看的毛片在线观看| 别揉我奶头 嗯啊视频| 国产精品99久久99久久久不卡 | 日日啪夜夜爽| 国国产精品蜜臀av免费| eeuss影院久久| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 日本熟妇午夜| 一级爰片在线观看| 日本黄色片子视频| 国产精品99久久99久久久不卡 | 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看 | 亚洲精品乱久久久久久| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 熟女电影av网| 成人美女网站在线观看视频| 婷婷色av中文字幕| 下体分泌物呈黄色| 久久6这里有精品| 欧美少妇被猛烈插入视频| 精品久久久久久久久亚洲| 精品国产一区二区三区久久久樱花 | 免费人成在线观看视频色| 亚洲精品中文字幕在线视频 | 免费黄网站久久成人精品| 99热6这里只有精品| 午夜福利视频1000在线观看| 2018国产大陆天天弄谢| 亚洲av成人精品一二三区| 午夜日本视频在线| 最近中文字幕2019免费版| 久久女婷五月综合色啪小说 | 三级男女做爰猛烈吃奶摸视频| 精品久久久噜噜| 午夜精品一区二区三区免费看| 中文字幕人妻熟人妻熟丝袜美| 国产人妻一区二区三区在| 精品一区在线观看国产| 国产成年人精品一区二区| 精品国产三级普通话版| 国产精品女同一区二区软件| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 亚洲高清免费不卡视频| 国产精品国产三级国产av玫瑰| 亚洲精品久久久久久婷婷小说| 99re6热这里在线精品视频| 国产成人精品一,二区| 日本三级黄在线观看| 亚洲图色成人| 国产精品一区www在线观看| 国产在视频线精品| 性色avwww在线观看| av卡一久久| 青春草亚洲视频在线观看| 激情五月婷婷亚洲| 日本爱情动作片www.在线观看| 深爱激情五月婷婷| 色视频在线一区二区三区| a级毛片免费高清观看在线播放| 精品午夜福利在线看| av国产久精品久网站免费入址| av在线天堂中文字幕| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 嫩草影院精品99| 久久久精品欧美日韩精品| 国产国拍精品亚洲av在线观看| 午夜福利高清视频| 成人免费观看视频高清| 久久精品国产a三级三级三级| 亚洲人与动物交配视频| a级毛色黄片| 亚洲成人久久爱视频| 简卡轻食公司| 色婷婷久久久亚洲欧美| 欧美成人一区二区免费高清观看| 一级毛片电影观看| 各种免费的搞黄视频| 国产一区二区三区av在线| 亚洲欧美精品自产自拍| 国产白丝娇喘喷水9色精品| 中文字幕久久专区| 高清毛片免费看| 97在线人人人人妻| 女人被狂操c到高潮| 美女cb高潮喷水在线观看| 69av精品久久久久久| 天美传媒精品一区二区| 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 久久ye,这里只有精品| 激情 狠狠 欧美| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 国产黄a三级三级三级人| 成人国产av品久久久| 亚洲精品久久久久久婷婷小说| 国产永久视频网站| av在线蜜桃| 99精国产麻豆久久婷婷| 九草在线视频观看| 日韩国内少妇激情av| 在线精品无人区一区二区三 | 一级爰片在线观看| 在线观看一区二区三区激情| 亚洲天堂国产精品一区在线| 高清在线视频一区二区三区| 久久久久久久久久久丰满| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 一区二区三区四区激情视频| 亚洲最大成人av| 一边亲一边摸免费视频| 爱豆传媒免费全集在线观看| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 日韩中字成人| 深夜a级毛片| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 99热国产这里只有精品6| 汤姆久久久久久久影院中文字幕| 边亲边吃奶的免费视频| 91午夜精品亚洲一区二区三区| 街头女战士在线观看网站| 免费看a级黄色片| 一区二区三区精品91| av播播在线观看一区| 欧美亚洲 丝袜 人妻 在线| 国产午夜精品久久久久久一区二区三区| 交换朋友夫妻互换小说| 一级毛片黄色毛片免费观看视频| 免费大片黄手机在线观看| 成人亚洲欧美一区二区av| 成年女人看的毛片在线观看| 2022亚洲国产成人精品| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 亚洲熟女精品中文字幕| 91aial.com中文字幕在线观看| 一区二区三区精品91| 男女下面进入的视频免费午夜| 亚洲欧美日韩无卡精品| 各种免费的搞黄视频| 亚洲av免费在线观看| 久久久久久久午夜电影| 欧美日韩视频高清一区二区三区二| 午夜老司机福利剧场| 简卡轻食公司| 国产黄色视频一区二区在线观看| 色婷婷久久久亚洲欧美| 