• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Survival analysis for observational and clustered data: an application for assessing individual and environmental risk factors for suicide

    2013-12-09 03:28:38KerryLouiseKNOXAlinaBAJORSKAChangyongFENGWanTANGPanWUXinMingTU
    上海精神醫(yī)學(xué) 2013年3期

    Kerry Louise KNOX, Alina BAJORSKA, Changyong FENG, Wan TANG, Pan WU, Xin Ming TU,*

    ·Biostatistics in psychiatry (15)·

    Survival analysis for observational and clustered data: an application for assessing individual and environmental risk factors for suicide

    Kerry Louise KNOX1,2, Alina BAJORSKA2, Changyong FENG3, Wan TANG3, Pan WU3, Xin Ming TU1,2,3*

    1. Introduction

    Survival analysis concerns the time from a well-defined origin to some event of interest, such as the time from surgery to death of a cancer patient, the time from wedding to divorce, and the time between the first and second suicide attempts. Although originated in research on the active lifetime of light bulbs and other electric devices, modern applications of survival analysis include many non-survival events. Thus, survival analysis may be more appropriately called the time-to-event analysis.

    Like all other statistical methods, survival analysis deals with random events such as death, divorce and suicide. Thus, the time to the event of interest is a nonnegative random variable, and usually right skewed because most events tend to occur in close proximity to each other after a period of time. In survival analysis we are primarily interested in the distribution of survival time and the influence on such a distribution of some explanatory variables such as age and gender. In some applications, data may also be clustered, or correlated,because of certain common features such as genetic traits or shared environmental factors. Clustered survival time data also arise from analyses involving multiple occurrences of an event from the same individual, such as repeated suicide attempts.

    Unlike their applications in randomized controlled trials, there are more issues to consider when applying survival analysis to observational data. To illustrate the principles involved, this article uses data from a study of the United States Air Force (USAF) active duty personnel during fiscal years 2004-2009, focusing on suicide in this population and on their utilization of medical services.We discuss how to assess the effect of demographic characteristics (e.g., gender), time-dependent factors(e.g., job characteristics), and changes of mental health conditions (e.g., depression and anxiety) on the risk of suicide. We also discuss how to analyze the impact of shared environmental factors such as organizational culture on the risk of suicide by accounting for data clustering due to nested data structures.

    2. Data sources and study population

    2.1 Data sources

    Our research population comprises approximately half a million USAF active duty members who were in service within a 6-year period between October 2003 and September 2009. Information about the population has been extracted from several data sources maintained by military centers. The personnel database, a part of the Military Personnel Data Systems managed by the USAF Personnel Center and updated monthly, was the source of individuals’ socio-demographic and jobrelated information for our analysis. The suicide event database maintained by the Suicide Event Surveillance System was used to identify suicide victims and time of suicide. Information on utilization of medical services was taken from four databases from the Military Health System’s Military Data Repository, which contained two outpatient medical care data sets and two inpatient medical care data sets:

    · Standard Ambulatory Data Record (SADR)for all ambulatory care provided in military facilities;

    · Health Care Service Record, Non-Institutional(HCSR-NI) for ambulatory care provided to USAF personnel in civilian facilities;

    · Standard Inpatient Data Record (SIDR) for inpatient care provided in military facilities;

    · Health Care Service Record, Institutional(HCSR-I) for inpatient care provided to USAF personnel in civilian facilities.

    Table 1. Characteristics of the cohort(309,861 servicemen) as of October 2006

    We extracted service utilization information from the above files for the period October 2000 through September 2009, except for the outpatient HCSR,which was based on the period October 2001 through September 2009. In all data sources, individuals’ real identifiers, such as their social security number, were replaced with artificial pseudo identifiers created by the agency that provided the data which identified each individual’s records within and across the files.This study received approvals from Institutional Review Boards at the USAF, Wilford Hall and the University of Rochester.

    2.2 Characteristics of the study population

    The characteristics of USAF active duty personnel as of October 2006, the midpoint of the study period, are shown in Table 1. Eighty-one percent were men, their mean (sd) age was 30.5 (7.7) and their mean duration of service was 9.5 (7.0) years. Most were married (61%),and a small proportion (6%) were divorced or widowed.Twenty percent were officers. Career groups represented different types of occupations. The personnel were distributed across 84 USAF bases organized into 9 Major Commands (one of the major command groups includes all personnel located outside of USAF bases).

    Average suicide rates for the entire study population and for demographic subgroups of the population are shown in Table 2. The rate was calculated by dividing the number of suicides that occurred during the study period by the total exposure time, and then multiplying the result by 100,000. For example, first dividing 227 suicides by 1,961,247 person-years and then multiplying the result by 100,000, we obtained the average rate of 11.6 suicides per 100 000 person-years for the study population. To compare relative rates between women and men, we obtained a rate ratio of 0.248, unadjusted for other factors, indicating that the rate of suicide for women was about one fourth of that for men. Similarly,we calculated the rate ratios for different age groups and for groups defined by different marital status. For age, we partitioned the study population based on the quartiles of the age distribution and used the first quartile as the referent for comparison.

    Using survival analysis methods (discussed in Section 3), we computed the 95% confidence interval for each rate ratio shown in Table 2. Age appeared significantly protective of suicide, that is, the rate was lower for older people than for younger people, when uncontrolled for other factors. Married individuals had a significantly lower suicide rate compared to their single and divorced or widowed counterparts; the rate in the divorced or widowed group was higher than among those who were single. However, these results may be confounded by age, since single people were generally younger than married ones, so the married status may simply be a correlate of age.

    3. Survival analysis methods for assessing risks of suicide

    3.1 Rate of suicide and proportional hazards model

    The rate of occurrence of a disease in epidemiologic research is often calculated as the ratio of the number of cases to total exposure time, as shown in Table 2. This measure integrates the number of suicide cases with total observation years from all subjects. This is a more sensible index than the percent, or proportion, of people who committed suicide, since it takes into account the length of observation time. However, the use of length of observation time as the denominator makes it more complicated (than in the case where number of individuals is the denominator) to assess the variability of the rate and the standard errors and confidence intervals for the rate. In this situation, the usual statistical methods for proportions cannot be used to provide confidence intervals. Further, the rates shown in Table 2 reflect the average risk of suicide over a period of 6years, rates that could have changed significantly during this period. Even within a subgroup, the likelihood of committing suicide could have varied substantially across the subjects over time as well. To address the statistical issue and to estimate ‘instantaneous’ risk at any point in time, survival analysis methods are needed to model temporal patterns and between-subject variability in suicide risk.

