林鎮(zhèn)超 王 燕
(復(fù)旦大學(xué)心理學(xué)系, 上海 200433)
以往的人類個(gè)體發(fā)展學(xué)說(shuō)習(xí)慣性地把身體結(jié)構(gòu)和生理功能的發(fā)展視為一種純粹的生物性過(guò)程,而傳統(tǒng)的心理學(xué)理論只是把上述生物性過(guò)程視為心理發(fā)展的基礎(chǔ), 相對(duì)忽視了心理因素對(duì)生物性發(fā)展的影響。
一方面, 傳統(tǒng)的生物學(xué)理所當(dāng)然地認(rèn)為人類個(gè)體會(huì)按照固定的生命歷程(life course)進(jìn)行生物性上的發(fā)展。例如, 人類嬰兒剛出生時(shí)非常被動(dòng),經(jīng)過(guò)相當(dāng)長(zhǎng)的一段成長(zhǎng)期后青春期才會(huì)到來(lái), 女性相比于男性會(huì)經(jīng)歷較長(zhǎng)的后生殖期(亦即停經(jīng))。另一方面, 盡管傳統(tǒng)的發(fā)展心理學(xué)強(qiáng)調(diào)認(rèn)知能力的發(fā)展以生理功能的發(fā)展為基礎(chǔ), 從而擺脫了身心二元論的嫌疑, 但是其依然遵循單方向作用的原則思考身體發(fā)展對(duì)心理發(fā)展的影響, 而從未系統(tǒng)地考慮心理因素對(duì)于身體發(fā)展的意義。例如,主流的心理學(xué)理論在探討兒童經(jīng)歷對(duì)個(gè)體成長(zhǎng)的影響時(shí)往往只考慮行為層面或心理層面的作用機(jī)制(例如, 母親敏感性對(duì)兒童依戀風(fēng)格的影響)。然而, 健康心理學(xué)關(guān)于心理因素作用于生理因素的研究(Matarazzo, 1980, 1982; Taylor, 1999)卻表明,身體和心理是相互作用的整體。
可見(jiàn), 傳統(tǒng)的從個(gè)體生命歷程角度進(jìn)行的研究割裂了生理與心理的統(tǒng)一, 但是越來(lái)越多的實(shí)證研究支持二者的整體性, 這是現(xiàn)有研究所忽視的最大的認(rèn)識(shí)論上的問(wèn)題。
在具體的研究領(lǐng)域, 童年經(jīng)歷、擇偶、性行為以及父母教養(yǎng)等問(wèn)題的傳統(tǒng)心理學(xué)研究, 特別是在依戀理論框架下的研究早已碩果累累。盡管依戀理論已經(jīng)成為人格發(fā)展研究、情緒發(fā)展研究、親密關(guān)系研究以及其他眾多的心理學(xué)研究的主導(dǎo)性理論框架(Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Cassidy &Shaver, 2008), 但是這些研究都共同存在一些顯而易見(jiàn)的問(wèn)題。第一, 過(guò)分關(guān)注個(gè)體成長(zhǎng)的人際環(huán)境而忽視客觀的物質(zhì)條件(例如, 父親工作的穩(wěn)定性或者家庭居住場(chǎng)所的穩(wěn)定性), 實(shí)證研究早已表明, 后者的個(gè)體差異也非常明顯且對(duì)個(gè)體的發(fā)展同樣具有重要影響(Belsky, Steinberg, & Draper,1991; Chisholm, 1996a, 1999b; Edgarton, 1992; Foley,1992); 第二, 過(guò)分強(qiáng)調(diào)依戀對(duì)于個(gè)體生存(甚至種群的生存)而言的進(jìn)化意義, 而忽視絕大多數(shù)進(jìn)化理論特別注重的繁衍功能(Belsky, 2007; Belsky et al., 1991; Chisholm, 1996, 1999a, 1999b)。
承認(rèn)、贊美達(dá)爾文的自然選擇進(jìn)化論思想(Bowlby, 1991)并將其作為理論基礎(chǔ), 是依戀理論歷久彌新, 牢牢占據(jù)學(xué)術(shù)主流的根本原因。不幸的是, 當(dāng)Bowlby著手研究依戀的時(shí)候, 人們對(duì)進(jìn)化的認(rèn)識(shí)依然只停留在達(dá)爾文時(shí)代的現(xiàn)象學(xué)和解剖學(xué)水平。因而, 依戀理論需要在更高一個(gè)層面來(lái)整合進(jìn)化學(xué)領(lǐng)域的新進(jìn)展并重新審視依戀的進(jìn)化性功能(Simpson & Belsky, 2008)。
針對(duì)上述問(wèn)題, 本文將從進(jìn)化的角度系統(tǒng)地介紹一種可能的、比較綜合的生命發(fā)展理論:生命史理論(life history theory; Belsky, 2007; Belsky,2010; Belsky et al., 1991; see also Charnov, 1993;Daan & Tinbergen, 1997; Roff, 1992, 2002; Stearns,1992)。它既對(duì)傳統(tǒng)的依戀研究進(jìn)行了擴(kuò)展, 又解決了生理與心理統(tǒng)一性的問(wèn)題。更確切地說(shuō), 生命史理論并非是一個(gè)完整的理論, 而是一個(gè)有待充實(shí)的、龐大的理論框架。
如何在關(guān)乎生存和繁衍的各種生命活動(dòng)中有效地分配時(shí)間、資源和能量, 是地球上每一個(gè)有機(jī)體都必須面臨的最基本的挑戰(zhàn)(Charnov, 1993;MacArthur & Wilson, 1967; Roff, 1992, 2002;Schaffer, 1983; Stearns, 1992)。例如, 過(guò)多地參與社會(huì)競(jìng)爭(zhēng)會(huì)影響有機(jī)體的免疫能力, 在教養(yǎng)子女上的付出會(huì)占用個(gè)體尋求新配偶的時(shí)間和資源。對(duì)此, 進(jìn)化學(xué)家和生態(tài)學(xué)家提出了生命史理論來(lái)對(duì)這個(gè)問(wèn)題進(jìn)行“怎么樣(how)”和“為什么(why)”兩個(gè)層面的回答。