一本一本综合久久| 亚洲精品第二区| 黄色视频在线播放观看不卡| 国产精品伦人一区二区| 亚洲久久久久久中文字幕| 欧美高清性xxxxhd video| 亚洲久久久久久中文字幕| 国产淫片久久久久久久久| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 天美传媒精品一区二区| 久久韩国三级中文字幕| 最近手机中文字幕大全| 99久久精品国产国产毛片| 国产精品熟女久久久久浪| 久久人人爽人人片av| 国产爽快片一区二区三区| 99久久人妻综合| 高清毛片免费看| 亚洲在线观看片| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验| 亚洲国产精品999| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添av毛片| 九草在线视频观看| 国产成人精品福利久久| 国产免费一级a男人的天堂| 国产精品成人在线| 草草在线视频免费看| 夫妻午夜视频| 18禁在线无遮挡免费观看视频| 三级国产精品片| 婷婷色av中文字幕| 日本三级黄在线观看| 新久久久久国产一级毛片| 国产色婷婷99| 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| 成年版毛片免费区| 纵有疾风起免费观看全集完整版| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线| 爱豆传媒免费全集在线观看| 久久ye,这里只有精品| 毛片一级片免费看久久久久| 最近手机中文字幕大全| 国产视频内射| 国产亚洲av片在线观看秒播厂| 免费大片黄手机在线观看| 久久热精品热| 黄片无遮挡物在线观看| 日韩大片免费观看网站| 久久精品久久精品一区二区三区| 好男人在线观看高清免费视频| 亚洲国产成人一精品久久久| av在线老鸭窝| 国产精品99久久久久久久久| av.在线天堂| 国产真实伦视频高清在线观看| 午夜激情久久久久久久| 69av精品久久久久久| 成年人午夜在线观看视频| 国产精品久久久久久精品古装| 99久久人妻综合| 小蜜桃在线观看免费完整版高清| 99精国产麻豆久久婷婷| 交换朋友夫妻互换小说| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 26uuu在线亚洲综合色| 97超视频在线观看视频| 夫妻午夜视频| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 精品一区二区三区视频在线| 九九爱精品视频在线观看| 真实男女啪啪啪动态图| 国产高清三级在线| 亚洲在久久综合| av网站免费在线观看视频| 日韩中字成人| 高清av免费在线| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 大片免费播放器 马上看| 国产精品一区www在线观看| 美女视频免费永久观看网站| 亚洲精品乱码久久久v下载方式| 国产伦精品一区二区三区四那| 人妻 亚洲 视频| 精品国产一区二区三区久久久樱花 | 国国产精品蜜臀av免费| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清| 一个人观看的视频www高清免费观看| 99久国产av精品国产电影| 美女视频免费永久观看网站| 国产又色又爽无遮挡免| 交换朋友夫妻互换小说| 美女国产视频在线观看| 综合色av麻豆| 中文乱码字字幕精品一区二区三区| 中文字幕免费在线视频6| 国产在线男女| 我的老师免费观看完整版| 日韩制服骚丝袜av| 777米奇影视久久| 卡戴珊不雅视频在线播放| 老司机影院成人| 欧美高清性xxxxhd video| 91精品一卡2卡3卡4卡| 纵有疾风起免费观看全集完整版| 中文欧美无线码| 午夜激情久久久久久久| 69av精品久久久久久| 免费在线观看成人毛片| 成人一区二区视频在线观看| 免费观看av网站的网址| 成人黄色视频免费在线看| 日韩强制内射视频| 亚洲国产色片| 亚洲av成人精品一区久久| 免费观看av网站的网址| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 黄片wwwwww| 夫妻午夜视频| 国国产精品蜜臀av免费| 五月天丁香电影| 香蕉精品网在线| 波多野结衣巨乳人妻| 免费大片黄手机在线观看| 久久久亚洲精品成人影院| 亚洲av男天堂| 日韩av免费高清视频| 日韩免费高清中文字幕av| 女的被弄到高潮叫床怎么办| 人妻一区二区av| 中文字幕av成人在线电影| 久久久久久久久大av| 亚洲色图综合在线观看| 狂野欧美激情性bbbbbb| 午夜精品一区二区三区免费看| 天堂中文最新版在线下载 | 国产一区亚洲一区在线观看| 丝袜美腿在线中文| 婷婷色av中文字幕| 人妻一区二区av| 久久久午夜欧美精品| 精品久久久久久久久av| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 欧美3d第一页| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合| 