    Table 2. Rate of suicide for the study population and for subgroups in the study population

    To reflect the changing risk of suicide over time, we fix the time origin, such as the time of entry to USAF, and use T to denote the lapse of time from the time origin to suicide. We then compute an average rate of suicide per person-year over a small interval, say (t,t+Δt), around a time point T=t, by dividing the number of suicides within the interval by the sum of observation times across all individuals still under observation by time t+Δt, that is,

    where K(t,t+Δt) is the number of suicides that occur within the interval and N,is the number of people still at risk (alive) at the time t. As the interval shrinks to the point t, we obtain, by using a mathematical argument, an instantaneous rate of suicide per person-year at time t:

    where f(t) is the probability density function, S(t)=1-F(t) is the survivor function and F(t) is the distribution function of T.

    The value h(t) is also known as the hazard function,which within our context describes an individual’s risk of suicide at time t. The hazard function is completely determined by the distribution function F(t)of T and vice versa. In most applications including our context, we model the hazard function, since it is a more meaningful and interpretable quantity.

    The commonly used approach for modeling temporal trend and between-subject variability is the proportional hazards model. If x denotes an explanatory variable such as gender or age at entry to USAF, the hazard function under this popular model is given by:

    or equivalently when expressed on the log scale,

    where h0(t) is the baseline hazard function, that is, the hazard in the absence of x. The hazard ratio exp(?)represents the effect of x on the hazard function per unit increase in x.

    Note that the effect of x on the hazard in this model is multiplicative and constant across time. For example,if x is a binary indicator with x=1 (0) representing female(male), the baseline hazard h0(t) represents the hazard of suicide for males over the duration of service, while the regression part, exp(?), is the hazard ratio (or relative hazard or risk score) for females compared to males.As another example, if x is deviation from average age at entry, the baseline hazard is the risk of suicide for an individual with this mean age, while h(t,x) is the hazard for a person x years older (x>0) or younger (x<0) at entry to USAF.

    The proportional hazards model is easily extended to include multiple explanatory variables. If xkdenotes a kthvariable for an individual (1≤k≤K), the hazard of suicide for the person is given by:

    The baseline hazard h0(t) can be parametrically modeled or left unspecified. An example of the parametric approach is a piecewise constant baseline hazard over time. Under this model, the observation period is divided into segments and within each time interval the hazard is constant. If we partition the study period into M intervals, t0<t1K≤tMand denote by h0mthe constant baseline hazard in the mthinterval, the piecewise constant baseline hazard has the form:

    In many applications, the baseline hazard is left unspecified, giving rise to the semi-parametric Cox proportional hazards model in the sense that h(t,x)has both a non-parametric baseline hazard h0(t) and aparametric component for the effects of explanatory variables. In the Cox regression model,the unspecified baseline hazard h0(t) is estimated empirically. If the interval is very small in the piecewise constant baseline hazard model, there will be little difference between the parametric and semi-parametric Cox proportional hazards models. The baseline hazard function from the latter model is approximated by the piecewise constant function of the former, with improved precision when smaller intervals are used. Thus, it is better to employ the piecewise constant baseline hazard model when significant variability of baseline hazard can be well described through a sparse partition of the study period, such as years or quarters in our context.

    3.2 Inference of model parameters

    In order to estimate effects of explanatory variables xkin the proportional hazards model, we need observed values of the failure time T(x1,K,xk) for all subjects in the sample. For individuals who commit suicide, T(x1,K,xk) is the time when the event occurred. But for most people who do not die of suicide, they are right censored at time c, where c is the last time the subject is observed,either at the end of the study period or at the time of discharge from the USAF. For such censored subjects, we only know that their failure times are greater than c, that is, T(x1,K,xk)>c.

    A standard approach for inference about parameters is the maximum likelihood. The maximum likelihood estimate is the value of the parameters that maximizes the likelihood function, which is the probability of observing the failure times T(x1,K,xk) in a study sample.For those who die by suicide, T(x1,K,xk) is the time of suicide, while for those who are censored, T(x1,K,xk) is set to c. An event indicator d is used to distinguish suicide,d=1, from censored events, d=0.

    For the proportional hazards model, the set of parameters, ?k, is of primary interest. As noted earlier,we may either specify a particular form for the baseline hazard h0(t) such as the piecewise constant model or leave it unspecified as in the Cox proportional hazards model. Regardless of how h0(t) is modeled, we obtain estimates of both ?kand h0(t); the only difference is that if h0(t) is specified, the modeled baseline hazard function is estimated, and otherwise, an empirical version of the baseline hazards is provided.

    3.3 Time-dependent explanatory variables

    When modeling data from studies with long durations,such as in the study of the natural history of a disease or a condition such as suicide, some important characteristics of the study sample may change significantly during the course of the study and it is important to take such changes into consideration. For example, our study was 6 years long during which time an individual might get promoted (from the enlisted to the officer status) or relocated to a different base or Major Command group.To account for such time-varying, or time-dependent,explanatory variables, we modify the proportional hazards model as follows:

    where xk(t) describes each person’s characteristics at time t. For example, if an individual joins the USAF as an enlisted serviceman and three years later gets promoted to officer status, we use a time-varying indicator x(t)to denote the change of status, with x(t)=0 for t≤3 before and x(t)=1 for t>3 after the change. This person’s hazard will change from h0(t) during the first 3 years to h0(t)·exp(?) after the promotion.

    Sometimes, a variable can be either coded as a time-invariant or time-varying variable, depending on the purpose of the analysis. For example, in the officer versus enlisted status case, if we want to know whether the original status at the entry affects a person’s hazard of suicide, the enlisted versus officer status at entry to service will be coded as a time-invariant variable. The effect of this variable indicates differential risks of suicide between enlisted and officer status at entry to service.

    3.4 Choice of time origin and its implications

    In most clinical trial studies, the origin of the failure time T(x1,K,xk) is typically set at the start of the study or at entry to the study if patients enter the study at different times. However, when modeling the natural history of a disease using observational data such as suicide,there are more options for selecting this time origin.Depending on the choice of starting time, the baseline hazard describes changes for different reasons and, as such, must be interpreted accordingly.

    In the context of the study of suicide, when the time origin is entry to USAF, the baseline hazard h0(t)describes how the rate of suicide changes during service.Thus, the regression part of the model cannot include the duration of service variable, but can contain variables to model temporal fluctuations of risk of suicide specific to calendar-times such as seasonality.Alternatively, if the time origin is set at the beginning of the study (October 2003) the baseline hazard reflects temporal fluctuations such as seasonality and thus the regression part can include variables on duration of service, but not on temporal variability during the study period.