在分配時(shí)間、資源和能量的過(guò)程中, 個(gè)體必須在軀體努力(somatic effort)和繁衍努力(reproductive effort)之間做出適應(yīng)于特定環(huán)境的最基本的權(quán)衡(Griskevicius, Delton, Robertson &Tybur, 2011; Griskevicius, Tybur, Delton, & Robertson,2011)。軀體努力是指?jìng)€(gè)體在維持和發(fā)展生理系統(tǒng)和物化資本(embodied capital; 例如, 知識(shí)、技能)上的投入, 可細(xì)分為軀體的維持(bodily maintenance)和成長(zhǎng)(growth)兩個(gè)維度; 繁衍努力是指?jìng)€(gè)體在同性競(jìng)爭(zhēng)、求偶、繁衍以及子女教養(yǎng)上的投入, 可進(jìn)一步分為擇偶(mating)和后代教養(yǎng)(parenting)兩個(gè)方面(Kaplan & Gangestad, 2005; Griskevicius,Delton et al., 2011; Griskevicius, Tybur et al., 2011;Simpson, Griskevicius, Kuo, Sung, & Collins, 2012)。這兩者是既相互關(guān)聯(lián)但又存在資源分配矛盾的不同的生命發(fā)展方向。
為了更形象地描述軀體努力和繁衍努力之間的關(guān)系, Kenrick和Luce (2000)將這個(gè)最基本的生命史權(quán)衡類比于一個(gè)銀行賬戶。軀體努力類似于存款, 而繁衍努力則代表取款?!按婵睢迸c“取款”是相互關(guān)聯(lián)的:一個(gè)更大的存款有利于將來(lái)的取款, 就好比一個(gè)有機(jī)體為了將來(lái)更好地繁衍而不斷提高軀體資本的積累(例如, 保持身體健康、獲取高學(xué)歷、得到體面的工作有利于提高追求高質(zhì)量配偶的成功率并為后代的生養(yǎng)提供堅(jiān)實(shí)的物質(zhì)保障)。然而, “存款”與“取款”之間的銜接又有時(shí)間風(fēng)險(xiǎn):在不穩(wěn)定的環(huán)境中, 戰(zhàn)爭(zhēng)動(dòng)亂、經(jīng)濟(jì)波動(dòng)等都可能導(dǎo)致存款努力所希望達(dá)到的預(yù)期破滅(例如, 繁衍后代之前就死于戰(zhàn)爭(zhēng))。因而, 在特定環(huán)境中, 與其承受巨大的風(fēng)險(xiǎn)繼續(xù)去“存款”, 不如把有限的精力和資源及時(shí)地投入到繁衍任務(wù)中。
權(quán)衡的傾向或者過(guò)程可以被概念化為生命史策略(life history strategy)或繁衍策略(reproductive strategy), 其是一個(gè)以“快策略”和“慢策略”為兩極的連續(xù)譜系。在快策略的一端, 個(gè)體更早地性成熟, 更早地發(fā)生性行為, 有更多的性伴侶, 以及生育更多的子女, 在后代教養(yǎng)上投入更少; 而在慢策略的一端, 個(gè)體更注重身體培養(yǎng)和技能發(fā)展,較遲開(kāi)始性行為, 更注重后代的質(zhì)量而非數(shù)量,更注重后代的教養(yǎng)(Belsky, 2010; Belsky, Houts, &Fearon, 2010; Ellis, 2004; Griskevicius, Delton et al., 2011; Griskevicius, Tybur et al., 2011; Kaplan &Gangestad, 2005; Simpson et al., 2012)。此外, 生命史策略和其他許多個(gè)體變量都存在關(guān)聯(lián)(L. Ellis,1988; Ellis, McFadyen-Ketchum, Dodge, Pettit, &Bates, 1999; Figueredo et al., 2006; Figueredo,Vásquez, Brumbach, & Schneider, 2004, 2007; Kruger& Nesse, 2006; Nettle, 2010; Wilson & Daly, 2004),例如, 人格特質(zhì)(Figueredo et al., 2004)。
自然選擇偏好于那些做出最佳權(quán)衡的個(gè)體(Schaffer, 1983)。在這里, 所謂“最佳”就是指最具有適應(yīng)性(adaptive), 在進(jìn)化視角下的意義并非指?jìng)€(gè)體的心理健康(例如, 幸福感), 而是強(qiáng)調(diào)個(gè)體能否把基因成功地遺傳下去(Belsky, Schlomer, &Ellis, 2012; Charnov, 1993; MacArthur & Wilson,1967; Roff, 1992, 2002; Schaffer, 1983; Stearns,1992)。特定繁衍策略的選擇使得個(gè)體適應(yīng)于不同的生活環(huán)境(Belsky et al., 2012; Ellis, Figueredo,Brumbach, & Schlomer, 2009)。例如, 在貧窮、動(dòng)亂的環(huán)境中更早生育子女有利于提高基因遺傳的概率, 從而避免個(gè)體在繁衍之前就死于惡劣環(huán)境。
生命史策略的選擇既有物種間的差異, 也有種群內(nèi)的個(gè)體差異。一般而言, 大多數(shù)人類個(gè)體都是以一種相當(dāng)慢的生命史策略來(lái)展開(kāi)生命軌跡(Gladden, Sisco, & Figueredo, 2008; Kaplan, Hill,Lancaster, & Hurtado, 2000), 而其他物種, 鼩例如鼱(shrew)則會(huì)采取快策略。無(wú)論是物種間的差異抑或是種群內(nèi)的個(gè)體差異, 特定生命史策略的選擇體現(xiàn)的依舊是生物的適應(yīng)性。
