• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Effects of music and video on perceived exertion during high-intensity exercise

    2017-04-10 02:54:15EnochChowJenniferEtnier
    Journal of Sport and Health Science 2017年1期

    Enoch C.Chow,Jennifer L.Etnier

    Department of Kinesiolog y,University of North Carolina at Greensboro,Greensboro,NC 27412,USA

    Original article

    Effects of music and video on perceived exertion during high-intensity exercise

    Enoch C.Chow,Jennifer L.Etnier*

    Department of Kinesiolog y,University of North Carolina at Greensboro,Greensboro,NC 27412,USA

    Background:Dissociative attentional stimuli(e.g.,music,video)are effective in decreasing ratings of perceived exertion(RPE)during low-tomoderate intensity exercise,but have inconsistent results during exercise at higher intensity.The purpose of this study was to assess attentional focus and RPE during high-intensity exercise as a function of being exposed to music,video,both(music and video),or a no-treatment control condition.

    Methods:During the firs session,healthy men(n=15)completed a maximal fitnes test to determine the workload necessary for high-intensity exercise(operationalized as 125%ventilatory threshold)to be performed during subsequent sessions.On 4 subsequent days,they completed 20 min of high-intensity exercise in a no-treatment control condition or while listening to music,watching a video,or both.Attentional focus,RPE, heart rate,and distance covered were measured every 4 min during the exercise.

    Results:Music and video in combination resulted in significanty lower RPE across time(partialη2=0.36)and the size of the effect increased over time(partialη2=0.14).Additionally,music and video in combination resulted in a significanty more dissociative focus than the other conditions (partialη2=0.29).

    Conclusion:Music and video in combination may result in lower perceived exertion during high-intensity exercise when compared to music or video in isolation.Future research will be necessary to test if reductions in perceived exertion in response to dissociative attentional stimuli have implications for exercise adherence.

    ?2017 Production and hosting by Elsevier B.V.on behalf of Shanghai University of Sport.This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

    Acute exercise;Attentional focus;Effort;Perceived exertion

    1.Introduction

    There are many physical and mental health benefit associated with participation in physical activity(PA).However, physical inactivity is widespread and has contributed to the increase in rates of obesity and chronic diseases.1Identifying ways to limit sedentary behavior,motivate individuals to become physically active,or encourage exercisers to continue their PA behaviors has developed into a popular area of research.One direction that this research has taken is to test the potential of music and/or video to influenc attention in a way that may ultimately result in a change in PA behavior.2–8

    The term“attention”describes the focus of an individual.9The direction of attention or attentional focus during PA has been described as ranging on a continuum from associative to dissociative and as potentially changing throughout the course of the activity.10When an individual has an associative focus,he or she concentrates on bodily sensations important for task performance,11such as breathing patterns,rhythm in movement,feelings of fatigue in the muscles,and/or heart rate(HR). In contrast,when using a dissociative focus,an individual concentrates on cues that are not relevant for task performance11such as auditory/visual stimuli and the environment,and this dissociative focus may provide distraction from his or her internal sensations(fatigue,breathing,and exertion).Based on the parallel-processing model and relevant to PA and exercise,perception is considered an active process that can influenc judgments of sensory cues.12According to this model,dissociative strategies can reduce ratings of perceived exertion(RPE)during exercise conducted at low-to-moderate intensities.This effect occurs because,while physiological input is important for judgments of exertion,dissociative strategies compete for thelimited channel capacity necessary to bring perceptions of exertion to awareness.Given this model,identifying ways to increase the extent to which an individual uses a dissociative focus during low-to-moderate intensity exercise may result in a significan reduction in perceived exertion,which may ultimately result in an acute benefi to PA behavior(i.e.,greater work output).One way to encourage a dissociative focus is through using music and/or video.13Research on music and video shows that each of these stimuli possesses dissociative attentional qualities that may result in an individual perceiving less exertion and increasing performance(work output)during PA.2–4

    Research conducted on the effectofmusic during PAsuggests that music can be used as an aid to reduce negative bodily sensations and perceived exertion.14,15However,consonant with the parallel-processing model,the benefit of music seem to be most consistently evident when an individual participates in low-tomoderate intensity PA.3,8,14–16When an individual engages in PA at higher intensity levels,music has been shown to lose its beneficia effect.16,17This is in accord with the parallel-processing model because this model suggests that there is a point at which exercise intensity ishigh enough thata person’sfocusnecessarily becomes more associative and at this intensity,external cues are no longer useful for maintaining a dissociative focus.

    A relevant question,of course,is what is meant by“high enough”with regard to the point at which internal bodily sensations dominate attentional capacity despite efforts to adopt a dissociative focus.The dual mode theory suggests that this critical point may be at ventilatory threshold(VT).18,19When an individual exercises below his or her VT,cognitive strategies (such as dissociating)are useful to maintain enjoyment of an activity and help an individual perceive that he or she is feeling better.However,when an individual exercises at an intensity level above VT,the dual-mode theory suggests that cognitive processes are more difficul to maintain,the focus shifts to an internal or associative focus due to bodily cues,and pleasure is reduced.

    This is further explained by Hutchinson and Tenenbaum10who indicate that the relationship between exercise intensity and music’s effect on exertion is due to a shift in attentional focus(dissociative to associative)when an individual progresses from low-to-moderate to high-intensity PA.They suggest that at low-to-moderate intensity,individuals can effectively utilize music as a dissociative strategy to distract their focus from their bodily and internal sensations(exertion, fatigue).However,if individuals begin to engage in highintensity activity,an individual’s focus shifts away from the distracter(music)to a more associative strategy(bodily sensations)and the combined effect of the increased physiological effort and the decreased ability to focus externally are reflecte in the observed increases in RPE.The result of this increase in perceived exertion is that individuals may perceive PA as too difficul or uncomfortable and the implication is that they may fail to maintain a habit of regular PA.Thus,understanding specificaly how music might be used to facilitate the use of a dissociative focus has implications for perceived exertion and performance.

    Research examining the effect of video on PA is less prevalent than research on music effects.6,7,9,13,20However,from the studies exploring the effects of video in the absence of music,it has also been shown to reduce RPE during exercise.7,9,20A limited number of studies2,5–7have also compared the combined effects of music and video to the effects of music or video in isolation based on the premise that the 2 in combination may have a stronger effect on attentional focus and RPE.2

    Barwood et al.2examined the effects of no-treatment,a nonmotivational video,and a combination of a motivational video and music on performance of a 30 min run consisting of a 15 min warm-up and a 15 min run at maximal effort(average HR=179.38 bpm)performed in a warm(~26°C)moist(~50% relative humidity)environment.Results showed that participants ran farther in the music and video condition than in either other condition despite reporting similar levels of RPE.Based on participants running farther without experiencing an increase in RPE,Barwood et al.2concluded that combined music and video did have a beneficia effect on perceived exertion during maximal exertion exercise,apparently countering finding of previous literature.However,even if one accepts Barwood et al.’s interpretation of their findings their study design negates the ability to attribute their finding to the unique effects of music and video in combination.In particular, their independent variable was actually motivational quality (absent,non-motivational,and motivational)rather than comparing combined music and video effects to effects of either in isolation.