    The choice of the time origin also has significant implications for the interpretation of variables in the regression part of the proportional hazards models. For example, if October 2003 is set as the time origin, then an individual’s age at this time origin (z1), is the person’s age at entry to service (x1) plus his or her duration of service at the time of origin (x2); thus, z1=x1+x2. The two age variables, z1and x1, will have implications for model estimates. To illustrate, let us suppose that we model risk of suicide using only age and duration of service as the explanatory variables. Then, the hazard for the age variable z1defined as October 2003 is given by:t,z1,x2)=h0(t)·exp(Y1·z1+Y2·x2)=h0(t)·exp(Y1·x1+[Y1+Y2]·x2),

    while the hazard for the age variable x1defined at entry to service is:

    By comparing the parameters, we see that Y1=?1and Y1+Y2=?2. Thus, although the change of definition does not affect the parameter estimate associated with age, it does alter the interpretation of the variable of duration of service x2. In this particular example, when used with age at entry to service, the hazard ratio (HR) of duration of service exp(?2) is equal to exp(Y1+Y2), the HR of age at October 2003 exp(Y1) times the HR of duration of service exp(Y2).

    3.5 Censoring and competing risks

    In order to make correct inference about a population in the presence of censoring, the uncensored observations must be representative of the whole study population. In other words, the censoring mechanism cannot operate selectively and censor subjects with unusually high (or low) risk for the event of interest. Under independent censoring assumption, censoring is independent of the failure time T; that is, the failure time has the same distribution for the events observed as for the censored observations (had they been observed beyond the censoring time).

    In our suicide study, in addition to censoring due to the limited study period, suicides might also have been censored by competing risks such as discharge from service, change of status from active duty to reserve or guard duty, and death from other causes. Some of these competing risks may cause dependent censoring,in which case the censored cases may have a higher (or lower) risk of the event. For example, discharge from the USAF may be a dependent censoring mechanism if individuals with mental health problems are more likely to be discharged than those without mental health problems. To assess such potential bias, we may perform sensitivity analysis by including suicides that occurred shortly after separation from active duty (e.g., up to 6 months after discharge). To find suicide cases among individuals discharged from the USAF, one may use the US National Death Index, the nationwide central index of death record information.

    Some suicides might also have been misclassified as accidental deaths, since the victim and possibly his or her commanders might be interested in disguising suicide deaths. Therefore additional analyses of accidental or violent deaths may also be useful in assessing potential bias due to dependent censoring. Since suicide is a rare event, the analyses of results may also be sensitive to errors in reporting.

    3.6 Accounting for data clustering

    Like other statistical models, survival analysis also relies on stochastic independence across subjects for valid inference. If observations within some group are correlated because of certain common features such as genetic traits or shared environmental factors, estimates of model parameters obtained from maximum likelihood may still be valid, but estimates of standard errors and associated confidence intervals are generally not. In our study, the data are clustered by the USAF bases as well as the Major Commands, because of shared environmental and social factors such as specific job demands,geographic locations, and the heightened awareness and detection of early signs of suicide by some bases (but not by all bases).

    One way to correct the bias introduced by such clustering is to use sandwich, or empirical, variance estimates. This robust variance estimate addresses bias‘post-hoc’ in the sense that the effect of data clustering is dealt with at the inference stage. An alternative is to model the correlation directly. The shared frailty model follows the latter path by explicitly modeling data clustering through random effects, or latent variables.

    Under the shared frailty model, the correlation within a cluster is modeled by a cluster-specific effect, or shared frailty, αi, in the hazard function:

    where i ranges over the different clusters and j iterates over the subjects within in the ithcluster. Under this approach, the random effects αiare assumed independent, following a gamma distribution with a mean equal to 1. When αi>1 (αi<1), it implies that individuals in the ithcluster have higher (lower) hazard compared to a cluster with the average frailty αi=1. The variance of αimeasures the variability across the clusters,with a larger value indicating higher between-cluster variability (or within-cluster correlation). We might write the same model on the log hazard scale:

    In this form, the shared frailty model resembles a standard random effects regression model with groupspecific random intercept, which is widely used in other contexts of clustered data such as longitudinal data analysis.

    When analyzing clustered data, it is important to have a large number of clusters, especially when the between-cluster variability is high, since accuracy of estimates is largely determined by the number of clusters. Within our context, there are two levels of data clustering, bases and Major Commands, with bases nested within Major Commands. Because there are only 9 Major Commands, but 84 bases, we addressed the base-clustering effect using the above cluster-specific methods (sandwich variance estimate and shared frailty model), while accounting for variability between Major Commands using fixed-effects, that is, by treating Major Commands as 9 subgroups.

    4. Application of survival models to examine risks of suicide

    The data for our study cohort was based on the personnel files extracted from the Military Personnel database. We utilized multiple monthly records per individual in our analysis. The individual’s first monthly record within the study period (October 2003 or entry month if the individual joined the USAF after October 2003) included full study-relevant information about the person. Personnel records for the subsequent months included only new information when changes occurred in the personnel data. The last record for the individual marked the end of the study period (September 2009)or the end of individual’s personnel data if the individual was separated from active duty before the end of study period.

    The data included individuals’ socio-demographic and job-related characteristics such as gender, age,marital status, officer or enlisted status, duration of service in the USAF, career group, location (bases), and Major Command (groups of USAF bases). The time was measured in years elapsed from the time origin selected such as the beginning of the study or entry to USAF. If using entry to USAF (October 2003) as the time origin,the time 0 refers to entry to service (October 2003).

    The analytical file was created in the counting process input style, a common file format for many popular statistical packages such as SAS and STATA. The observation period for each person was divided into a set of periods reflecting the changes of time-varying variables so that all time-varying and time-invariant variables became constant within each period. Thus,each individual had multiple lines of data, with each line representing a different period, beginning with the first and ending with the last period. For example, if a person joined the USAF as enlisted and three years later got promoted to officer status, this person would have at least two lines of data. In the first line, the time-varying status indicator, x(t)=0, indicating the referent’s enlisted status before the change, while in the second line, x(t)=1,denoting the new officer status beginning in year 4. If there are multiple time-varying variables, the number of lines is defined by the different combinations of the variables so that each line represents a period within which all the time-varying variables assume a constant value.

    Table 3. Comparison of hazard ratios of piecewise constant baseline hazard (Model 1) and Cox models (Models 2 and 3) with time origin set at October 2003 (Models 1 and 2) and entry to service (Model 3)

    4.1 Demographic variables

    We first focused on demographic variables. We used two different time origins to illustrate how interpretations of model estimates would change with the two choices of time origin discussed above. We considered three survival models. The first (Model 1) was a piecewise constant baseline hazard model, with the beginning of the study period (October 2003) as the time origin and fiscal years entered as discrete-time indicators to control for temporal changes in the hazard. The other two models were Cox proportional hazards models with two different time origins ? the beginning of the study period(Model 2) and the time of entry to USAF (Model 3).

    Although Models 1 and 2 used the same time origin,the regression part of Model 2 could not contain the indicators of fiscal years, since such temporal changes were accounted for by the (unspecified) baseline hazard.For Model 3, the service duration variable could not be included in the regression component because the(unspecified) baseline hazard of the Cox model accounted for the changes of service duration (from the time of entry into the USAF), but the regression did include the indicators of fiscal years which modeled temporal changes in hazard of suicide during the study period, October 2003-September 2009.