国产女主播在线喷水免费视频网站| 五月玫瑰六月丁香| 性色avwww在线观看| 又爽又黄a免费视频| 成人亚洲欧美一区二区av| 99热网站在线观看| 丝袜美腿在线中文| 成人亚洲精品av一区二区| 日韩在线高清观看一区二区三区| 在线看a的网站| 久久久久久久久大av| 亚洲婷婷狠狠爱综合网| 精品99又大又爽又粗少妇毛片| 成人高潮视频无遮挡免费网站| 亚洲精品aⅴ在线观看| 各种免费的搞黄视频| 欧美日韩国产mv在线观看视频 | 亚洲欧美一区二区三区国产| 国产免费又黄又爽又色| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 大码成人一级视频| 插阴视频在线观看视频| 日日啪夜夜爽| 国产男女超爽视频在线观看| 久久久久精品久久久久真实原创| 97在线视频观看| 五月天丁香电影| 欧美97在线视频| 国产白丝娇喘喷水9色精品| 黄片无遮挡物在线观看| 舔av片在线| 国产成人aa在线观看| 真实男女啪啪啪动态图| 成年版毛片免费区| 99久久中文字幕三级久久日本| 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 尤物成人国产欧美一区二区三区| 久久鲁丝午夜福利片| 深夜a级毛片| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 三级国产精品片| 波多野结衣巨乳人妻| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 精品午夜福利在线看| 在线 av 中文字幕| 日韩电影二区| 2021天堂中文幕一二区在线观| 午夜亚洲福利在线播放| 久久久久性生活片| 亚洲人成网站在线观看播放| 99精国产麻豆久久婷婷| 亚洲av成人精品一二三区| 天美传媒精品一区二区| 国产精品伦人一区二区| 日韩一区二区视频免费看| 亚洲精品视频女| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 天堂俺去俺来也www色官网| 国产探花极品一区二区| 精品国产乱码久久久久久小说| 嫩草影院入口| 久久久久久久国产电影| 精品国产露脸久久av麻豆| 一边亲一边摸免费视频| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 中文在线观看免费www的网站| 国产精品一区二区三区四区免费观看| 自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇| 少妇的逼好多水| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 黄色欧美视频在线观看| 边亲边吃奶的免费视频| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 好男人在线观看高清免费视频| 日韩欧美一区视频在线观看 | 免费观看无遮挡的男女| 美女高潮的动态| 亚洲最大成人中文| 3wmmmm亚洲av在线观看| 色视频在线一区二区三区| 婷婷色av中文字幕| 国产高清不卡午夜福利| 一本色道久久久久久精品综合| 人妻少妇偷人精品九色| 伦精品一区二区三区| 日韩制服骚丝袜av| 一本一本综合久久| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| 日韩欧美精品v在线| 69人妻影院| 最近最新中文字幕大全电影3| 免费观看av网站的网址| 中文精品一卡2卡3卡4更新| 一级黄片播放器| 久久鲁丝午夜福利片| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 黄色视频在线播放观看不卡| 欧美激情国产日韩精品一区| 亚洲精品,欧美精品| 日本av手机在线免费观看| av线在线观看网站| 亚洲成色77777| 亚洲欧美成人综合另类久久久| 久久97久久精品| 亚洲欧洲国产日韩| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 中文字幕人妻熟人妻熟丝袜美| 国内精品美女久久久久久| 欧美日韩视频高清一区二区三区二| 国产在线一区二区三区精| 男女啪啪激烈高潮av片| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 乱码一卡2卡4卡精品| 欧美亚洲 丝袜 人妻 在线| 毛片一级片免费看久久久久| 亚洲丝袜综合中文字幕| 亚洲av中文av极速乱| 亚洲,一卡二卡三卡| 国产成人a区在线观看| 亚洲成人精品中文字幕电影| 国语对白做爰xxxⅹ性视频网站| 伦精品一区二区三区| 国产一区亚洲一区在线观看| 人妻系列 视频| 国产午夜福利久久久久久| 一级二级三级毛片免费看| 高清在线视频一区二区三区| 午夜福利高清视频| 一区二区三区四区激情视频| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验| 搡女人真爽免费视频火全软件| 秋霞在线观看毛片| 亚洲国产高清在线一区二区三| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 