    In another study designed to explore the combined effects of music and video,Lin and Lu7asked participants to exercise“as hard as possible”on a stationary bicycle for 12 min while in no-treatment,music,video,and combined music and video conditions.Results indicated that HR was equivalent across the 4 conditions(range of means=174–178 bpm).However,RPE were significanty less in all treatment conditions as compared to the control condition,and the amount of work completed was significanty greater in the combined condition than in the control condition.Importantly,this is the firs study to demonstrate that the effects of combined music and video differ from those of music or video in isolation when participants exercise at a high-intensity level(average HR=88%age-predicted maximum HR,JH Lin,personal communication,February 27, 2015).However,there are 2 limitations of this study.First,the researchers did not assess the effects on attentional focus,hence limiting our ability to identify the role of attentional focus in this relationship.Second,because participants were asked to exercise at a particular level of perceived exertion(i.e.,“as hard as possible”),the influenc of the combined intervention on perceived exertion actually had the potential to influenc the exercise intensity itself and this is clearly a threat to the internal validity of the study.

    Two studies have tested the effects of music and video in combination on attentional focus and/or RPE at objectivelydetermined high intensity levels.Jones et al.6asked participants to exercise for 10 min at+10%VT(average HR=150.44 bpm) and?5%VT(average HR=131.87 bpm)under conditionswhere they listened to music,watched video,had the combination of music and video,or were in a control condition.Results indicated that attention was most dissociative in the combined condition as compared to the other 3 conditions and this effect was evident regardless of exercise intensity.However,one limitation of this study is that they did not include measures of RPE so it is not clear if this dissociative focus was reflecte in a decreased perception of exertion.

    Hutchinson et al.5used a within-subjects design to compare the effects of music,music and video,and no-treatment on exercise performance at?10VT(target HR=148.4 bpm)and at +10VT(target HR=181.5 bpm).Their results showed that regardless of exercise intensity,the music and video condition resulted in significanty lower RPE as compared to the no-treatment condition.Additionally,irrespective of exercise intensity,the extent to which the participants were able to dissociate was significanty greater for music and video as compared to music only which was also significanty greater than the no-treatment condition.The limitation of this study is that the researchers did not include a video only condition,so their results do not allow for conclusions as to whether or not music and video is better than video alone in terms of its impact on attentional focus and RPE.

    Given the limitations of the extant literature exploring the possibility that music and video in combination can benefi exercise performance and perceived exertion during exercise above VT,the purpose of this study was to use a complete design to compare the effects of music,video,and the combination of music and video on attentional focus and RPE during high-intensity exercise.This study was designed to address the limitations of the past research in 3 ways:(1)by measuring attentional focus and RPE,(2)by including all 4 possible combinations of music(present/absent)and video(present/absent), and(3)by using an objective and self-referenced exercise intensity to ensure that participants were exercising at a highintensity(operationalized as+25%VT).It was hypothesized that participants in the combined music and video treatment group would report a more dissociative focus and lower RPE when compared to the music only,video only,or control conditions.Based on past research and the parallel processing model,it was further hypothesized that at this high-intensity of exercise,music alone and video alone would have a negligible impact on attentional focus and RPE as compared to the control condition.

    2.Methods

    2.1.Participants

    Healthy male participants(n=15;age:21.87±1.41 years, mean±SD)were recruited from a large university in the southeastern United States for participation.We limited the sample to men because we were concerned that even regularly active female participants might not be able to complete the exercise protocol on the stationary cycle at+25%VT because of their lesser muscle mass in the legs.All participants signed a consent form(approved by the institutional review board)and confi med that they were healthy enough to participate in PA using the American College of Sports Medicine’s(ACSM’s)Preparticipation Questionnaire(2006).Our inclusion criteria required that if a participant had a prior history of health problems(heart disease,pulmonary disease,and stroke),was physically unable to perform the cycling task,was taking medication (e.g.,for asthma)that could influenc exertion or coordination during PA,or had a condition that limited maximal output during PA,he would not be permitted to participate without consent from a medical doctor.However,all of our participants were healthy enough to participate without a physician’s consent.All participants had to be regularly active(30 min/ week at least 3 time/week)for 6 months prior to beginning the study.This was assessed using the Godin Leisure Time Exercise Questionnaire.21This requirement was necessary to ensure that participants would be able to complete the high-intensity exercise sessions.

    2.2.Materials and measures

    2.2.1.RPE

    During all sessions,a participant’s RPE was measured using the Borg RPE scale22as a measure of perceived exercise intensity.This scale ranges from 6(very,very light)to 20(very,very hard).Scherr et al.23reported strong correlations between RPE and HR(r=0.74)and between RPE and blood lactate (r=0.83)during incremental exercise and concluded that RPE could be used as an accurate tool to monitor an individual’s exercise intensity.

    2.2.2.Attention

    Attentional focus was measured using a visual analog scale ranging from“0”indicating pure dissociation to“10”indicating pure association.3,24Attention was measured in millimeters (mm);thus,a 100 mm vertical line separated the numeric anchors.Participants were asked to draw a line perpendicular to the line reflectin their attentional focus.The location of this perpendicular line was measured starting from the 0 mm mark. Fifty millimeters was considered a neutral focus where a participant was equally dissociating and associating while cycling. Any measurement less than 50 mm was considered a dissociative focus,while a measurement more than 50 mm was considered an associative focus.

    2.2.3.HR

    Participants were fitte with a Polar HR monitor(Model F6; Polar Electro,Lake Success,NY,USA)prior to beginning the exercise.Participants were asked to wear the HR monitor and sit quietly for 5 min.Upon completion of the sitting period, baseline HR was assessed.HR was then assessed after the 5 min warm-up(0 min)and every 4 min during the 20 min exercise bout.

    2.2.4.Distance

    The distance covered(in kilometers)during the exercise session was recorded after the 5 min warm-up(0 min)and every 4 min during the 20 min exercise bout.Distance covered by the participant was displayed on a digital screen located on the bicycle such that only the researcher could view the screen.