    Model 1 is identical to Model 2, except that Model 1 explicitly models the baseline hazard using a constant value within each 1-year interval, rather than leaving it completely unrestricted, as is done in Model 2. The estimated baseline hazard is shown in Table 4, along with the estimates for the demographic variables. The intercept 17.31 per 100,000 person-years represented the baseline hazard of suicide for the 2004 fiscal year for a single, 26-year old enlisted man at the entry to the service. The hazards for the remaining fiscal years can be calculated by multiplying the 2004 rate by the estimated hazard ratios. Since the hazard ratios were smaller than one, the remaining fiscal years all had lower hazards than the referent fiscal year 2004. However, only the 2005 rate was significantly lower.

    All three models yielded quite similar results.Although the age variable was defined differently in Models 2 and 3, its interpretation remains the same;it represents the effect on the hazard function per unit(year) increase in age.

    4.2 Age and duration of service

    Age was set at the time origin (October 2003) for Models 1 and 2 and at entry to service for Model 3. In Section 3.4,we discussed how different definitions of age would alter interpretations of estimates for other variables. We now illustrate such considerations with the real study data.

    To this end, we ref i t Model 2 above by re-setting age to the age at the time of entry to service. The estimated age effect of this new model (Model 5) is shown in Table 4, compared to the original age effect of Model 2, which is labeled ‘Model 4’ in the table. The age variable in both models is interpreted the same way as the effect of being 1 year older at any time t on the hazard of suicide. The effect and interpretation of service duration, however,is different between the two models. In Model 4, the effect represents an increased risk of suicide (HR=1.038,albeit not significant) for each additional year of service for two people with the same age. In Model 5, such an interpretation does not work, since two people who joined the USAF at the same age also had the same years of service. As discussed in Section 3.4, the HR in Model 5 is the product of the service duration HR and age HR from Model 4. So this “aging while in service” phenomenon is the effect of two polarizing factors, with ‘a(chǎn)ge’ reducing and‘service’ increasing the risk of suicide.

    4.3 Mental disorder diagnoses

    We next examined whether mental disorders were predictive of suicide. We considered three mental disorders: any mood disorder (mainly depression), any anxiety disorder, or any substance use disorder (mainly alcohol abuse or dependency). We used codes from the ninth edition of the International Classification of Diseases(ICD-9) to identify treatments for the three broad mental disorders:

    · Any mood disorder identified by codes for depression or bipolar disorder;

    · Any anxiety disorder identified by codes for anxiety or Post Traumatic Stress Disorder(PTSD);

    · Any substance disorder identified by codes for alcohol or other substance abuse or dependency.

    Table 4. Comparison of estimates of service duration between two definitions of age with one defined as October 2003 (Model 4)and the other at entry to service (Model 5)

    Table 5A. Cox proportional hazard model for suicide with mental disorders diagnoses as risk factors, adjusted for socio-demographic and job related characteristics (Estimated effects of socio-demographic and job related characteristics)

    We refer to the three categories as mood, anxiety and substance disorders. We wanted to know not only if any of these conditions would predict suicide, but also the timing of the effect, that is, if suicide was more likely to occur within the first year of the episode compared to the subsequent period. In identifying episodes of mental disorders, we used all types of medical care utilization:outpatient and inpatient encounters in either military or civilian settings.

    The concept of the episode originated from the idea of an individual’s first treatment encounter that included a specif i ed diagnosis. Due to limitations of the dataset, however, it was not possible to establish the time when the very first encounter occurred. Instead,we determined the first encounter based on a two-year window determined by the availability of the treatment history across the inpatient/outpatient and civilian/military databases. After two years of no encounter with the treatment services, the clock was reset to 0 and a new encounter was established as the beginning of a new episode. Thus, the beginning of a new episode of the disorder was defined as a treatment encounter preceded by at least a two-year period of no treatment for this disorder. Likewise, the end of the spell was defined as two years after the last treatment of the episode.

    The mood and anxiety disorder variables were categorical, each with the value 0 if the individual was not in an ongoing episode, 1 if the individual was in the first year of the episode, and 2 if the individual was in the second or subsequent years of the episode. Due to fewer events, substance disorder was dichotomized with value 0 if not in an ongoing episode and 1 if the in first or subsequent years of the episode. The three mental disorder variables were all time-varying variables toreflect the changing status of the disorders over time.

    Table 5B. Cox proportional hazard model for suicide with mental disorders diagnoses as risk factors, adjusted for socio-demographic and job related characteristics (Estimated effects of mental disorders.)

    Table 5C. Cox proportional hazard model for suicide with mental disorders diagnoses as risk factors, adjusted for socio-demographic and job related characteristics (Comparison of effects of duration of an episode within the same disorder and between the different disorders)

    To evaluate how the hazard of suicide depends on the mental disorders while controlling for sociodemographic and job characteristics, we performed the Cox proportional hazards model with October 2003 as the time origin. We created four age groups based on the quartiles of age distribution at entry to service and used the first quartile as the referent comparison group. The same approach was used for creating the groups of service duration. We also controlled for the effect of Major Commands through fixed effects, while accounting for intra-base correlations through the sandwich variance estimate.

    The estimated hazard ratios for the demographic variables are shown in Table 5A, those for mental disorders are shown in Table 5B, and differential effects of duration of an episode (e.g., first year vs. second year of mood) within the same disorder as well as between the disorders is shown in Table 5C.

    As shown in Table 5A, age, gender, marital status,duration of service, officer status, and major command were all significantly associated with risk of suicide.Women were at lower risk (HR=0.16, p<0.001), and divorced or widowed service members had twice the hazard of suicide compared to single or married individuals. The effect of age was protective; for example, being in the third quartile of age (28-35.5 years) decreased the risk of suicide by approximately 60% (HR=0.417, p=0.002) compared to the youngest(referent) group. When controlling for age and other factors, longer service duration increased the risk of suicide: individuals with more than 14 years of service had about twice the risk of suicide compared to the group with at most 6.8 years of service. Officer status was also protective.