由于自然選擇塑造了環(huán)境敏感性(environmentally sensitive)的生命發(fā)展系統(tǒng), 個(gè)體會(huì)選擇特定的生命史策略從而適應(yīng)于不同的成長(zhǎng)環(huán)境(Belsky et al., 1991; Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Ellis et al., 2009)。值得注意的是, 對(duì)環(huán)境的敏感性和易感性存在個(gè)體差異, 和其他心理行為變量一樣,生命史策略同樣受到遺傳和基因的影響(Figueredo et al., 2004)。生命史權(quán)衡是熔鑄于成長(zhǎng)經(jīng)歷的、經(jīng)由基因和環(huán)境共同調(diào)節(jié)的動(dòng)態(tài)過(guò)程(Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, Mesman, &Juffer, 2008; Belsky & Hartman, 2014; Belsky &Pluess, 2009a, 2009b, 2011, 2013; Ellis, Boyce,Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Van IJzendoorn,2011; Pluess & Belsky, 2010, 2013)。
在微觀層面上, 個(gè)體如何做出特定的生命史策略選擇?抑或說(shuō), 生命史策略是如何形成和發(fā)展的?
早在1982年, 人類學(xué)家Draper和Harpending(1982)就提供了一個(gè)頗具創(chuàng)新性的進(jìn)化學(xué)模型用以解釋父親缺失(father absence)對(duì)女孩成長(zhǎng)的影響。借鑒Trivers (1974)的親代投資理論, Draper和Harpending (1982)認(rèn)為, 作為表征家庭社會(huì)地位的核心人物, 父親缺失意味著個(gè)體無(wú)法接受良好的教養(yǎng)、未來(lái)成長(zhǎng)條件的惡化, 這些預(yù)期促使女孩更早地性成熟, 特別是月經(jīng)初潮的提前到來(lái), 從而有利于其從其他男性身上更早更多地獲取資源。
盡管Draper和Harpending (1982)的理論模型缺乏系統(tǒng)性, 以至于無(wú)法解釋更多現(xiàn)象并做出其他合理預(yù)測(cè), 但是, 一個(gè)非常具有啟發(fā)性的嶄新的問(wèn)題卻進(jìn)入了心理學(xué)研究的視角:早期經(jīng)歷如何影響個(gè)體的繁衍策略?
對(duì)此, Belsky, Steinberg和Draper (1991)在整合依戀理論、社會(huì)化理論、社會(huì)學(xué)習(xí)理論等相關(guān)學(xué)科知識(shí)的基礎(chǔ)上, 借鑒進(jìn)化學(xué)領(lǐng)域的“生命史”概念(see Charnov, 1993; Roff, 1992, 2002; Stearns,1992)提出了一個(gè)全新的生命發(fā)展模型。他們認(rèn)為,兒童將依戀視為感知環(huán)境條件的線索(Belsky,2007, 2010; Belsky & Fearon, 2002, 2008; Belsky et al., 1991; Simpson & Belsky, 2008; Simpson et al.,2012)。如果兒童的照料者有能力投入時(shí)間、精力、資源從而實(shí)現(xiàn)所謂的敏感性、盡責(zé)性, 那么個(gè)體所處的生活環(huán)境就有可能是資源豐富的并且是安全穩(wěn)定的, 個(gè)體也就相應(yīng)地形成安全依戀的內(nèi)部工作模式(internal working model; Bowlby, 1973)。特別地, 嬰兒沒(méi)有能力去評(píng)估它所處的環(huán)境, 但是它能判斷照料者是否滿足它們的生理和情感要求(Del Giudice, 2009a; Simpson & Belsky, 2008)。
Belsky等人(1991, 2007)由此提出了一個(gè)相對(duì)籠統(tǒng)的發(fā)展路徑:(1)早期的家庭背景(family context; 例如, 父母婚姻狀態(tài)、經(jīng)濟(jì)條件、社會(huì)環(huán)境); (2)早期的撫養(yǎng)經(jīng)歷(childrearing experiences;例如, 照料者的敏感水平、盡責(zé)性); (3)心理和行為的發(fā)展(psychological/behavioral development;例如, 依戀模式、內(nèi)部工作模式); (4)軀體的發(fā)展(somatic development; 例如, 何時(shí)性成熟); (5)特定繁衍策略的選擇。
在此基礎(chǔ)上, 兩個(gè)截然不同的、帶有原型(prototype)性質(zhì)的發(fā)展軌跡被用以闡明生命史策略的兩個(gè)典型取向。數(shù)量取向(quantity-oriented)的繁衍策略產(chǎn)生于具有非常多負(fù)性應(yīng)激源的家庭環(huán)境中(例如, 家庭關(guān)系不和、婚姻破裂、經(jīng)濟(jì)拮據(jù)), 相應(yīng)地, 非安全型的依戀模式和機(jī)會(huì)主義的人際關(guān)系取向就會(huì)形成, 從而促成一系列快策略的結(jié)果(例如, 青春期的提前到來(lái), 對(duì)短期配偶關(guān)系的偏好, 養(yǎng)育更多子女但缺乏相應(yīng)的親代投資)。相反, 質(zhì)量取向(quality- oriented)的繁衍策略形成于良好的撫養(yǎng)環(huán)境(例如, 夫妻關(guān)系和諧, 經(jīng)濟(jì)條件殷實(shí)), 敏感的、盡責(zé)的、有積極情感的父母教養(yǎng)行為會(huì)促使個(gè)體形成安全型依戀風(fēng)格、互惠型的人際關(guān)系取向, 青春期和首次性行為的時(shí)間也就推遲了。值得注意的是, 數(shù)量取向和質(zhì)量取向只是繁衍策略連續(xù)譜系上的兩個(gè)極端, 五階段的發(fā)展路徑也只是對(duì)理論所涉及的主要領(lǐng)域的一種抽象概括, 并沒(méi)有限定嚴(yán)格的因果次序(Belsky,2007, 2010; Belsky et al., 1991)。例如, 不良的成長(zhǎng)環(huán)境不一定會(huì)導(dǎo)致非安全型依戀。