    2.2.5.Exercise intensity

    Exercise intensity was determined relative to VT calculated following a maximal exercise test.This was done because VT is thought to be critical in determining the extent to which central physiological cues dominate over external psychological factors.16,18Participants were fitte with a facemask that covered their nose and mouth and that allowed for the capture of exhaled air by the Viasys Vmax Encore 29c metabolic cart (SensorMedics,Yorba Linda,CA,USA).Participantsperformed a ramped exercise protocol in which they started exercising at 100 W for 3 min and resistance was increased by 25 W every 3 min untilvolitionalexhaustion.VT wascalculated by the Vmax Series Software V20-1a(Viasys Health Care,Yorba Linda,CA, USA)from data collected by the metabolic cartduring the graded exercise test.Thissoftware system usesdualcriteria ofthe ratio of pulmonary ventilation to oxygen consumption(Ve/VO2)and the ratio of pulmonary ventilation to carbon dioxide production(Ve/ VCO2)to identify VT.The VT identifie by the software was confi med for the firs 5 participants using the modifie V-slope method which requires visual inspection of the test data plotted with VO2on thex-axisand Ve on they-axisto identify the pointat which the slope of the relationship changes from linear to exponential.After a participant’s VO2maxand VT were calculated,the participant’s workload(resistance on the ergometerin Watts)that corresponded to high intensity(125%VT)was identified In the rare instances(n=2)when the participant’s 125%VT fell in between 2 workloads,the researcher identifie the workload which corresponded to 70%VO2max(another measure of high intensity)and this was used as the workload for the subsequent exercise sessions.The ACSM guidelines25classify 125%VT and/or 70%VO2maxas high-intensity exercise.The maximal exercise test and all exercise sessions were performed on a Corival Lode recumbent bicycle ergometer(Lode,Groningen,the Netherlands).

    2.2.6.Treatment conditions

    In all treatment conditions,the fina 20 min(before credits containing songs“Urban”and“Jardin Chinois”)of Cirque du Soleil:La Noubawas selected and presented to the participants. This particular choice for the treatments was selected because the music and video had audio or visual consistency when presented in isolation,and when combined the stimulus was still harmonious and did not give the perception of being disjointed.The entertainment selection also did not have any vocals and the music was an acoustic-based instrumental.All participants selected a comfortable volume level for the headphones(QuietComfort?noise-canceling headphones;BOSE GmbH,Friedrichsdorf,Germany)during their firs session and then in future exercise sessions,this same volume was used.

    In the music condition,participants listened to music through noise-canceling headphones.Participants only heard the music and were not able to view the video.In the video condition,participants watched the segment of video on a computer monitor(Apple MacBook 13″×8.5″display;Apple Inc., Cupertino,CA,USA).While viewing the video segment,participants wore noise-canceling headphones but no music or noise was projected through them.In the music and video combined condition,participants listened to the music through noise-canceling headphones and viewed the video segment simultaneously.In the control condition,participants were fitte with noise-canceling headphones but no sound was projected through them and the television remained turned off.

    2.3.Procedures

    The study consisted of 5 sessions for each participant.Every session was separated by at least 48 h and occurred during the same hour of the day.Participants were asked not to engage in any PA 2 h prior to testing,not to engage in high-intensity PA at all on the day of the test,and not to eat or consume caffeine 3 h before the test.

    On the firs day ofthe study,participantssigned the consentand completed questionnaires.Participants were then equipped with an HR monitor and their baseline HR was measured.After this measure was taken,they began the ramped exercise test.Participants were asked to cycle on a stationary bicycle at 80–95 rpm, and the workload on the bicycle was set at 100 W for all participants.Every 3 min the workload was increased by 25 W until the participant reached his volitional exhaustion.During the last minute of every 3 min interval,RPE,attentional focus,HR,and distance were measured.The graded exercise testconcluded when the participantcould no longertolerate an increase in workload or maintain the minimumcycling cadence of70 rpmand volitionally terminated the test.Participants were deemed to have reached a“true”VO2maxifthey metatleast2 of3 criteriaforthemaximaltest (RPE over 17,HR over 170 which equates to approximately 85% age-predicted max forthissample,respiratory quotientover1.10). All participants met this requirement.Upon completion of the VO2maxtest,participants were randomly assigned to one of 4 possible orders of presentation of the 4 treatment conditions.

    During the 4 experimentalsessions,allparticipants performed the same cycling task;only the treatmentdiffered.Allparticipants were given an HRmonitorand proceeded to the stationary bicycle. The cart holding the video projector and the stationary bicycle were positioned in the exact same location for every session regardless if a participant was exposed to a treatment.The video projector was a 13-inch fla screen computer monitor positioned on a cartapproximately 3.5 feetfrom the seatofthe bicycle and at eyelevelofa seated participant.The monitorwasslightly offsetto the rightby 2 inchesso thatthe bicycle pedalswould notstrike the cart and so that the monitor was in the direct eye line of the participant.Participants were free to adjust the seat before an exercise bout to compensate for their height.During the 5 min warm-up,exercise intensity wasgradually raised to the calculated workload reflectin 125%VT and thistime-pointwasrecorded as the0 min measurement.Allparticipantsmaintained cycling atthis workload and pedaled at a speed of between 80 and 95 rpm for additional 20 min.RPE,HR,attentional focus,and distance were recorded during the last40 s ofevery 4 min segmentuntilconclusion of the 20 min.If a participant was not maintaining his work rate at 80–95 rpm,he was encouraged to increase or decrease his cycling speed in order to return to the desired rpm range.In all cases,workload was reduced by 10 W if the participant could not maintain the rpm requirementso thatparticipantscould complete 20 min ofactivity.Thisoccurred for2 participantson 2 occasions.Verbal cues were not otherwise given to a participant during a session.When participants completed all experimental conditions,they were debriefed and compensated USD50 for completing the study.

    2.4.Statistical analysis

    The study used a randomized-conditions within-subjects experimental design.Given the design of the study,a power analysis conducted using G*Power 3.0.1026indicated that withn=16,there would be sufficien statistical power(β=0.80)to observe a statistically significan effect(α<0.05)with an observed medium-to-large effect(f=0.30).Data for distance and HRwere evaluated using two-way repeated measuresanalyses of variance(RMANOVAs)to test the effects of treatment (control,music,video,and combined)and time(0 min,4 min, 8 min,12 min,16 min,20 min).Data for RPE and attentional focus were evaluated using two-way RMANOVAs to test the effects of treatment(control,music,video,and combined)and time(4 min,8 min,12 min,16 min,and 20 min).Mauchly’stest of sphericity was examined for all effects and when necessary a Huynh–Feldt adjustment was made.Post hocanalyses were conducted for significan main effects and simple effects were compared for significan interactions.Analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics Version 21.0(IBM Corp.,Armonk, NY,USA).

    3.Results

    Descriptive data for HR,RPE,attentional focus,and work completed for each condition and at each measurement point are presented in Table 1.

    3.1.Distance

    Distance was not normally distributed for any combination of Treatment×Time.Hence,the necessary assumptions for repeated measures ANOVA were not satisfied For this reason,we only present descriptive data in Table 1 to illustrate that the distance covered increased as time increased on the cycling task from the end of the 5 min warm-up (0 min:1.93±0.07 kg,mean±SE)through the 20 min of exercise (4 min: 3.85±0.12 kg; 8 min: 5.98±0.16 kg; 12 min:8.23±0.26 kg;16 min:10.52±0.29 kg;20 min: 12.53±0.35 kg)with the increase over time remaining steady across the various time periods.