    Table 5B shows that episodes of mood, anxiety or substance disorder increased the risk of suicide. The hazard of suicide was almost 8.7 (p<0.001) times higher for an individual in the first year of a mood episode(without concurrent anxiety) than someone without an ongoing episode of a mood disorder, all other things being equal. The hazard ratio (HR) for anxiety in the first year (without mood) was 4.2 (p<0.001). There are two interaction terms in the bottom three rows of Table 5B. The estimated coefficient for the interaction term of Mood 1styear and Anxiety 1styear is negative(-0.673), implying that the risk of suicide death for a subject with both a 1styear mood disorder and a 1styear anxiety disorder is less than the combined risk of the two disorders (that is, overall risk=2.168+1.446-0.673=2.941;shown in Table 5C); but this overall risk of 2.941 is greater than the risk of someone with either a 1styear mood disorder (risk=2.168) or a 1styear anxiety disorder(risk=1.446), all other things being equal. On the other hand, the interaction term for the Mood 2nd+ year and Anxiety 1styear is positive (0.891), indicating that the concurrent presence of a 2nd+ year mood disorder and a 1styear anxiety disorder magnifies the risk of suicide to a value greater than the combined risk of the two disorders(that is, overall risk=0.583+1.446+0.891=2.920; shown in Table 5C), all other things being equal.

    Table 5C shows that for both mood and anxiety disorders the first year of the episode was associated with a higher risk of suicide than subsequent years: HRs were 4.9 (8.7/1.8) (p<0.001) for mood and 2.6 (4.2/1.7)(p=0.07) for anxiety. The first year of a mood episode was more predictive of suicide than the first year of an anxiety episode; HR=2.1 (p=0.08), but the difference in hazard for suicide between mood episodes and anxiety episodes disappeared after the first year of the episodes. The final two rows of Table 5C provide the statistical significance of two composite null hypotheses. The second-to-last row indicates the probability that individuals with either a 1styear mood disorder or a 1styear anxiety disorder or both type of disorders are at the same risk of suicide (i.e.,both have a coefficient of zero) as all other individuals.The final row indicates the probability that individuals with either a 2nd+ year mood disorder or a 2nd+ year anxiety disorder or both types of disorders are at the same risk of suicide as all other individuals. The p-values for both of these composite null hypotheses are <0.05,which indicates that these hypotheses are rejected; that is, these disorders are significantly associated with the risk of suicide.

    Note that when factors are closely linked and change together (e.g., longer service is associated with older age), they should be included together in the model and interpreted with respect to each other. For example,when age was not included in the model, the effect of longer service became protective of suicide; but when both age and length of service are included in the model,age is a protective factor and length of service is a risk factor for suicide. Thus, when factors are closely linked,exclusion of one of the factors can result in misleading interpretation of the remaining factors.

    4.4 Variation across bases and major commands

    The effect coding was used to create dummy indicators of the different Major Commands in the analysis. Under this coding scheme, the baseline hazard represents the mean, or average, effect over all nine Major Commands,while each dummy indicator of Major Command shows the difference between the corresponding Major Command and the mean effect of all Major commands.

    Figure 1. Hazard ratio of suicide by each Major Commandcompared to mean hazard of all Major Commands

    There was significant variability in the hazard of suicide across the 9 Major Commands (p=0.026), as shown in Table 5A. Figure 1 also depicts the hazard ratios of the 9 Major Commands compared to the baseline hazard, along with a 90% CI of the baseline hazard. We can see that hazards of suicide were significantly higher at Major Commands C and E, but were significantly lower at Major Command G.

    Since the sandwich variance estimate used in the above analysis addresses data clustering at the base level ‘implicitly’ through estimation, we could not obtain estimates of variability across the bases. To get some ideas about the latter variability, we ref i t the data with the shared frailty model. We obtained quite similar estimates for all the variables reported in Tables 5A,5B and 5C. However, the shared frailty model yielded additional information about the variability of suicide at the base level. The estimated variance of the Gamma random effect αiwas 0.023, which was significantly different from 0 (p<0.001). Thus, there was significant variability in suicide across the 84 USAF bases.

    5. Comparison of methods and software packages

    We discussed a range of models for survival data and their applications to modeling suicide using observational data. With piecewise constant baseline hazard and Cox models, we can model effects of multiple risk or protective variables on suicide and even account for correlated outcomes using robust variance estimates or shared frailty. One advantage of the piecewise constant baseline hazard model over the Cox model is shorter computing times. Another is a direct control of the baseline hazard, providing the ability to model temporal trends specific to a study such as fiscal years in our application. However, as it requires dividing the study period into smaller time intervals, the piecewise constant baseline hazard model demands additional programming efforts which, depending on how refined the time intervals are, may result in very large files,potentially exceeding limited memory capacity.

    We used SAS and STATA packages to prepare data and perform model fitting for all the examples. The two packages are similar in many respects, but there are also some differences. In our application, we found that when fitting the Cox model, it took longer for SAS (version 9.2)than STATA (version 8) to obtain model estimates. In addition, STATA has convenient built-in commands for splitting records and performing other manipulations necessary to create appropriate file structures for fitting survival models. A shared frailty option is also available in Stata when fitting the Cox as well as fully parametric models such as the piecewise constant baseline hazard model. The sandwich variance estimate is available in either software package. One advantage of SAS is that the results are presented in nice-looking tables.

    6. Discussion

    We have discussed how to assess effects of individual and environmental factors on the rate of a disease using survival analysis methods. We started with simple calculations of rates, but finished the discussion with quite complex survival analysis models. By using data from a real observational study on risk of suicide, we have introduced not only the basic elements of the modeling theory, but also important considerations such as selection of time of origin and data clustering when modeling the natural history of a disease such as suicide.With respect to the USAF study population in our analysis, we found that mental disorders such as mood disorders, anxiety disorders and substance use disorders were significantly associated with the risk of suicide.We also found significant variation in rates of suicide across the 84 bases and across the 9 Major Commands,after adjusting for individual differences. High variability may indicate higher prevalence of such mental health problems specific to an organization due to shared environmental and social factors such as specific job demands and geographic locations. However, it may also indicate higher levels of awareness and detection of the problems. Observational studies in general cannot tease apart such confounding effects. Nonetheless, results from such study data are still informative, since they generate research questions and hypotheses for further investigation.

    Conflict of interest

    The authors report no conflict of interest related to this manuscript.

    Acknowledgements

    The research reported in this manuscript was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health R01 MH07501 7-01A1, and by the National Institute of Mental Health R34 MH096854-02, both to Dr. Kerry L. Knox.

    10.3969/j.issn.1002-0829.2013.03.10

    1Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, USA

    2Veterans Integrated Service Network 2 Center of Excellence for Suicide Prevention, Canandaigua VA Medical Center, Canandaigua, NY, USA

    3Department of Biostatistics, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, USA

    *correspondence: xin_tu@urmc.rochester.edu

    Dr. Kerry L. Knox's research focuses on the epidemiology and investigations of the effectiveness of prevention programs for reducing suicide in military and Veteran populations. Her research on suicide began with studies of the Air Force Suicide Prevention Program (AFSPP) in 2000,funded by the National Institutes of Mental with Dr. Knox as PI since 2002. She has a broad background, and experience, beginning in cancer epidemiology conducting randomized control trials, then transitioned to graduate work in biological anthropology, with an emphasis on investigating the relationship between social stress and reproductive suppression. Her postdoctoral work in neurobiology and physiology continued this thematic interest, through basic scientific research on the relationship between stress and poor outcomes. Since 2000 Dr.Knox's disciplinary home has been solidly based in psychiatric epidemiology, with an overall abiding interest in the relationship between stress (biological, social, psychological, psychiatric or deployment related) and poor outcomes, especially suicide.