可見(jiàn), 相較于以往的發(fā)展心理學(xué)理論, 特別是社會(huì)化理論, 生命史理論有兩個(gè)最為明顯的創(chuàng)新點(diǎn):第一, 摒棄大多數(shù)理論“貼標(biāo)簽”的傳統(tǒng), 不對(duì)依戀類型和人際關(guān)系模式做價(jià)值判斷和倫理取舍, 而是強(qiáng)調(diào)特定的生命史策略對(duì)環(huán)境的適應(yīng)性;第二, 從個(gè)體發(fā)展的角度提出心理影響生理的假設(shè), 具體而言, 就是早期經(jīng)歷對(duì)性成熟時(shí)間的影響(Belsky, 2010; Belsky et al., 2010)。
有趣的是, 早期經(jīng)歷對(duì)性成熟時(shí)間的影響集中體現(xiàn)在女性的身上, 特別是關(guān)乎女性月經(jīng)初潮的時(shí)間(Belsky, 2010; Ellis, 2004; see also Del Giudice, 2011; Draper & Harpending, 1982)。需要說(shuō)明的是, 這并非是由于對(duì)男性性成熟的測(cè)量存在困難而導(dǎo)致的研究數(shù)據(jù)的偏誤(Belsky et al.,2007; Del Giudice, 2009a)。事實(shí)上, Ellis和Essex(2007)已經(jīng)發(fā)現(xiàn), 無(wú)論針對(duì)男性抑或是女性, 都可以把腎上腺功能初現(xiàn)(adrenarche)作為衡量性成熟時(shí)間的指標(biāo)(腎上腺功能初現(xiàn)是青春期的最初階段; Auchus & Rainey, 2004; Herdt & McClintock,2000; Ibanez, Dimartino-Nardi, Potau, & Saenger,2000)。例如, Ellis和Essex (2007)發(fā)現(xiàn), 低質(zhì)量的父母教養(yǎng)和高水平的婚姻沖突會(huì)導(dǎo)致兒童在6至7歲時(shí)就腎上腺功能初現(xiàn)(see also Ellis, 2004)。
不過(guò), 即使是以腎上腺功能初現(xiàn)作為衡量指標(biāo), 早期經(jīng)歷對(duì)性成熟時(shí)間的影響依然主要體現(xiàn)在女性身上。對(duì)此, Del Giudice和Belsky (2010)指出:對(duì)于女性而言, 最重要的生命史權(quán)衡在于軀體努力和繁衍努力之間; 而對(duì)于男性, 擇偶與親代投資之間的權(quán)衡更為重要。因而, 相較于男性的青春期, 女性的青春期更能體現(xiàn)個(gè)體的生命史策略, 更加容易受到早期經(jīng)歷的影響。
這種生命史權(quán)衡的性別差異是通過(guò)成人依戀風(fēng)格的性別差異來(lái)實(shí)現(xiàn)的(Del Giudice, 2008,2009a, 2009b, 2009c, 2011; Del Giudice & Belsky,2010):盡管兒童早期的依戀風(fēng)格沒(méi)有性別差異,但隨著年齡的增長(zhǎng), 男性更多地表現(xiàn)出回避型依戀, 而女性在中等壓力情境下更多地表現(xiàn)為焦慮型依戀, 在高壓力情境下才會(huì)往回避型依戀轉(zhuǎn)變。顯而易見(jiàn), 回避型依戀使得男性表現(xiàn)得更加堅(jiān)強(qiáng)、好斗, 有利于其投身社會(huì)競(jìng)爭(zhēng)從而獲取資源, 焦慮型依戀促使女性更加注重和男性伴侶的聯(lián)結(jié)從而能從伴侶身上獲得更多資源, 生命史權(quán)衡的性別差異具有進(jìn)化意義上的適應(yīng)性(Chisholm,1996, 1999a, 1999b; Davis, Shaver, & Vernon, 2004;Del Giudice, 2011; Del Giudice & Belsky, 2010,2011)。
綜上所述, 性成熟時(shí)間是生命史理論區(qū)別于其他人類發(fā)展理論的核心變量(Belsky, 2010), 它有力地揭示了早期經(jīng)歷影響個(gè)體繁衍策略的基本路徑。值得注意的是, 生命史策略和依戀相互印證:前者偏向于行為學(xué)上的測(cè)量, 而后者偏重于心理學(xué)上的解釋; 前者反映了后者的進(jìn)化學(xué)機(jī)理,而后者為前者建構(gòu)了存在的載體。至此, 生命史理論從宏觀角度的“why”和微觀角度的“how”系統(tǒng)地揭示了人類個(gè)體的生命史現(xiàn)象。
盡管生命史理論仍以依戀作為個(gè)體心理終身發(fā)展的核心, 但它的解釋范疇并不拘囿于與依戀有關(guān)的現(xiàn)象, 甚至不局限于心理現(xiàn)象。部分研究甚至從經(jīng)濟(jì)學(xué)角度、社會(huì)學(xué)角度給予了實(shí)證支持。