    3.2.HR

    There were no significan differences in HR as a function of the interaction of Treatment×Time(F(8.87,124.22)=1.18,p>0.32,partialη2=0.08.)As expected,there was a significan difference in HR as a function of time(F(1.29,18.01)=442.73,p<0.001,partialη2=0.97),but there was no significan difference in HR as a function of treatment(F(3,42)=0.38,p>0.77, partialη2=0.03).The main effect for time indicated that HR increased significanty from baseline(73.47±1.79 bpm) to the firs 4 min of exercise(144.30±3.31 bpm)and then continued to increase significanty as time engaged on the cycling task increased(8 min:151.03±3.72 bpm;12 min: 154.30±3.89 bpm; 16 min: 157.22±3.92 bpm; 20 min: 160.23±3.93 bpm).

    3.3.RPE

    There was a significan interaction between treatment and time(F(7.67,107.42)=2.34,p=0.03,partialη2=0.14).In particular,from 8 min to 12 min and from 16 min to 20 min, RPE increased less during the combined condition(8 min–12 min:Mchange=0.67;16 min–20 min:Mchange=0.00)than it did for the other 3 treatment conditions(8 min–12 min: Mchange=1.22;16 min–20 min:Mchange=0.62)(Fig.1A).

    Table 1 Descriptive statistics by measurement time and condition(mean±SD).

    Fig.1.Means±SE values for(A)ratings of perceived exertion(RPE)and(B) attentional focus as a function of the significan interaction of time by treatment.Anchors for the attentional focus scale are included on they-axis. *Combined group is significanty different from all 3 other groups;#Combined group is significanty different from the music only and video only groups;?Combined group is significanty different from control group;?Combined group is significanty different from the control group and the music only group.

    3.4.Attentional focus

    There was no significan interaction between treatment and time(F(12,168)=1.25,p=0.26,partialη2=0.08).There were significan main effects for treatment(F(3,42)=5.68,p=0.002,partialη2=0.29)and time(F(1.38,19.30)=81.08,p<0.001, partialη2=0.85). Attentional focus was significanty(p<0.05)more dissociative in the combined condition (41.92±4.21)when compared to the music only(57.83±4.66),video only(54.16±4.68),and control (64.23±5.34)conditions.There was no significan difference in attentional focus between the music only,video only,and control conditions(p>0.05).The main effect for time showed that an individual’s attentional focus shifted significanty toward a more associative focus as time engaged in the cycling task increased (4 min: 31.03±3.78; 8 min: 45.30±3.50;12 min:58.10±3.67;16 min:66.67±3.87; 20 min:71.57±3.53)(Fig.1B).

    4.Discussion

    The parallel-processing model predicts that dissociative strategies that focus attention away from task-relevant bodily cues will compete with physiological input for access to limited attentional challenges and will result in the perception of less exertion during low-to-moderate intensity exercise.However,at higher intensities of exercise,physiological cues are expected to dominate attention to the extent that dissociative strategies lose their effectiveness.12It has been suggested that by combining music and video,perceived exertion may be lowered even at higher intensities of exercise.2However,research examining the potential benefit of presenting music and video in combination is very limited,2,5–7and there is no past research in which defini tive statements could be drawn regarding the potential additive effects of music and video in combination on attentional focus and RPE during high-intensity exercise.Hence,the purpose of this study was to examine if the presentation of combined music and video while cycling would influenc an individual’s perceived exertion during high-intensity exercise.

    Results of this study support previous research2,16,17in that during high-intensity exercise,music or video in isolation is not effective at influencin attentional focus or RPE in comparison to a control condition.As previous researchers have noted10and concordant with the parallel-processing model,12it is likely that at this higher intensity of exercise,an associative focus begins to dominate as the physiological demands of the exercise become increasingly attention-demanding.However,the find ings also indicated that when participants were presented with music and video simultaneously,they reported a significanty lower RPE when compared to the other treatment conditions (music only,video only,and control),despite exercising at the same workload,having the same HR response,and covering the same virtual distance.Importantly,this effect tended to increase with time so that as the duration of higher intensity exercise increased,the benefit of the combined music and video actually became more pronounced.This findin supports our hypothesis that participants would report a lower RPE when presented with music and video simultaneously compared to the other conditions.We also hypothesized that participants would report a more dissociative focus throughout the cycling task in the combined condition than when given music only, video only,or the control condition.The results also supported this hypothesis as participants rated their attentional focus as significanty more dissociative in the combined condition than when compared to the other conditions.This provides an important extension to the literature because the finding demonstrate that music and video in combination do provide greater benefitto exercisers than either provided in isolation even when performing high-intensity exercise.

    The results of this study demonstrate that participants cycling for 20 min at an intensity level above VT were unable to maintain a dissociative attention style when presented with music only,video only,or control conditions.This was determined through the participants’rating of attentional focus at each 4 min time point.In all conditions,the main effect for time indicated that attentional focus became increasingly less dissociative over the course of the 20 min exercise session(Fig.1B). However,when in the combined condition,participants started out with a focus that was much more dissociative than when in the other conditions,and hence they were able to maintain a dissociative or neutral focus through 12 min and their focus only became slightly associative by 16 min.In contrast,when in the other 3 conditions,participants switched from a dissociative to an associative focus by the 12 min point.Thus,it appears that the combined music and video provided an attentional distraction that kept participants’focus more dissociative and this occurred in conjunction with the reporting of a lower RPE. Future research will be necessary to determine if the change in attentional focus over time due to external distracters mediates the delayed onset of perceived exertion over time.

    Before discussing the conclusions of this study,it is important to consider the limitations.The firs limitation is that the sample was a relatively homogeneous group consisting of young,regularly active men.Hence,the results may not generalize to other samples,and future research is necessary to determine if these effects are possible in other populations. Second,the specifi type of music/video that was selected was purposefully selected to ensure that treatment conditions only differed in the intended fashion(i.e.,due to the level of the independent variable).Future research will be necessary to see if other music/video combinations that differ in terms of potentially relevant variables(e.g.,beat,motivational quality,preference,and synchronicity)produce similar effects as were found with this particular selection.Third,although we chose an intensity of exercise that is considered to be hard(highintensity)according to ACSM guidelines,25obviously there are higher intensities of exercise at which we might not see the same beneficia effects of music and video in combination. Relatedly,because this level of exercise intensity is not one that would likely be chosen by a recreational exerciser,the implications relative to enhancing exercise adherence through music and video remain to be explored.Lastly,although it has been suggested that the effects of music/video on RPE are mediated by attentional focus,the within-subjects design of this study limited our ability to statistically test for mediation.Future study using a between-subjects design and appropriate meditational techniques will be necessary to assess the extent to which differences in attentional focus actually explain differences in RPE.