    亚洲欧美日韩卡通动漫| 久久久国产成人免费| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| 国产私拍福利视频在线观看| 女同久久另类99精品国产91| 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 中文字幕高清在线视频| 91麻豆av在线| 国产精华一区二区三区| 久久精品久久久久久噜噜老黄 | 国内精品久久久久精免费| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看 | 此物有八面人人有两片| 成人美女网站在线观看视频| 一区福利在线观看| 99久久99久久久精品蜜桃| 简卡轻食公司| 久久久成人免费电影| 国内精品一区二区在线观看| 一个人免费在线观看的高清视频| 中文字幕高清在线视频| 性色avwww在线观看| 一进一出抽搐gif免费好疼| 男人的好看免费观看在线视频| 少妇人妻一区二区三区视频| 99热这里只有精品一区| 精品午夜福利在线看| 男女那种视频在线观看| or卡值多少钱| 色综合亚洲欧美另类图片| 美女大奶头视频| 国产日本99.免费观看| 国产不卡一卡二| 高潮久久久久久久久久久不卡| 噜噜噜噜噜久久久久久91| 伦理电影大哥的女人| 精品久久久久久成人av| 草草在线视频免费看| 99热精品在线国产| 免费人成视频x8x8入口观看| 国产精品一区二区三区四区免费观看 | 精品一区二区三区人妻视频| 国产精华一区二区三区| 欧美在线黄色| 脱女人内裤的视频| 嫩草影院精品99| 老熟妇乱子伦视频在线观看| 亚洲人成伊人成综合网2020| 美女免费视频网站| 男女那种视频在线观看| 床上黄色一级片| 成人国产综合亚洲| 亚洲精品久久国产高清桃花| 精品国产亚洲在线| 亚洲人成网站在线播| 国产国拍精品亚洲av在线观看| 国产探花在线观看一区二区| 成人无遮挡网站| 亚洲精品一卡2卡三卡4卡5卡| 免费观看的影片在线观看| 神马国产精品三级电影在线观看| 最新在线观看一区二区三区| 国产一区二区激情短视频| 99久久99久久久精品蜜桃| 国产麻豆成人av免费视频| а√天堂www在线а√下载| 九九久久精品国产亚洲av麻豆| 午夜福利在线在线| 亚洲国产高清在线一区二区三| 国产亚洲精品综合一区在线观看| 色综合欧美亚洲国产小说| 成人av在线播放网站| 啪啪无遮挡十八禁网站| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 日韩欧美精品v在线| 身体一侧抽搐| 国产激情偷乱视频一区二区| 中出人妻视频一区二区| 最近最新免费中文字幕在线| 国产一区二区在线av高清观看| 亚洲激情在线av| 亚洲av成人不卡在线观看播放网| 在线a可以看的网站| 亚洲成av人片在线播放无| 日本一本二区三区精品| 精品午夜福利视频在线观看一区| 中国美女看黄片| 女生性感内裤真人,穿戴方法视频| 久久精品久久久久久噜噜老黄 | 亚洲精品影视一区二区三区av| 免费高清视频大片| 久久亚洲精品不卡| ponron亚洲| 欧美最新免费一区二区三区 | 欧美+亚洲+日韩+国产| 欧美一区二区精品小视频在线| 亚洲 欧美 日韩 在线 免费| 三级男女做爰猛烈吃奶摸视频| 三级男女做爰猛烈吃奶摸视频| 成年女人看的毛片在线观看| 亚洲av五月六月丁香网| 两性午夜刺激爽爽歪歪视频在线观看| 99热精品在线国产| 国产亚洲精品综合一区在线观看| 脱女人内裤的视频| 午夜久久久久精精品| 精品午夜福利视频在线观看一区| 99久久精品国产亚洲精品| 欧美xxxx性猛交bbbb| 国产人妻一区二区三区在| 国产淫片久久久久久久久 | 亚洲av免费高清在线观看| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 99国产精品一区二区蜜桃av| a级毛片免费高清观看在线播放| 哪里可以看免费的av片| 草草在线视频免费看| 3wmmmm亚洲av在线观看| 午夜福利欧美成人| av天堂在线播放| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 波多野结衣高清无吗| 国产高潮美女av| 亚洲 国产 在线| 日本黄大片高清| 久久精品91蜜桃| 婷婷亚洲欧美| 中文字幕高清在线视频| 一本精品99久久精品77| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看| 一级作爱视频免费观看| 97热精品久久久久久| 18美女黄网站色大片免费观看| 岛国在线免费视频观看| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 偷拍熟女少妇极品色| 99国产综合亚洲精品| 日本 欧美在线| 毛片一级片免费看久久久久 | 国产精品综合久久久久久久免费| 免费在线观看影片大全网站| 不卡一级毛片| 国产精品一区二区性色av| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 此物有八面人人有两片| 午夜福利在线在线| 又粗又爽又猛毛片免费看| 久久精品夜夜夜夜夜久久蜜豆| 精品一区二区免费观看| 国产精品免费一区二区三区在线| 男女做爰动态图高潮gif福利片| 搡女人真爽免费视频火全软件 | 国内毛片毛片毛片毛片毛片| 90打野战视频偷拍视频| 床上黄色一级片| 搡老妇女老女人老熟妇| 欧美成人一区二区免费高清观看| 欧美成人一区二区免费高清观看| 亚洲欧美日韩卡通动漫| 国模一区二区三区四区视频| 久久久久国内视频| 一边摸一边抽搐一进一小说| 日韩高清综合在线| 欧美乱妇无乱码| 久99久视频精品免费| 亚洲成a人片在线一区二区| 久久精品国产亚洲av天美| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 亚洲三级黄色毛片| 可以在线观看毛片的网站| 亚洲无线观看免费| 波野结衣二区三区在线| 在线观看免费视频日本深夜| 国产午夜精品久久久久久一区二区三区 | 国产欧美日韩精品亚洲av| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 可以在线观看的亚洲视频| 久久草成人影院| 老师上课跳d突然被开到最大视频 久久午夜综合久久蜜桃 | 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 日韩 亚洲 欧美在线| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 亚洲成av人片免费观看| 成人av在线播放网站| 麻豆av噜噜一区二区三区| 桃色一区二区三区在线观看| 别揉我奶头~嗯~啊~动态视频| 成人av在线播放网站| 欧美日韩亚洲国产一区二区在线观看| 精品午夜福利视频在线观看一区| 亚洲久久久久久中文字幕| 精品日产1卡2卡| 99久久精品一区二区三区| 中文亚洲av片在线观看爽| 在线观看av片永久免费下载| 日本黄色视频三级网站网址| 人妻夜夜爽99麻豆av| 精品人妻视频免费看| 最新中文字幕久久久久| 亚洲av成人av| 一区二区三区四区激情视频 | 91字幕亚洲| 热99在线观看视频| 国产精品av视频在线免费观看| www.