在經(jīng)濟(jì)衰退時(shí)期, 人們幾乎在所有消費(fèi)品上的開(kāi)銷都會(huì)有所減少(Bohlen, Carlotti, & Mihas,2010; Katona, 1974), 然而, 女性在美容產(chǎn)品上的開(kāi)支卻會(huì)有所增加(Koehn, 2001; Elliott, 2008)。這種反常的行為經(jīng)濟(jì)學(xué)現(xiàn)象被一些新聞?dòng)浾哒{(diào)侃為“口紅效應(yīng)” (lipstick effect; Koehn, 2001)。借鑒生命史理論的解釋框架, Hill, Rodeheffer, Griskevicius,Durante和White (2012)認(rèn)為, 女性在美容產(chǎn)品上的反常投入是為了提高其在擇偶時(shí)的吸引力, 以期求得資源豐富的配偶。一方面, 在經(jīng)濟(jì)衰退時(shí)期, 高質(zhì)量的男性的數(shù)量會(huì)減少, 加劇了女性間的競(jìng)爭(zhēng); 另一方面, 在經(jīng)濟(jì)衰退時(shí)期, 高質(zhì)量的男性更能保證后代繁衍中的親代投入(Buss, 1994;Clutton-Brock, 2009; Cotton, Small, & Pomiankowski,2006; Jennions & Petrie, 1997; Symons, 1979)。
Griskevicius等人(2011)則發(fā)現(xiàn), 實(shí)驗(yàn)室情境中虛擬的環(huán)境線索同樣能啟動(dòng)個(gè)體做出特定的生命史權(quán)衡:在面臨死亡的威脅和資源匱乏的情境中, 個(gè)體的生命史策略會(huì)發(fā)生轉(zhuǎn)變。特別地, 對(duì)于那些童年時(shí)成長(zhǎng)于貧窮家庭的個(gè)體而言, 死亡威脅相關(guān)的線索會(huì)促使其更加注重當(dāng)下的生活(value the present), 更加追求眼前的利益, 更愿意投身風(fēng)險(xiǎn)行為; 相反, 那些童年時(shí)成長(zhǎng)于富裕家庭的個(gè)體則會(huì)因此更加注重未來(lái)的發(fā)展并避免更多的風(fēng)險(xiǎn), 表現(xiàn)出所謂的“延遲滿足(delayed rewards)”。Chisholm (1999b)認(rèn)為, 死亡相關(guān)的線索會(huì)促使個(gè)體有意識(shí)地或無(wú)意識(shí)地產(chǎn)生與繁衍相關(guān)的預(yù)期(Promislow & Harvey, 1990; Wilson &Daly, 1997), 從而影響生命史權(quán)衡。
近期一項(xiàng)研究還發(fā)現(xiàn)(Dunkel, Mathes, &Beaver, 2013), 壽命預(yù)期會(huì)影響個(gè)體的自我控制(self-control), 進(jìn)而影響其犯罪意向。壽命預(yù)期為5個(gè)月、5年、50年的個(gè)體, 其自我控制的水平依次遞增, 相應(yīng)地, 犯罪意向的分?jǐn)?shù)依次遞減。研究結(jié)果進(jìn)一步表明, 當(dāng)壽命預(yù)期很短的時(shí)候, 生命史策略更能解釋個(gè)體的自我控制和犯罪意向。
可見(jiàn), 盡管繁衍策略主要形成于個(gè)體生命早期的 5至 7年, 但是它依然持續(xù)地受到當(dāng)下經(jīng)歷的鞏固、調(diào)整和重塑(Del Giudice & Belsky, 2011;Simpson et al., 2012), 無(wú)論當(dāng)下環(huán)境信息是真實(shí)的還是實(shí)驗(yàn)室啟動(dòng)的。這種生命史策略的轉(zhuǎn)變體現(xiàn)了個(gè)體的決策動(dòng)機(jī):企圖利用所感知到的環(huán)境信息來(lái)做出最恰當(dāng)?shù)男袨闆Q策從而適應(yīng)于環(huán)境變化, 實(shí)現(xiàn)利益最大化。進(jìn)一步地, 并非所有的環(huán)境線索的效價(jià)都是相同的, 人們對(duì)生存和繁衍更緊密相關(guān)的環(huán)境線索更敏感(李宏利, 陸慧菁, 張雷,2011)。
總之, 以上研究從依戀以外的角度論證了生命史理論的要義:通過(guò)感知和利用環(huán)境線索來(lái)做出最具有適應(yīng)性的權(quán)衡。
未來(lái)的研究需要注意兩個(gè)問(wèn)題:(1)深入挖掘人類社會(huì)規(guī)范在生命史權(quán)衡中的意義; (2)探索實(shí)證研究的新方法, 擴(kuò)展實(shí)證研究的新路徑。
所謂的“適應(yīng)性”本身就是一種動(dòng)態(tài)的建構(gòu),它根植于人與環(huán)境的互動(dòng)模式。從這個(gè)角度講,人類個(gè)體的生命史權(quán)衡要遠(yuǎn)比其他哺乳動(dòng)物復(fù)雜,因?yàn)槿祟惿鐣?huì)并非是單純的物理性生態(tài)系統(tǒng)(Figueredo & Jacobs, 2010)。例如, Sherman,Figueredo和Funder (2013)認(rèn)為, 慢策略更加具有社會(huì)適應(yīng)性, 因?yàn)槿祟惿鐣?huì)的規(guī)范(normativeness)本來(lái)就是主要由慢策略的個(gè)體(或群體)所建立的。這些慢策略的個(gè)體往往出生于優(yōu)越的上層社會(huì),偏好親社會(huì)行為(prosocial behavior), 并且往往是社會(huì)規(guī)則的制定者, 因而, 親社會(huì)行為模式往往是他們所推崇的, 進(jìn)而使得他們能從中獲利。簡(jiǎn)言之, 當(dāng)借鑒生命史理論來(lái)思考個(gè)體的發(fā)展的時(shí)候, 研究者必須清醒地意識(shí)到社會(huì)性動(dòng)物不僅要面對(duì)自然選擇的壓力, 而且其行為還要受限于由其創(chuàng)生的社會(huì)規(guī)范。未來(lái)的研究不應(yīng)當(dāng)只是純粹地把進(jìn)化學(xué)的理論從動(dòng)物界延伸到人類社會(huì), 而需要更多地強(qiáng)調(diào)人類及人類社會(huì)自身的特殊性。