    5.Conclusion

    Based on the finding of this research,it is concluded that young,regularly active men engaging in a 20 min bout of highintensity exercise are ableto use music and video in combination as a method to maintain a dissociative focus and perceive significanty lower exertion during their exercise bout.This findin is important because it suggests that by combining video and music,benefit can be obtained at a higher intensity of exercise than previously observed.If these effects are replicated and found to extend to other populations,the implication is that combined video and music may serve to lessen the perceived exertion required for a high-intensity exercise bout and that a mediatorofthiseffectisattentionalfocus.Onelogicalfollow-up to this research would be to explore whether or not this effect on perceived exertion hasimplicationsforexercise adherence.Ifan exerciserperceivesthathe isworking lesshard,itispossible that he willexercise forlongerorthathe willreturn to exerciseagain. Furthermore,given that the current guidelines for PA25include vigorousintensity exercise asone option formeeting recommendations,understanding ways to reduce perceptions of exertion during high-intensity exercise may have important public health implications.Although future study in this area is necessary and encouraged,the finding of this study make an important extension to the literature by demonstrating that music and video can be used in combination to reduce RPE even when exercising ata high intensity.

    Acknowledgment

    This research was supported by the Theodore&Loretta Williams Graduate Research Award Fund for Arts Health at the University of North Carolina at Greensboro.The funding agency did not have any input with regards to any aspect of study design,study conductance,or manuscript preparation or submission.

    Authors’contributions

    ECC conceived of the study,collected and analyzed the data, and drafted the manuscript;JLE provided input into the design of the study,assisted with data analysis and interpretation,and provided input and editorial feedback throughout the writing and revising of the manuscript.Both authors have read and approved the fina version of the manuscript,and agree with the order of presentation of the authors.

    Competing interests

    Neither of the authors declare competing financia interests.

    1.Booth FW,Hawley JA.The erosion of physical activity in Western societies:an economic death march.Diabetologia 2015;58:1730–4.

    2.Barwood MJ,Weston NJV,Thelwell R,Page J.A motivational music and video intervention improves high-intensity exercise performance.J Sports Sci Med 2009;8:435–42.

    3.Razon S,Basevitch I,Land WM,Thompson B,Tenenbaum G.Perception of exertion and attention allocation as a function of visual and auditory conditions.Psychol Sport Exerc 2009;10:636–43.

    4.Annesi JJ.Effects of music,television,and a combination entertainment system on distraction,exercise adherence,and physical output in adults. Can J Behav Sci 2001;33:193–202.

    5.Hutchinson JC,Karageorghis CI,Jones L.See hear:psychological effects of music and music-video during treadmill running.Ann Behav Med 2015;49:199–211.

    6.Jones L,Karageorghis CI,Ekkekakis P.Can high-intensity exercise be more pleasant?:attentional dissociation using music and video.J Sport Exerc Psychol 2014;36:528–41.

    7.Lin JH,Lu FJH.Interactive effects of visual and auditory intervention on physical performance and perceived effort.J Sports Sci Med 2013;12: 388–93.

    8.Potteiger JA,Schroeder JM,Goff KL.Influenc of music on ratings of perceived exertion during 20 minutes of moderate intensity exercise. Percept Mot Skills 2000;91:848–54.

    9.Stanley CT,Pargman D,Tenenbaum G.The effect of attentional coping strategies on perceived exertion in a cycling task.J Appl Sport Psychol 2007;19:352–63.

    10.Hutchinson JC,Tenenbaum G.Attention focus during physical effort:the mediating role of task intensity.Psychol Sport Exerc 2007;8:233–45.

    11.Stevinson CD,Biddle SJH.Cognitive orientations in marathon running and“hitting the wall”.Br J Sports Med 1998;32:229–35.

    12.Rejeski WJ.Perceived exertion:an active or passive process.J Sport Psychol 1985;7:371–8.

    13.Nethery VM.Competition between internal and external sources of information during exercise:influenc on RPE and the impact of the exercise load.J Sports Med Phys Fitness 2002;42:172–8.

    14.Karageorghis CI,Terry PC.The psychophysical effects of music in sport and exercise:a review.J Sport Behav 1997;20:54–68.

    15.Karageorghis CI,Terry PC,Lane AM,Bishop DT,Priest DL.The BASES Expert Statement on use of music in exercise.J Sports Sci 2012;30:953–6.

    16.Boutcher SH,Trenske M.The effects of sensory deprivation and music on perceived exertion and affect during exercise.J Sport Exerc Psychol 1990;12:167–76.

    17.Schwartz SE,Fernhall B,Plowman SA.Effects of music on exercise performance.J Cardiopulm Rehabil Prev 1990;10:312–6.

    18.Ekkekakis P.The dual-mode theory of affective responses to exercise in metatheoretical context:I.Initial impetus,basic postulates,and philosophical framework.Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol 2009;2:73–94.

    19.Ekkekakis P.The dual-mode theory of affective responses to exercise in metatheoretical context:II.Bodiless heads,ethereal cognitive schemata, and other improbable dualistic creatures,exercising.Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol 2009;2:139–60.

    20.Schücker L,Anheier W,Hagemann N,Strauss B,V?lker K.On the optimal focus of attention for efficien running at high-intensity.Sport Exerc Perform Psychol 2013;2:207–19.

    21.Godin G,Shephard RJ.A simple method to assess exercise behavior in the community.Can J Appl Sport Sci 1985;10:141–6.

    22.Borg GA.Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion.Med Sci Sports Exerc 1982;14:377–81.

    23.Scherr J,Wolfarth B,Christle JW,Pressler A,Wagenpfeil S, Halle M.Associations between Borg’s rating of perceived exertion and physiological measures of exercise intensity.Eur J Appl Physiol 2013;113:147–55.

    24.Tammen VV.Elite middle and long distance runners associative/ dissociative coping.J Appl Sport Psychol 1996;8:1–8.

    25.American College of Sports Medicine.Guidelines for exercise testing and prescription.Philadelphia,PA:Lippincott Williams&Wilkins; 2006.

    26.Faul F,Erdfelder E,Lang AG,Buchner A.G*Power 3:a fl xible statistical power analysis program for the social,behavioral,and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 2007;39:175–91.

    Received 8 April 2015;revised 3 September 2015;accepted 11 October 2015 Available online 5 January 2016

    Peer review under responsibility of Shanghai University of Sport.

    *Corresponding author.

    E-mail address:jletnier@uncg.edu(J.L.Etnier).