色视频.com| 久久6这里有精品| 亚洲成人久久爱视频| 亚洲久久久久久中文字幕| 免费av观看视频| 一区二区三区激情视频| 99国产极品粉嫩在线观看| 99热只有精品国产| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 色5月婷婷丁香| av天堂在线播放| or卡值多少钱| 成人性生交大片免费视频hd| 级片在线观看| 欧美日韩综合久久久久久 | 日本熟妇午夜| 国产男靠女视频免费网站| or卡值多少钱| 久久99热6这里只有精品| 9191精品国产免费久久| 日韩人妻高清精品专区| 麻豆国产97在线/欧美| 精品无人区乱码1区二区| 欧美激情国产日韩精品一区| 亚洲片人在线观看| 久久6这里有精品| 午夜久久久久精精品| 午夜影院日韩av| 在现免费观看毛片| 欧美区成人在线视频| 俄罗斯特黄特色一大片| 真人做人爱边吃奶动态| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 尤物成人国产欧美一区二区三区| 中文字幕人成人乱码亚洲影| 久9热在线精品视频| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 国产一区二区三区在线臀色熟女| 男女那种视频在线观看| 免费av毛片视频| 日本撒尿小便嘘嘘汇集6| 国产视频一区二区在线看| 国内毛片毛片毛片毛片毛片| 国产精品电影一区二区三区| 欧美午夜高清在线| 一进一出抽搐动态| 欧美激情国产日韩精品一区| 琪琪午夜伦伦电影理论片6080| 日本撒尿小便嘘嘘汇集6| 搡老熟女国产l中国老女人| 欧美黑人欧美精品刺激| 亚洲天堂国产精品一区在线| 久久婷婷人人爽人人干人人爱| 久久99热6这里只有精品| 亚洲一区二区三区色噜噜| 69av精品久久久久久| 国产av麻豆久久久久久久| 亚洲激情在线av| 久久久国产成人精品二区| 一进一出抽搐gif免费好疼| 欧美xxxx黑人xx丫x性爽| 亚洲国产日韩欧美精品在线观看| 亚洲av成人av| 亚洲在线观看片| 男人舔女人下体高潮全视频| 如何舔出高潮| 午夜影院日韩av| 成人性生交大片免费视频hd| 亚洲一区二区三区色噜噜| 亚洲五月天丁香| 中文资源天堂在线| 变态另类丝袜制服| 高清在线国产一区| 黄色女人牲交| 亚洲国产精品久久男人天堂| 少妇丰满av| 97超级碰碰碰精品色视频在线观看| 精品一区二区三区人妻视频| 成年人黄色毛片网站| 亚洲av二区三区四区| 国产伦人伦偷精品视频| 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 国产成人福利小说| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| 亚洲av成人精品一区久久| 国产高清激情床上av| 成年免费大片在线观看| 最好的美女福利视频网| xxxwww97欧美| 婷婷精品国产亚洲av在线| 好男人在线观看高清免费视频| 变态另类成人亚洲欧美熟女| 久久午夜福利片| 久久久久国产精品人妻aⅴ院| 国产精品爽爽va在线观看网站| 十八禁国产超污无遮挡网站| 日韩欧美三级三区| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 久久这里只有精品中国| 色综合欧美亚洲国产小说| 2021天堂中文幕一二区在线观| 亚洲人成电影免费在线| 999久久久精品免费观看国产| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 又黄又爽又免费观看的视频| 狠狠狠狠99中文字幕| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区 | 99国产精品一区二区三区| 嫩草影院精品99| 99精品在免费线老司机午夜| 有码 亚洲区| 亚洲在线观看片| 国产精品一及| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 尤物成人国产欧美一区二区三区| 亚洲激情在线av| 国产久久久一区二区三区| 中文字幕人成人乱码亚洲影| 欧美另类亚洲清纯唯美| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 亚洲精品456在线播放app | 在线观看美女被高潮喷水网站 | 久久精品影院6| 观看免费一级毛片| 欧美黑人欧美精品刺激| 国内精品久久久久精免费| 久久久久久久久久黄片| 久久久精品大字幕| 亚洲18禁久久av| 99热这里只有精品一区| 九九久久精品国产亚洲av麻豆| 色综合欧美亚洲国产小说| 成人三级黄色视频| 99久国产av精品| 美女大奶头视频| 国产单亲对白刺激| 精品久久久久久久久亚洲 | 国产探花在线观看一区二区| 美女高潮喷水抽搐中文字幕| 怎么达到女性高潮| 国产三级黄色录像| 精品久久久久久久人妻蜜臀av| 国产成人福利小说| 18+在线观看网站| 亚洲国产欧美人成| 国产精品不卡视频一区二区 | 亚洲av二区三区四区| 啪啪无遮挡十八禁网站| 小说图片视频综合网站| 毛片女人毛片| 国产精品综合久久久久久久免费| 亚洲欧美日韩高清专用| 成人av一区二区三区在线看| 午夜激情欧美在线| 亚洲乱码一区二区免费版| 桃色一区二区三区在线观看| 91av网一区二区| 国产v大片淫在线免费观看| 久久国产乱子伦精品免费另类| 亚洲成a人片在线一区二区| 国产男靠女视频免费网站| 国产亚洲欧美98| 国产午夜精品久久久久久一区二区三区 | 一级a爱片免费观看的视频| 国产精品伦人一区二区| 国内揄拍国产精品人妻在线| 日韩欧美国产在线观看| 波多野结衣高清作品| 国内精品久久久久精免费| 亚洲精品亚洲一区二区| 欧美成人性av电影在线观看| 国产黄片美女视频| 男插女下体视频免费在线播放| 女同久久另类99精品国产91| 久久九九热精品免费| 国产精品一区二区性色av| 午夜精品久久久久久毛片777| 成人毛片a级毛片在线播放| 午夜精品久久久久久毛片777| 91久久精品电影网| 免费在线观看亚洲国产| 91av网一区二区| 色综合欧美亚洲国产小说| 国产不卡一卡二| 国产成人啪精品午夜网站| 久久99热这里只有精品18| 精品久久久久久久久久久久久| 免费电影在线观看免费观看| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 嫩草影院精品99| 特级一级黄色大片| 老鸭窝网址在线观看| 欧美区成人在线视频| 嫁个100分男人电影在线观看| 亚洲成人免费电影在线观看| 别揉我奶头~嗯~啊~动态视频| 91字幕亚洲| xxxwww97欧美| 男人舔女人下体高潮全视频| 老女人水多毛片| 十八禁网站免费在线| 99热精品在线国产| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 老司机深夜福利视频在线观看| av视频在线观看入口| 伦理电影大哥的女人| 一边摸一边抽搐一进一小说| 每晚都被弄得嗷嗷叫到高潮| 欧美色欧美亚洲另类二区| 国产欧美日韩一区二区精品| .