然而, 盡管生命史理論的研究發(fā)現(xiàn)成長(zhǎng)于不良環(huán)境的個(gè)體更容易做出一些不被社會(huì)規(guī)范所接受的行為, 但是我們并非要戴著有色眼鏡去區(qū)別對(duì)待成長(zhǎng)于不同環(huán)境的個(gè)體, 相反, 我們需要借助一些努力來(lái)扭轉(zhuǎn)、規(guī)避這些現(xiàn)象。雖然生命史策略是個(gè)體的無(wú)意識(shí)偏好(Kenrick, Griskevicius,Neuberg, & Schaller, 2010; Simpson, Griskevicius,& Kim, 2011; Simpson et al., 2012), 但是, 我們卻不能否認(rèn)意識(shí)層面的思考和社會(huì)教化的作用(Geronimus, 1996)。生命史理論的研究并沒(méi)有取代人們對(duì)于價(jià)值觀的思考(Belsky et al., 2012), 恰恰相反, 它為這種思考提供了一個(gè)著力點(diǎn):人保留了“人”作為動(dòng)物的基本屬性, 但是人應(yīng)當(dāng)有“人”所特有的思考和追求。
不過(guò), 當(dāng)文化因素被納入研究范疇的時(shí)候,問(wèn)題隨之復(fù)雜:快策略和慢策略如何在社會(huì)規(guī)范體系下被重新定義?例如, 在中國(guó), 計(jì)劃生育政策的實(shí)施會(huì)影響到人們的繁衍策略。
上述問(wèn)題的懸而未決使得相關(guān)的實(shí)證研究具有相當(dāng)大的局限性。量化生命史策略的最大的困難就在于:生命史權(quán)衡在個(gè)體生命史歷程中是動(dòng)態(tài)的、情境性的, 在人類發(fā)展歷程中同樣是動(dòng)態(tài)的、情境性的。例如, 現(xiàn)代化進(jìn)程使得人們初婚的時(shí)間不斷推遲, 東方文明比西方社會(huì)在性態(tài)度上更加保守, 動(dòng)亂貧窮的環(huán)境和穩(wěn)定富足的環(huán)境可以在一個(gè)相對(duì)較小的區(qū)域共存。因而, 對(duì)生命史策略的量化必須強(qiáng)調(diào)大數(shù)據(jù)思維, 建構(gòu)一個(gè)能夠不斷更新的常模。更重要的是, 生命史理論的實(shí)證研究需要探索新方法、新路徑。如何在實(shí)驗(yàn)室情境下設(shè)計(jì)巧妙的實(shí)驗(yàn)區(qū)分不同生命史策略的個(gè)體, 抑或說(shuō)如何更準(zhǔn)確地評(píng)估個(gè)體的生命史權(quán)衡傾向, 是未來(lái)研究亟需解決的問(wèn)題。
李宏利, 陸慧菁, 張雷. (2011). 繁衍線索對(duì)風(fēng)險(xiǎn)信息加工的影響.心理學(xué)報(bào), 43, 1320–1328.
Auchus, R. J., & Rainey, W. E. (2004). Adrenarche–Physiology, biochemistry and human disease.Clinical Endocrinology, 60, 288–296.
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H.,Pijlman, F. T. A., Mesman, J., & Juffer, F. (2008).Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility:Dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR)moderates intervention effects on toddlers’ externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial.Developmental Psychology, 44, 293–300.
Belsky, J. (2007). Childhood experiences and reproductive strategies. In R. Dunbar & L. Barrett (Eds.),The Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology(pp. 237–254).Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Belsky, J. (2010). Childhood experiences and reproductive strategies.Psicothema, 22(1), 28–34.
Belsky, J., & Fearon, R. M. P. (2002). Infant-mother attachment security, contextual risk and early development.Development and Psychopathology, 14, 293–310.
Belsky, J., & Fearon, R. M. P. (2008). Precursors of attachment security. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.),Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications(2nd ed., pp. 295–316). New York: Guilford Press.
Belsky, J., & Hartman, S. (2014). Gene-environment interaction in evolutionary perspective: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences.World Psychiatry,13, 87–89.