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jshs.2015.12.007

    2095-2546/?2017 Production and hosting by Elsevier B.V.on behalf of Shanghai University of Sport.This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

    19禁男女啪啪无遮挡网站| 中文字幕高清在线视频| 久久久久久久精品精品| 99国产精品99久久久久| 啦啦啦中文免费视频观看日本| 国产免费现黄频在线看| 久久人人爽av亚洲精品天堂| 国产精品一区二区在线不卡| 制服人妻中文乱码| 国产人伦9x9x在线观看| 久久综合国产亚洲精品| 久久午夜综合久久蜜桃| 亚洲国产精品一区二区三区在线| 女性生殖器流出的白浆| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 性色av一级| 丝瓜视频免费看黄片| 国产精品香港三级国产av潘金莲 | 精品国产乱码久久久久久小说| 亚洲欧美精品自产自拍| 色94色欧美一区二区| 欧美日韩亚洲综合一区二区三区_| 男女免费视频国产| 免费人妻精品一区二区三区视频| 人人妻人人澡人人看| 国产一区亚洲一区在线观看| 国产黄色免费在线视频| 久久鲁丝午夜福利片| 国产精品二区激情视频| 国产精品久久久人人做人人爽| 亚洲第一青青草原| 天天添夜夜摸| 久久国产精品大桥未久av| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清 | 欧美精品一区二区大全| 伦理电影免费视频| 欧美日韩黄片免| 亚洲成人国产一区在线观看 | 久久久久久久大尺度免费视频| 人妻一区二区av| 又大又爽又粗| www.熟女人妻精品国产| 国产精品一区二区精品视频观看| 熟女av电影| 久久99一区二区三区| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清 | 亚洲精品第二区| 国产成人欧美| videos熟女内射| 亚洲人成网站在线观看播放| 五月开心婷婷网| 欧美精品一区二区免费开放| 91精品伊人久久大香线蕉| 亚洲精品国产av蜜桃| 成年人免费黄色播放视频| 亚洲色图 男人天堂 中文字幕| 我的亚洲天堂| 成年av动漫网址| 国产精品九九99| 亚洲人成电影免费在线| 亚洲 欧美一区二区三区| 久久久久久久国产电影| 美女高潮到喷水免费观看| 欧美日韩一级在线毛片| av国产久精品久网站免费入址| 乱人伦中国视频| 色94色欧美一区二区| 少妇人妻久久综合中文| 亚洲精品国产av成人精品| 男女午夜视频在线观看| 99精国产麻豆久久婷婷| 中文字幕色久视频| 久热爱精品视频在线9| 人妻 亚洲 视频| 天天躁夜夜躁狠狠久久av| 丰满人妻熟妇乱又伦精品不卡| 亚洲欧美精品综合一区二区三区| 久热这里只有精品99| 久久免费观看电影| 精品少妇久久久久久888优播| 丝袜在线中文字幕| 最近中文字幕2019免费版| 欧美黄色淫秽网站| 一级黄色大片毛片| 国产精品.久久久| 国产成人av教育| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 国产淫语在线视频| 国产精品偷伦视频观看了| 久久人人爽av亚洲精品天堂| 悠悠久久av| 欧美激情极品国产一区二区三区| 午夜老司机福利片| 中文字幕精品免费在线观看视频| 80岁老熟妇乱子伦牲交| 欧美亚洲 丝袜 人妻 在线| 中文精品一卡2卡3卡4更新| 免费日韩欧美在线观看| 一区福利在线观看| 亚洲天堂av无毛| 午夜福利视频精品| 欧美97在线视频| 日本91视频免费播放| 天堂中文最新版在线下载| 2018国产大陆天天弄谢| 亚洲欧美精品综合一区二区三区| 精品少妇黑人巨大在线播放| 99久久精品国产亚洲精品| 又黄又粗又硬又大视频| 午夜福利视频在线观看免费| 国产精品九九99| 激情视频va一区二区三区| 久久久久网色| 丰满饥渴人妻一区二区三| 91字幕亚洲| 久久久欧美国产精品| 99国产综合亚洲精品| 一区在线观看完整版| 亚洲天堂av无毛| 国产精品一区二区在线不卡| 侵犯人妻中文字幕一二三四区| av线在线观看网站| 麻豆av在线久日| 50天的宝宝边吃奶边哭怎么回事| 女人高潮潮喷娇喘18禁视频| 久久久久久久久久久久大奶| 中文字幕av电影在线播放| 人人妻,人人澡人人爽秒播 | 久久久久精品国产欧美久久久 | 亚洲精品一区蜜桃| 90打野战视频偷拍视频| 日本a在线网址| videos熟女内射| 1024香蕉在线观看| 精品人妻熟女毛片av久久网站| 19禁男女啪啪无遮挡网站| 久久精品国产亚洲av高清一级| 国产成人一区二区三区免费视频网站 | 看免费成人av毛片| 亚洲精品日韩在线中文字幕| 可以免费在线观看a视频的电影网站| 高潮久久久久久久久久久不卡| 精品国产国语对白av| 晚上一个人看的免费电影| 男女床上黄色一级片免费看| 国产精品久久久久久人妻精品电影 | 亚洲图色成人| 另类精品久久| av天堂在线播放| 尾随美女入室| 在线看a的网站| 女警被强在线播放| 国产日韩一区二区三区精品不卡| 亚洲图色成人| 婷婷色麻豆天堂久久| 国产精品一区二区免费欧美 | 国产一区有黄有色的免费视频| 电影成人av| 青青草视频在线视频观看| 18禁观看日本| 亚洲国产欧美一区二区综合| 亚洲,一卡二卡三卡| 久久久久久人人人人人| 母亲3免费完整高清在线观看| 中文字幕最新亚洲高清| 乱人伦中国视频| 国产成人精品久久二区二区91| 夫妻午夜视频| 免费在线观看影片大全网站 | 欧美中文综合在线视频| 亚洲国产欧美日韩在线播放| 国产免费福利视频在线观看| 久久久久久久精品精品| 精品一区二区三卡| 久久精品久久久久久噜噜老黄| 男男h啪啪无遮挡| 欧美成人精品欧美一级黄| 韩国精品一区二区三区| 欧美 亚洲 国产 日韩一| 一区二区三区激情视频| 人妻 亚洲 视频| 欧美激情高清一区二区三区| 制服诱惑二区| 久久久久久久久免费视频了| 一级a爱视频在线免费观看| 欧美 日韩 精品 国产| 久久久精品免费免费高清| 美女高潮到喷水免费观看| 国产一区二区三区av在线| 青春草亚洲视频在线观看| 9热在线视频观看99| 青春草视频在线免费观看| 波野结衣二区三区在线| bbb黄色大片| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜爱| 久久久国产欧美日韩av| 久久99精品国语久久久| 欧美国产精品va在线观看不卡| 欧美老熟妇乱子伦牲交| 