国产精品久久| 最后的刺客免费高清国语| 999久久久精品免费观看国产| 成人av在线播放网站| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 中出人妻视频一区二区| 国产精品av视频在线免费观看| 国产综合懂色| 亚洲精品粉嫩美女一区| 国产伦一二天堂av在线观看| 性插视频无遮挡在线免费观看| 久久国产精品影院| 午夜精品久久久久久毛片777| 久久欧美精品欧美久久欧美| 色综合欧美亚洲国产小说| 精品一区二区免费观看| 欧美最黄视频在线播放免费| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| .国产精品久久| 亚洲av免费高清在线观看| 91字幕亚洲| 国产单亲对白刺激| 亚洲综合色惰| 99精品在免费线老司机午夜| 我要搜黄色片| 一个人看的www免费观看视频| a在线观看视频网站| av在线蜜桃| 狂野欧美白嫩少妇大欣赏| 免费看a级黄色片| 中文字幕免费在线视频6| 他把我摸到了高潮在线观看| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添小说| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图 | 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看 | 男女做爰动态图高潮gif福利片| 久久人妻av系列| 精品久久国产蜜桃| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 亚洲欧美清纯卡通| 又粗又爽又猛毛片免费看| 男人狂女人下面高潮的视频| 亚洲综合色惰| 赤兔流量卡办理| 一夜夜www| 小说图片视频综合网站| 一进一出抽搐动态| 欧美bdsm另类| 日韩大尺度精品在线看网址| 我要搜黄色片| 亚洲成人免费电影在线观看| 三级毛片av免费| 国产一区二区在线观看日韩| 成人美女网站在线观看视频| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图 | bbb黄色大片| av国产免费在线观看| 国产又黄又爽又无遮挡在线| 国产一区二区三区在线臀色熟女| 国语自产精品视频在线第100页| 国产精华一区二区三区| av在线观看视频网站免费| 国产成人啪精品午夜网站| 欧美精品国产亚洲| 一个人看视频在线观看www免费| 中国美女看黄片| 日韩免费av在线播放| 蜜桃亚洲精品一区二区三区| 国产私拍福利视频在线观看| 免费av观看视频| 欧美黑人巨大hd| 一区二区三区高清视频在线| 久久精品国产99精品国产亚洲性色| 久久久成人免费电影| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜添小说| а√天堂www在线а√下载| 成人特级黄色片久久久久久久| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产 | 亚洲五月天丁香| 国产精品综合久久久久久久免费| 色综合欧美亚洲国产小说| 欧美黑人欧美精品刺激| 国产成+人综合+亚洲专区| 日韩高清综合在线| 亚洲精品在线观看二区| 精品久久久久久成人av| 长腿黑丝高跟| 亚洲最大成人中文| 99在线视频只有这里精品首页| 亚洲国产精品999在线| 级片在线观看| 听说在线观看完整版免费高清| av福利片在线观看| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产 | 欧美xxxx黑人xx丫x性爽| 大型黄色视频在线免费观看| 校园春色视频在线观看| 国产一区二区激情短视频| 激情在线观看视频在线高清| 欧美日韩福利视频一区二区| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 看免费av毛片| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 免费在线观看影片大全网站| 亚洲第一欧美日韩一区二区三区| www.999成人在线观看| 两性午夜刺激爽爽歪歪视频在线观看| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 日韩欧美精品v在线| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 免费大片18禁| 一区二区三区激情视频| 欧美中文日本在线观看视频| 波多野结衣高清作品| 免费看光身美女| 欧美色视频一区免费| 精品久久久久久久久亚洲 | 日韩人妻高清精品专区| 亚洲男人的天堂狠狠| 国产三级黄色录像| 国产精品1区2区在线观看.| 亚洲成人久久爱视频| 搡女人真爽免费视频火全软件 | 欧美色欧美亚洲另类二区| 久久精品国产清高在天天线| 一本综合久久免费| 精品欧美国产一区二区三| 精品人妻视频免费看| 国内精品久久久久精免费| 日日干狠狠操夜夜爽| 两个人的视频大全免费| 亚洲男人的天堂狠狠| 欧美性猛交黑人性爽| 日本成人三级电影网站| 美女高潮的动态| 国产高清视频在线观看网站| 国产毛片a区久久久久| 国产美女午夜福利| 日韩欧美一区二区三区在线观看| 91九色精品人成在线观看| 午夜福利视频1000在线观看| 美女免费视频网站| 免费一级毛片在线播放高清视频| 国产精品久久视频播放| 国产野战对白在线观看| 真实男女啪啪啪动态图| 精品免费久久久久久久清纯| 成人毛片a级毛片在线播放| 国产久久久一区二区三区| 最近视频中文字幕2019在线8| 成人特级黄色片久久久久久久| 成年女人毛片免费观看观看9| 国产精品一区二区三区四区久久| 国产精品一区二区三区四区免费观看 | 亚洲熟妇熟女久久| 婷婷亚洲欧美| 免费在线观看亚洲国产| 亚洲av免费在线观看| 亚洲国产精品合色在线| 热99re8久久精品国产| a级毛片a级免费在线| 变态另类成人亚洲欧美熟女| 美女高潮的动态| 天堂√8在线中文| 国产精品98久久久久久宅男小说| 亚洲成a人片在线一区二区| 国产高清视频在线观看网站| 成年版毛片免费区| 国产日本99.免费观看| 精品午夜福利在线看| 伦理电影大哥的女人| aaaaa片日本免费| 97超视频在线观看视频| 国产毛片a区久久久久| x7x7x7水蜜桃| 国产成年人精品一区二区| 欧美日韩瑟瑟在线播放| 99热6这里只有精品| 亚洲自偷自拍三级| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 亚洲成a人片在线一区二区| АⅤ资源中文在线天堂| 伊人久久精品亚洲午夜| 给我免费播放毛片高清在线观看| 神马国产精品三级电影在线观看| 人妻久久中文字幕网| 日韩欧美三级三区| 色尼玛亚洲综合影院| 少妇的逼好多水| 欧美精品啪啪一区二区三区| 美女 人体艺术 gogo| 男女床上黄色一级片免费看| 网址你懂的国产日韩在线| 成人鲁丝片一二三区免费| 国产精品三级大全|