Belsky, J., Houts, R. M., & Fearon, R. M. P. (2010). Infant attachment security and the timing of puberty: Testing an evolutionary hypothesis.Psychological Science, 21, 1195–1201.
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009a). Beyond diathesis-stress:Differential susceptibility to environmental influences.Psychological Bulletin, 135, 885–908.
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009b). The nature (and nurture?)of plasticity in early human development.Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 345–351.
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2011). Differential susceptibility to long-term effects of quality of child care on externalizing behavior in adolescence?International Journal of Behavioral Development, 36, 2–10.
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2013). Genetic moderation of early child-care effects on social functioning across childhood:A developmental analysis.Child Development, 84, 1209–1225.
Belsky, J., Schlomer, G. L., & Ellis, B. J. (2012). Beyond cumulative risk: Distinguishing harshness and unpredictability as determinants of parenting and early life history strategy.Developmental Psychology, 48, 662–673.
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization.Child Development, 62, 647–670.
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L. D., Houts, R. M., Friedman, S. L.,DeHart, G., Cauffman, E., … Susman, E. (2007). Family rearing antecedents of pubertal timing.Child Development,78, 1302–1321.
Bohlen, B., Carlotti, S., & Mihas, L. (2010). How the recession has changed US consumer behavior.McKinsey Quarterly, 1, 17–20.
Bowlby, J. (1973).Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation:Anxiety and anger.New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1991).Charles Darwin: A new life. New York:Norton.
Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity.Development and Psychopathology, 17, 271–301.
Buss, D. M. (1994). The strategies of human mating.American Scientist, 82, 238–249.
Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (2008). Preface. In J. Cassidy &P. R. Shaver (Eds.),Handbook of attachment: Theory,research, and clinical applications(2nd ed., pp. xi–xvi).New York: Guilford Press.
Charnov, E. L. (1993).Life history invariants. Oxford,England: Oxford University Press.
Chisholm, J. S. (1996). The evolutionary ecology of attachment organization.Human Nature, 7, 1–37.
Chisholm, J. S. (1999a). Attachment and time preference.Human Nature, 10, 51–83.
Chisholm, J. S. (1999b).Death, hope, and sex. New York:Cambridge University Press.
Clutton-Brock, T. (2009). Sexual selection in females.Animal Behaviour, 77, 3–11.
Cotton, S., Small, J., & Pomiankowski, A. (2006). Sexual selection and condition-dependant mate preferences.Current Biology, 16, 755–765.
Daan, S., & Tinbergen, J. (1997). Adaptation of life histories.In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.),Behavioural ecology:An evolutionary approach(4th ed., pp. 311–333). Oxford,England: Blackwell.
Davis, D., Shaver, P. R., & Vernon, M. L. (2004). Attachment style and subjective motivations for sex.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1076–1090.
Del Giudice, M. (2008). Sex-biased ratio of avoidant ?ambivalent attachment in middle childhood.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 369–379.
Del Giudice, M. (2009a). Sex, attachment, and the development of reproductive strategies.Behavioral and Brain Sciences,32, 1–67.
Del Giudice, M. (2009b). Human reproductive strategies: An emerging synthesis?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32,45–67.
Del Giudice, M. (2009c). On the real magnitude of psychological sex differences.Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 264–279.
Del Giudice, M. (2011). Sex differences in romantic attachment:A meta-analysis.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,37, 193–214.
Del Giudice, M., & Belsky, J. (2010). Sex differences in attachment emerge in middle childhood: An evolutionary hypothesis.Child Development Perspectives, 4, 97–105.
Del Giudice, M., & Belsky, J. (2011). The development of life history strategies: Toward a multi-stage model. In D.M. Buss & P. H. Hawley (Eds.),The evolution of personality and individual differences(pp. 154–176). New York: Oxford University Press.
Draper, P., & Harpending, H. (1982). Father absence and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary perspective.Journal of Anthropological Research, 38, 255–272.
Dunkel, C. S., Mathes, E., & Beaver, K. M. (2013). Life history theory and the general theory of crime: Life expectancy effects on low self-control and criminal intent.Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology,7, 12–23.
Edgarton, R. B. (1992).Sick societies: Challenging the myth of primitive harmony. New York: Free Press.
Elliott, L. (2008). Into the red: “Lipstick effect” reveals the true face of the recession.The Guardian.Retrieved December 22, 2008, from http://www.guardian.co.uk
Ellis, B. J. (2004). Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: An integrated life history approach.Psychological Bulletin,130, 920–958.
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg,M. J., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionaryneurodevelopmental theory.Development and Psychopathology,23, 7–28.
Ellis, B. J., & Essex, M. J. (2007). Family environments,adrenarche and sexual maturation: A longitudinal test of a life history model.Child Development, 78, 1799–1817.
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer,G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk.Human Nature, 20, 204–268.
Ellis, B. J., McFadyen-Ketchum, S., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G.A., & Bates, J. E. (1999). Quality of early family relationships and individual differences in the timing of pubertal maturation in girls: A longitudinal test of an evolutionary model.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 387–401.
Ellis, L. (1988). Criminal behavior and r/K selection: An extension of gene-based evolutionary theory.Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 697–708.
Figueredo, A. J., & Jacobs, W. J. (2010). Aggression,risk-taking, and alternative life history strategies: The behavioral ecology of social deviance. In M. Frias-Amernta & V. Corral-Verdugo (Eds.),Bio-psychosocial perspectives on interpersonal violence(pp. 3–28). Hauppauge,NY: Nova Science.