自拍欧美九色日韩亚洲蝌蚪91| 久久精品久久久久久久性| 国产精品av久久久久免费| svipshipincom国产片| 久久久久久久国产电影| 大香蕉久久成人网| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 一二三四社区在线视频社区8| 成人国产av品久久久| 最新在线观看一区二区三区 | 黑丝袜美女国产一区| 国产人伦9x9x在线观看| 国产免费又黄又爽又色| 国产麻豆69| 777久久人妻少妇嫩草av网站| 黄色一级大片看看| 国产亚洲欧美精品永久| 精品人妻一区二区三区麻豆| 亚洲人成电影免费在线| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 久久中文字幕一级| 午夜福利视频精品| 高清视频免费观看一区二区| 亚洲国产中文字幕在线视频| 成年av动漫网址| xxxhd国产人妻xxx| 久久久精品免费免费高清| 国产麻豆69| 亚洲熟女毛片儿| 香蕉丝袜av| 怎么达到女性高潮| 国产精品,欧美在线| 久久久久久亚洲精品国产蜜桃av| 欧美日韩黄片免| 成人午夜高清在线视频 | 一区福利在线观看| 亚洲精品中文字幕在线视频| 99国产极品粉嫩在线观看| 88av欧美| 在线观看午夜福利视频| 精品第一国产精品| 亚洲中文字幕一区二区三区有码在线看 | 1024视频免费在线观看| 级片在线观看| 看黄色毛片网站| 亚洲欧美日韩无卡精品| 成人午夜高清在线视频 | 国产又色又爽无遮挡免费看| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放 | 久久国产精品人妻蜜桃| 久久久久久人人人人人| or卡值多少钱| 国产97色在线日韩免费| 欧美国产精品va在线观看不卡| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 亚洲一码二码三码区别大吗| 天堂影院成人在线观看| 少妇粗大呻吟视频| 亚洲九九香蕉| 特大巨黑吊av在线直播 | 两性夫妻黄色片| 久久国产亚洲av麻豆专区| av中文乱码字幕在线| 日韩欧美国产一区二区入口| 久久精品影院6| 在线观看免费午夜福利视频| 精品高清国产在线一区| 精品人妻1区二区| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区黑人| 狂野欧美激情性xxxx| 欧美精品啪啪一区二区三区| 免费av毛片视频| 禁无遮挡网站| 精品一区二区三区av网在线观看| 精品电影一区二区在线| 极品教师在线免费播放| 校园春色视频在线观看| 国产一级毛片七仙女欲春2 | 亚洲一码二码三码区别大吗| 国产aⅴ精品一区二区三区波| 少妇 在线观看| 亚洲精品粉嫩美女一区| 久久久国产成人精品二区| 免费在线观看黄色视频的| 亚洲精品av麻豆狂野| 日韩欧美国产一区二区入口| 性色av乱码一区二区三区2| 三级毛片av免费| 国产激情偷乱视频一区二区| 亚洲三区欧美一区| 久久香蕉国产精品| 两人在一起打扑克的视频| 白带黄色成豆腐渣| 亚洲精品国产精品久久久不卡| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类 | 最好的美女福利视频网| 日韩三级视频一区二区三区| 在线看三级毛片| 国产亚洲精品一区二区www| 精品国产一区二区三区四区第35| 国内毛片毛片毛片毛片毛片| 欧美黄色淫秽网站| 一级a爱视频在线免费观看| 国产一卡二卡三卡精品| 最近最新中文字幕大全免费视频| 国产极品粉嫩免费观看在线| 欧美乱色亚洲激情| 国产精品,欧美在线| 亚洲成a人片在线一区二区| 国产精品亚洲一级av第二区| 欧美黑人欧美精品刺激| 免费在线观看亚洲国产| www日本在线高清视频| av中文乱码字幕在线| 欧美性猛交黑人性爽| 女人被狂操c到高潮| 久久国产精品影院| 久久婷婷人人爽人人干人人爱| 一本久久中文字幕| 国产99久久九九免费精品| 日韩精品青青久久久久久| 香蕉丝袜av| 国内精品久久久久久久电影| 757午夜福利合集在线观看| 国产精品久久久久久精品电影 | 一级毛片精品| 国产野战对白在线观看| 亚洲国产精品成人综合色| 夜夜躁狠狠躁天天躁| 久久久久亚洲av毛片大全| 日韩av在线大香蕉| 国内精品久久久久精免费| 国产爱豆传媒在线观看 | www.www免费av| 一区二区三区国产精品乱码| 中文字幕人成人乱码亚洲影| 香蕉国产在线看| 黑人欧美特级aaaaaa片| 韩国精品一区二区三区| 色播亚洲综合网| 黄色a级毛片大全视频| 自线自在国产av| 99久久无色码亚洲精品果冻| 日日夜夜操网爽| 麻豆久久精品国产亚洲av| 香蕉丝袜av| 国产片内射在线| 神马国产精品三级电影在线观看 | 在线观看免费日韩欧美大片| 中文资源天堂在线| 亚洲一区高清亚洲精品| 日韩国内少妇激情av| 亚洲男人天堂网一区| 好男人在线观看高清免费视频 | 黑丝袜美女国产一区| 午夜久久久在线观看| 一二三四在线观看免费中文在| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| 老司机午夜十八禁免费视频| 欧美 亚洲 国产 日韩一| 91麻豆精品激情在线观看国产| 99热6这里只有精品| 在线观看66精品国产| 亚洲成av片中文字幕在线观看| 夜夜夜夜夜久久久久| 成人特级黄色片久久久久久久| videosex国产| 精品久久久久久成人av| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| 久久久久久久精品吃奶| 日日爽夜夜爽网站| 午夜福利欧美成人| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 亚洲午夜精品一区,二区,三区| 午夜免费观看网址| 国内精品久久久久久久电影| 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 亚洲无线在线观看| 夜夜夜夜夜久久久久| 亚洲va日本ⅴa欧美va伊人久久| 国产三级在线视频| 欧美人与性动交α欧美精品济南到| 成人国产综合亚洲| 色综合站精品国产| 人妻久久中文字幕网| 一区二区三区高清视频在线| 日韩av在线大香蕉| 一级毛片高清免费大全| 黄片小视频在线播放| 国产v大片淫在线免费观看| 日韩欧美 国产精品| a级毛片在线看网站| 精品久久久久久久人妻蜜臀av| av在线播放免费不卡| 久99久视频精品免费| 国产熟女xx| 国产不卡一卡二| 欧美日韩福利视频一区二区| 免费无遮挡裸体视频| 亚洲av电影在线进入| 欧美久久黑人一区二区| 日韩 欧美 亚洲 中文字幕| 国产国语露脸激情在线看| 