大又大粗又爽又黄少妇毛片口| 在线观看一区二区三区| 简卡轻食公司| 成人亚洲精品一区在线观看 | 天堂中文最新版在线下载 | 99热全是精品| 国产老妇女一区| 亚洲av电影在线观看一区二区三区 | 国产免费又黄又爽又色| 国产 一区精品| 久久久久网色| 色综合色国产| 精品久久国产蜜桃| 97在线人人人人妻| 亚洲精品,欧美精品| 亚洲va在线va天堂va国产| 黄色一级大片看看| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 久久精品国产a三级三级三级| 日本与韩国留学比较| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看| 岛国毛片在线播放| 一级二级三级毛片免费看| 国产日韩欧美在线精品| 又大又黄又爽视频免费| 韩国av在线不卡| 插逼视频在线观看| 久久精品久久精品一区二区三区| 可以在线观看毛片的网站| 99久久精品热视频| 色视频www国产| 麻豆久久精品国产亚洲av| 日韩免费高清中文字幕av| 国产精品99久久久久久久久| 在线观看三级黄色| 成年av动漫网址| 免费看a级黄色片| 一区二区三区乱码不卡18| 99re6热这里在线精品视频| 国产成人精品婷婷| 国产91av在线免费观看| 亚洲精品日韩av片在线观看| 99热6这里只有精品| 亚洲欧美精品自产自拍| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 欧美变态另类bdsm刘玥| 最近最新中文字幕免费大全7| 美女视频免费永久观看网站| 亚洲av欧美aⅴ国产| 一级毛片我不卡| 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看 | 国产久久久一区二区三区| 2021天堂中文幕一二区在线观| av女优亚洲男人天堂| 成人免费观看视频高清| 国产精品女同一区二区软件| 亚洲丝袜综合中文字幕| 国产成人91sexporn| 日本黄色片子视频| 久久热精品热| 一区二区三区乱码不卡18| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区| 亚洲最大成人av| 日韩成人伦理影院| 国产精品女同一区二区软件| 内射极品少妇av片p| 色视频在线一区二区三区| 国产又色又爽无遮挡免| 精品午夜福利在线看| 亚洲不卡免费看| 国产真实伦视频高清在线观看| 亚洲成人久久爱视频| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 午夜福利视频精品| 搞女人的毛片| 欧美日韩视频高清一区二区三区二| 亚洲欧美成人精品一区二区| 少妇的逼好多水| 观看免费一级毛片| 国产伦精品一区二区三区视频9| 国产精品久久久久久精品古装| 午夜福利在线在线| 亚洲欧美日韩另类电影网站 | av在线老鸭窝| 亚洲av男天堂| 日本三级黄在线观看| 亚洲精品日韩av片在线观看| 人妻系列 视频| 午夜福利高清视频| 一区二区三区乱码不卡18| av福利片在线观看| 久久久精品欧美日韩精品| 国产欧美日韩精品一区二区| xxx大片免费视频| 久久久a久久爽久久v久久| 精品久久久久久电影网| 亚洲av在线观看美女高潮| 99热国产这里只有精品6| 在线亚洲精品国产二区图片欧美 | 亚洲欧美日韩东京热| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 免费av观看视频| 久久久精品94久久精品| 联通29元200g的流量卡| 亚州av有码| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 日日撸夜夜添| 国产日韩欧美亚洲二区| 亚洲国产av新网站| 直男gayav资源| 免费播放大片免费观看视频在线观看| 97精品久久久久久久久久精品| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 成人漫画全彩无遮挡| 午夜精品一区二区三区免费看| av在线观看视频网站免费| 人妻少妇偷人精品九色| 日韩人妻高清精品专区| 97精品久久久久久久久久精品| 日韩av在线免费看完整版不卡| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 91午夜精品亚洲一区二区三区| 少妇 在线观看| 欧美亚洲 丝袜 人妻 在线| 久久久久性生活片| 国产极品天堂在线| a级毛色黄片| 街头女战士在线观看网站| 男人狂女人下面高潮的视频| 成人黄色视频免费在线看| 特大巨黑吊av在线直播| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 免费黄网站久久成人精品| 美女国产视频在线观看| 国产日韩欧美亚洲二区| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 国产色婷婷99| 午夜激情久久久久久久| 联通29元200g的流量卡| 国产亚洲最大av| 国产伦理片在线播放av一区| 欧美性感艳星| 国产欧美亚洲国产|