Figueredo, A. J., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., & Schneider,S. M. R. (2004). The heritability of life history strategy:The K-factor, covitality, and personality.Social Biology,51, 121–143.
Figueredo, A. J., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., Schneider,S., Sefcek, J. A., Tal, I. R., … Jacobs, W. J. (2006).Consilience and life history theory: From genes to brain to reproductive strategy.Developmental Review, 26, 243–275.
Figueredo, A. J., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., & Schneider,S. M.R. (2007). The K-factor, covitality, and personality:A psychometric test of life history theory.Human Nature,18, 47–73.
Foley, R. (1992). Evolutionary ecology and fossil hominids.In E. A. Smith & B. Winterholder (Eds.),Evolutionary ecology and human behavior(pp. 29–64). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Geronimus, A. T. (1996). What teen mothers know.Human Nature, 7, 323–352.
Gladden, P. R., Sisco, M., & Figueredo, A. J. (2008). Sexual coercion and life-history strategy.Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 319–326.
Griskevicius, V., Delton, A. W., Robertson, T. E., & Tybur, J.M. (2011). Environmental contingency in life history strategies: The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on reproductive timing.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 241–291.
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Delton, A. W., & Robertson, T.E. (2011). The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards: A life history theory approach.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,100, 1015–1026.
Herdt, G., & McClintock, M. (2000). The magical age of 10.Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29, 587–606.
Hill, S. E., Rodeheffer, C. D., Griskevicius, V., Durante, K.,& White, A. E. (2012). Boosting beauty in an economic decline: Mating, spending, and the lipstick effect.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,103, 275–291.
Ibanez, L., Dimartino-Nardi, J., Potau, N., & Saenger, P.(2000). Premature adrenarche—Normal variant or forerunner of adult disease.Endocrine Reviews, 21, 671–696.
Jennions, M. D., & Petrie, M. (1997). Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: A review of causes and consequences.Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 72, 283–327.
Kaplan, H. S., & Gangestad, S. W. (2005). Life history theory and evolutionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.),The handbook of evolutionary psychology(pp. 68–95).Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Kaplan, H. S., Hill, K., Lancaster, J. L., & Hurtado, A. M.(2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet,intelligence, and longevity.Evolutionary Anthropology, 9,156–185.
Katona, G. (1974). Psychology and consumer economics.Journal of Consumer Research, 1, 1–8.
Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., Neuberg, S. L., & Schaller,M. (2010). Renovating the pyramid of needs: Contemporary extensions built upon ancient foundations.Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 292–314.
Kenrick, D. T., & Luce, C. L. (2000). An evolutionary life-history model of gender differences and similarities.In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.),The developmental social psychology of gender(pp. 35–63). Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.
Koehn, N. F. (2001). Estee Lauder and the market for prestige cosmetics.Harvard Business School Cases, 801-362,1–44.
Kruger, D. J., & Nesse, R. M. (2006). An evolutionary life-history framework for understanding sex differences in human mortality rates.Human Nature, 17, 74–97.
MacArthur, R. H., & Wilson, E. O. (1967).The theory of island biogeography.Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Matarazzo, J. D. (1980). Behavioral health and behavioral medicine: Frontiers for a new health psychology.American Psychologist, 35, 807–817.
Matarazzo, J. D. (1982). Behavioral health’s challenge to academic, scientific, and professional psychology.American Psychologist, 37, 1–14.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007).Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change.New York:Guilford Press.
Nettle, D. (2010). Dying young and living fast: Variation in life history across English neighborhoods.Behavioral Ecology, 21, 387–395.
Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2010). Differential susceptibility to parenting and quality child care.Developmental Psychology,46, 379–390.
Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2013). Vantage sensitivity: Individual differences in response to positive experiences.Psychological Bulletin, 139, 901–916.
Promislow, D. E. L., & Harvey, P. H. (1990). Living fast and dying young: A comparative analysis of life-history variation among mammals.Journal of Zoology, 220, 417–437.
Roff, D. A. (1992).The evolution of life histories: Theory and analysis. New York: Chapman & Hall.
Roff, D. A. (2002).Life history evolution. Sunderland, MA:Sinauer.
Stearns, S. (1992).The evolution of life histories. New York:Oxford University Press.
Schaffer, W. M. (1983). The application of optimal control theory to the general life history problem.American Naturalist, 121, 418–431.
Sherman, R. A., Figueredo, A. J., & Funder, D. C. (2013).The behavioral correlates of overall and distinctive life history strategy.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 873–888.
Simpson, J. A., & Belsky, J. (2008). Attachment theory within a modern evolutionary framework. In J. Cassidy &P. R. Shaver (Eds.),Handbook of attachment: Theory,research, and clinical applications(2nd ed., pp. 131–157).New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., & Kim, J. S. (2011).Evolution, life history theory, and personality. In L. M.Horowitz & S. Strack (Eds.),Handbook of interpersonal psychology: Theory, research, assessment, and therapeutic interventions(pp. 75–89). New York: Wiley.
Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Kuo, S. I., Sung, S., &Collins, W. A. (2012). Evolution, stress, and sensitive periods: The influence of unpredictability in early versus late childhood on sex and risky behavior.Developmental Psychology, 48, 674–686.
Symons, D. (1979).The evolution of human sexuality.New York: Oxford University Press.
Taylor, S. E. (1999).Health psychology(4th ed.). New York:McGraw-Hill.
Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent-offspring conflict.American Zoologist, 14, 249–264.
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1997). Life expectancy, economic inequality, homicide and reproductive timing in Chicago neighbourhoods.British Medical Journal, 314, 1271–1274.
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (2004). Do pretty women inspire men to discount the future?Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 271(S 4), 177–179.