成人特级黄色片久久久久久久| 久久九九热精品免费| 动漫黄色视频在线观看| 女警被强在线播放| 色在线成人网| 国产成人啪精品午夜网站| 国产精品久久久久久精品电影 | 99精品欧美一区二区三区四区| 日韩大尺度精品在线看网址| 免费高清在线观看日韩| 在线十欧美十亚洲十日本专区| 人成视频在线观看免费观看| 亚洲三区欧美一区| 国产精品美女特级片免费视频播放器 | 免费观看精品视频网站| 精品久久久久久久毛片微露脸| 国产亚洲精品一区二区www| 51午夜福利影视在线观看| 国产精品国产高清国产av| 久久精品91无色码中文字幕| 麻豆久久精品国产亚洲av| 黄色片一级片一级黄色片| 男女午夜视频在线观看| 99久久综合精品五月天人人| 久久天堂一区二区三区四区| 看免费av毛片| 久久中文字幕一级| av免费在线观看网站| 又大又爽又粗| 国内少妇人妻偷人精品xxx网站 | 亚洲成人久久爱视频| 亚洲av成人不卡在线观看播放网| 91麻豆av在线| 黄片小视频在线播放| 国产精品电影一区二区三区| 久久人妻福利社区极品人妻图片| 曰老女人黄片| 成人手机av| 老熟妇仑乱视频hdxx| 在线看三级毛片| 久久国产精品男人的天堂亚洲| 丰满人妻熟妇乱又伦精品不卡| 亚洲男人天堂网一区| 免费一级毛片在线播放高清视频| 国产伦人伦偷精品视频| 又紧又爽又黄一区二区| 国产熟女午夜一区二区三区| 村上凉子中文字幕在线| 国产av一区在线观看免费| 国产激情偷乱视频一区二区| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看 | 精品人妻1区二区| 1024手机看黄色片| 免费无遮挡裸体视频| 国产精品自产拍在线观看55亚洲| 日韩欧美在线二视频| 久久久久亚洲av毛片大全| 中亚洲国语对白在线视频| 丁香六月欧美| 中文字幕人成人乱码亚洲影| 欧美日韩一级在线毛片| 国产亚洲精品久久久久久毛片| 日日干狠狠操夜夜爽| 欧美乱色亚洲激情| 嫩草影院精品99| 精品久久久久久久人妻蜜臀av| 亚洲熟妇熟女久久| 国产精品久久电影中文字幕| 国产又色又爽无遮挡免费看| 黄片播放在线免费| 99国产综合亚洲精品| 精品福利观看| 国产真实乱freesex| 欧美日本亚洲视频在线播放| 国产av一区在线观看免费| 变态另类丝袜制服| 欧美国产精品va在线观看不卡| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片 | 制服诱惑二区| 国产精品综合久久久久久久免费| 久久草成人影院| 国产91精品成人一区二区三区| 亚洲av中文字字幕乱码综合 | 亚洲三区欧美一区| 99久久国产精品久久久| 久久狼人影院| 国产成人av激情在线播放| 成在线人永久免费视频| 欧美精品亚洲一区二区| 操出白浆在线播放| 日韩精品免费视频一区二区三区| 亚洲avbb在线观看| 国产不卡一卡二| 午夜福利成人在线免费观看| 成人精品一区二区免费| 12—13女人毛片做爰片一| 日韩大码丰满熟妇| 无限看片的www在线观看| 哪里可以看免费的av片| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看 | 黄色女人牲交| 日本在线视频免费播放| 一区二区日韩欧美中文字幕| 亚洲国产中文字幕在线视频| 久久精品夜夜夜夜夜久久蜜豆 | 欧美一级毛片孕妇| 亚洲第一av免费看| 国产亚洲欧美精品永久| 哪里可以看免费的av片| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 一级作爱视频免费观看| 精品国产一区二区三区四区第35| 看免费av毛片| 欧美zozozo另类| 亚洲精品在线美女| 成人国语在线视频| 岛国视频午夜一区免费看| 国产熟女xx| 国产成人影院久久av| 成人国产一区最新在线观看| 色尼玛亚洲综合影院| 女性生殖器流出的白浆| 亚洲精品一卡2卡三卡4卡5卡| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 在线观看一区二区三区| 亚洲 欧美一区二区三区| av视频在线观看入口| 999久久久精品免费观看国产| 首页视频小说图片口味搜索| 亚洲成av片中文字幕在线观看| 又黄又粗又硬又大视频| 国产av又大| 美女免费视频网站| 看免费av毛片| 免费人成视频x8x8入口观看| 亚洲全国av大片| 久久伊人香网站| a级毛片a级免费在线| 一级黄色大片毛片| 桃色一区二区三区在线观看| 88av欧美| www日本在线高清视频| 麻豆成人午夜福利视频| av在线播放免费不卡| 国产精品野战在线观看| 色精品久久人妻99蜜桃| 午夜福利在线在线| 最近最新中文字幕大全电影3 | 一级毛片精品| 老鸭窝网址在线观看| 韩国精品一区二区三区| 亚洲天堂国产精品一区在线| 久久人妻福利社区极品人妻图片| 999久久久精品免费观看国产| 国产v大片淫在线免费观看| 99在线人妻在线中文字幕| 免费无遮挡裸体视频| 午夜激情福利司机影院| 免费电影在线观看免费观看| 老司机深夜福利视频在线观看| 最近在线观看免费完整版| 男女下面进入的视频免费午夜 | 亚洲久久久国产精品| 757午夜福利合集在线观看| 久99久视频精品免费| 两个人免费观看高清视频| 热re99久久国产66热| 18美女黄网站色大片免费观看| 最好的美女福利视频网| 午夜免费鲁丝| 男女视频在线观看网站免费 | 免费看美女性在线毛片视频| 亚洲一区二区三区色噜噜| 99久久综合精品五月天人人| av有码第一页| 亚洲人成网站在线播放欧美日韩| 少妇的丰满在线观看| 日韩三级视频一区二区三区| 黄色成人免费大全| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类 | 久久99热这里只有精品18| 色综合婷婷激情| 在线观看免费日韩欧美大片| 国产真人三级小视频在线观看| 国产1区2区3区精品| 日韩高清综合在线| 18禁黄网站禁片免费观看直播| 免费一级毛片在线播放高清视频| 久久久久国内视频| 美女大奶头视频| 精品久久久久久久人妻蜜臀av| 十八禁人妻一区二区| 精品久久久久久,| 亚洲精品av麻豆狂野| 一边摸一边抽搐一进一小说| 亚洲欧美日韩无卡精品| 久久 成人 亚洲| 一本久久中文字幕| 日韩 欧美 亚洲 中文字幕| 午夜日韩欧美国产| 欧美成狂野欧美在线观看| 久久亚洲精品不卡| 美国免费a级毛片| 精品熟女少妇八av免费久了| 亚洲激情在线av| www日本在线高清视频| 久久这里只有精品19| 亚洲,欧美精品.| 激情在线观看视频在线高清| 免费在线观看成人毛片| 长腿黑丝高跟| 久久国产精品影院| 亚洲欧美精品综合久久99|